Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nmvwc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-15T06:45:29.622Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineers and industrial designers while designing and problem-solving

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2020

Sonia Vieira*
Affiliation:
Politecnico di Milano, Milano, Italy
John S. Gero
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Charlotte, Charlotte, NC, USA
Jessica Delmoral
Affiliation:
INEGI-FEUP, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
Valentin Gattol
Affiliation:
Austrian Institute of Technology, Seibersdorf, Austria
Carlos Fernandes
Affiliation:
Saint John Hospital, Porto, Portugal
Marco Parente
Affiliation:
Faculty of Engineering, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
António A. Fernandes
Affiliation:
Faculty of Engineering, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
*
Corresponding author Sonia Vieira sonia.dasilvavieira@polimi.it
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This paper presents results from an experiment using electroencephalography to measure neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineers and industrial designers when designing and problem-solving. In this study, we adopted and then extended the tasks described in a previous functional magnetic resonance imaging study reported in the literature. The block experiment consists of a sequence of three tasks: problem-solving, basic design and open design using a physical interface. The block is preceded by a familiarizing pre-task and then extended to a fourth open design task using free-hand sketching. This paper presents the neurophysiological results from 36 experimental sessions of mechanical engineers and industrial designers. Results indicate significant differences in activations between the problem-solving and the open design tasks. The paper focuses on the two prototypical tasks of problem-solving layout and open design sketching and presents results for both aggregate and temporal activations across participants within each domain and across domains.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Designing is an activity that is carried out over time. Design research has focussed on design cognition as a multimodal activity with a temporal aspect, different from problem-solving (Dorst Reference Dorst2011; Gero Reference Gero1990). The emerging field of design neurocognition distinguishes itself from the traditional field of cognitive neuroscience by studying the temporal and multimodal behaviour involved in designing (Park & Alderman Reference Park and Alderman2018). Designing entails a range of cognitive activities that can take minutes to generate solutions. Using neurophysiological measurements for assessing well-defined cognitive processes that only take seconds, with repeated tasks, is a reductionist viewpoint not applicable to the complex activities of concurrent cognitive processes occurring while designing. Neural responses of cognitive processes cannot be observed in isolation from other ongoing processes (Benedek et al. Reference Benedek, Christensen, Fink and Beaty2019). Shorter tasks do not allow gathering more complex behavioural assessments of higher order cognitive functions. The advantages and disadvantages of different neurophysiological measurements are beyond the scope of this paper. Designing involves many different processes and strategies that may vary over time (Hay et al. Reference Hay, Duffy, McTeague, Pidgeon, Vuletic and Grealy2017). Understanding how the brain interacts to produce higher cognitive functions such as those involved in designing and disentangling its cognitive processes through neurophysiological measurements asks for a macro perspective by distinguishing phases or stages of designing from theoretical models (Gero & Kannengiesser Reference Gero and Kannengiesser2004; Hatchuel & Weil Reference Hatchuel and Weil2009). Measurements of brain activity while designing provide a more objective foundation on which to confirm or build temporal theories of designing, than do cognitive measurements from protocol studies alone. Design neurocognition provides the context for the convergence of researchers from diverse disciplines and encourages contributions from the fields of neurocognition, cognitive psychology, neurophysiology, artificial intelligence and design theory, among others. This research context allows testing and verifying to what extent designing is different from other cognitive processes (Dorst Reference Dorst2011; Gero Reference Gero1990) and how it unfolds.

The present study does not look into cognitive processes, but at the neurophysiological activations throughout entire design tasks performed by designers from two different domains. We look at how problem-solving and designing translate into neurophysiological activations and unfold over time with the cognitive processes involved. We describe preliminary results from a controlled experiment that takes advantage of the temporal resolution of electroencephalography (EEG). A low-cost portable EEG device is used to measure neurophysiological activations across time when mechanical engineers and industrial designers are problem-solving and designing. This approach augments prior work on design cognition based on protocol studies, comparing mechanical engineers and industrial designers.

1.1. Designing and problem-solving

The study of the cognitive behaviour of designers based on methods from cognitive psychology, such as protocol analysis (Ericsson & Simon Reference Ericsson and Simon1993; Crutcher Reference Crutcher1994; Kan & Gero Reference Kan and Gero2017), has elucidated a number of characteristics of design cognition (Purcell et al. Reference Purcell, Williams, Gero and Colbron1993; Jiang et al. Reference Jiang, Gero, Yen and Gero2014; Hay et al. Reference Hay, Duffy, McTeague, Pidgeon, Vuletic and Grealy2017). The notions of problem space and solution space have populated design research interpretations of the designing process (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001; Kruger & Cross Reference Kruger and Cross2006) throughout its half century of formal study (Jones & Thornley Reference Jones and Thornley1963). One of the initial and core research questions is whether designing, as a cognitive process, is distinct from problem-solving (Goel & Pirolli Reference Goel and Pirolli1989, Reference Goel and Pirolli1992; Visser Reference Visser2009). In problem-solving all the characteristics of the problem are firstly defined, therefore, problem-solving is a closed task based on known constants and known variables. Designing involves the search for variables that relate to what is not known, such variables are context sensitive, can bring change and have variant meanings according to the design situation (Gero Reference Gero1990). In designing the characteristics of the problem are not all defined from the start, making designing an open task. Variables are introduced and change through evaluation processes and interdependencies within design issues (Vieira Reference Vieira2012). Distinguishing designing from problem-solving has implications for design research and design education in particular, since much of education is based on problem-solving theories.

1.2. Neurophysiological studies in design research

Neurophysiological studies offer insights into brain activity during the designing process. It has commensurability of measurements which makes such studies a robust approach for design research (Pidgeon et al. Reference Pidgeon, Grealy, Duffy, Hay, McTeague, Vuletic, Coyle and Gilbert2016; Borgianni & Maccioni Reference Borgianni and Maccioni2020). Neurophysiological design studies based on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) started a decade ago (Alexiou et al. Reference Alexiou, Zamenopoulos, Johnson and Gilbert2009) with a controlled experiment reporting preliminary results on the distinction between designing and problem-solving. Recent fMRI studies focussed on domain-related design issues (Hay et al. Reference Hay, Duffy, Gilbert, Lyall, Campbell, Coyle and Grealy2020), sustainability judgments (Goucher-Lambert, Moss & Cagan Reference Goucher-Lambert, Moss and Cagan2017), design ideation and inspirational stimuli (Goucher-Lambert, Moss & Cagan Reference Goucher-Lambert, Moss and Cagan2019) of mechanical engineers, graphic designers (Ellamil et al. Reference Ellamil, Dobson, Beeman and Christoff2011), and architects (Bermudez et al. Reference Bermudez, Krizaj, Lipschitz, Elliot Bueler, Rogowska and Yurgelun2017).

Studies using electroencephalography (EEG) commenced more than 40 years ago (Martindale & Hines Reference Martindale and Hines1975) investigating cortical activation during multiple tasks. Some 20 years later a study on categorization tasks of experts and novices made use of EEG in design research (Göker Reference Göker1997). In the last 10 years, single domain-related EEG design studies (Nguyen & Zheng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2010; Vecchiato et al. Reference Vecchiato, Jelic, Gaetano, Maglione, Matteis and Babiloni2015; Liu, Zeng & Ben Hamza Reference Liu, Zeng and Ben Hamza2016; Liang et al. Reference Liang, Lin, Yao, Chang, Liu and Chen2017; Liang et al. Reference Liang, Chang and Liu2018; Liu et al. Reference Liu, Li, Xiong, Cao and Yuan2018) and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) design studies (Shealy, Hu & Gero Reference Shealy, Hu and Gero2018; Shealy & Gero Reference Shealy and Gero2019) have been used to understand acts of designing from a neurophysiological perspective. Design is a temporal activity. EEG has started to play a role in design research because of its high temporal resolution, readily available software, reduction in the cost of portable equipment and relatively little need for specialized training. This has made the collection and analysis of EEG signals a viable option for design researchers. The limitations of low-cost EEG equipment when compared to medical grade systems, include low number of channels, physical stability, lower signal to noise ratio, lower sampling rate and serial measurement of electrodes (lasting a quarter of a second for the equipment used in this experiment). These are addressed by scalp preparation prior to electrode fixing, contact quality, artifact removal and filtering of the measurement to increase the signal to noise ratio, and outlier analysis that reduces variability. The sampling rate of the low-cost EEG device used in this study is higher than the cognitive activation rate found in previous cognitive studies (Gero and McNeil Reference Gero and Mc Neil1998; Goldschmidt Reference Goldschmidt2014). The cognitive actions of interest while designing span multiple seconds (Kan & Gero Reference Kan and Gero2017). Typical design sessions of 45 min generate between 600 and 1200 segments using protocol analysis, where each segment codes a cognitive action. This results in an average between 2.25 and 4.5 s per action. This is well within the capacity of a low-cost EEG device. Although low-cost EEG equipment has lower signal to noise ratio potentially resulting in a higher variability of the results and higher standard deviation than medical-grade equipment, the statistical approaches we describe in Section 2.6, compensate for the potential effects on the results due to the limitations of the equipment. Low-cost EEG non-invasive portable equipment becomes a viable measuring tool for the level of resolution we are interested in and for achieving preliminary results (Badcock et al. Reference Badcock, Mousikou, Mahajan, Lissa, Thie and McArthur2013; Badcock et al. Reference Badcock, Preece, Wit, Glenn, Fieder, Thie and McArthur2015; Bashivan et al. Reference Bashivan, Rish and Heisig2016; Hashemi et al. Reference Hashemi, Pino, Moffat, Mathewson, Aimone, Bennett, Schmidt and Sekuler2016; Krigolson et al. Reference Krigolson, Williams, Norton, Hassall and Colino2017).

1.3. Electroencephalography temporal resolution

Electroencephalography records electrical brain activity with electrodes placed along the scalp according to a standard distribution. Neurons transmit signals down the axon and the dendrites via an electrical impulse. EEG activity reflects the summation of the synchronous activity of thousands or millions of neurons that have similar spatial orientation. When cells have similar spatial orientation, their ions line up and create waves that can be detected. Pyramidal neurons of the cortex are thought to produce the most EEG signal because they are well-aligned and fire together (Sawyer Reference Sawyer2011; Dickter & Kieffaber Reference Dickter and Kieffaber2014). EEG measures electromagnetic fields generated by this neural activity. Activity from deep sources of the brain is more difficult to detect than currents near the skull, thus EEG is more sensitive to cortical activity (Dickter & Kieffaber Reference Dickter and Kieffaber2014; Hinterberger et al. Reference Hinterberger, Zlabinger and Blaser2014). Despite EEG’s limited spatial resolution, it can offer high temporal resolution in the order of milliseconds in a portable device. EEG’s high temporal resolution makes it a more suitable tool than fMRI to investigate designing as a temporal activity. However, fMRI offers both whole brain scans and a very high spatial resolution.

1.4. EEG results in design research

In design research, results from controlled experiments have identified neurophysiological EEG signals in which more visual thinking is spent during solution generation than in solution evaluation of a layout task (Nguyen & Zheng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2010). Further EEG bands associated with the design activities have been identified, namely beta 2, gamma 1 and gamma 2 (Liu, Zeng & Ben Hamza Reference Liu, Zeng and Ben Hamza2016), higher alpha power conducive for open ended tasks and divergent thinking, and theta and beta power associated to convergent thinking in decision-making and constraints tasks (Liu et al. Reference Liu, Li, Xiong, Cao and Yuan2018). EEG has been used in the study of visual attention and association in expert designers (Liang et al. Reference Liang, Lin, Yao, Chang, Liu and Chen2017), visual communication during idea incubation in expert and novice designers (Liang et al. Reference Liang, Chang and Liu2018), the study of effort, fatigue and concentration in conceptual design (Nguyen, Nguyen & Zeng Reference Nguyen, Nguyen and Zeng2017), and in the study of the neurophysiological correlates of embodiment and motivational factors during the perception of virtual architecture (Vecchiato et al. Reference Vecchiato, Jelic, Gaetano, Maglione, Matteis and Babiloni2015).

This paper describes a study from a larger research project whose goal is to investigate neurophysiological activation of designers across multiple design domains, namely mechanical engineering, industrial design, graphic design and architecture (Vieira et al. Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral, Gattol, Fernandes and Fernandes2019a, Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral, Gattol, Fernandes and Fernandes2019b, Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral, Gattol and Fernandes2020a). The study reported in this paper is based on the analysis of mechanical engineers’ and industrial designers’ neurophysiological activations using an EEG headset in the context of performing problem-solving and design tasks in a laboratory setting. The aims of the study are to:

  1. (i) distinguish neurophysiological activations between designing and problem-solving for both mechanical engineering and industrial design domains.

  2. (ii) distinguish neurophysiological activations between mechanical engineering and industrial design domains.

In the research reported in this paper we test the following hypotheses:

  1. (i) Hypothesis 1: the neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineers when problem-solving and designing are significantly different.

  2. (ii) Hypothesis 2: the neurophysiological activations of industrial designers when problem-solving and designing are significantly different.

  3. (iii) Hypothesis 3: the neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineers and industrial designers are significantly different for problem-solving and designing.

  4. (iv) Hypothesis 4: the neurophysiological temporal distributions of activations of problem-solving and designing are significantly different for mechanical engineers.

  5. (v) Hypothesis 5: the neurophysiological temporal distributions of activations of problem-solving and designing are significantly different for industrial designers.

  6. (vi) Hypothesis 6: the neurophysiological temporal activations of mechanical engineers and industrial designers are significantly different for problem-solving and designing.

2. Methods

The research team of this study consisted of seven researchers including design scientists, a data analyst, a statistician, a cognitive psychologist and a neurophysiologist. For the present study the analysis focussed on a subset of 55 experiment sessions of mechanical engineering and industrial design professional participants. The tasks and experimental procedure were piloted prior to the full study, through five different sessions each of which produced changes resulting in the final experiment.

The hypotheses are tested by using a problem-solving task as the control/reference and statistically comparing an open design task with the reference task. We compare: absolute values known as transformed power (Pow) and task-related power (TRP) described in Section 2.5. This study was approved by the local ethics committee of the University of Porto.

2.1. Participants

The participants were a convenience sample, the subset of 55 experimental sessions comprises 26 mechanical engineers and 29 industrial designers. Results are based on 36 right-handed participants, 18 mechanical engineers, aged 25–40 (M = 28.9, SD = 4.2), 10 males (age M = 29.0, SD = 5.3) and 8 females (age M = 28.7, SD = 2.5); and 18 industrial designers, aged 25–43 (M = 31.7, SD = 7.3), 10 males (age M = 35.1, SD = 7.2) and 8 females (age M = 27.5, SD = 5.1). The participants are all professionals (experience M = 5.9, SD = 6.0). The sample has 20 participants with experience in designing of 5 years or less, and 16 participants with experience in designing of more than 5 years. The experience of the industrial designers compared to the mechanical engineers is statistically different. However, the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) indicates no significant effect of domain on the EEG scores after controlling for experience, F(1, 33) = 2.3, p = 0.15.

2.2. Experiment tasks design

We adopted and replicated the problem-solving and layout design tasks described in the Alexiou et al. (Reference Alexiou, Zamenopoulos, Johnson and Gilbert2009) fMRI-based study. We matched Tasks 1 and 2 with the problem-solving and design tasks (Alexiou et al. Reference Alexiou, Zamenopoulos, Johnson and Gilbert2009) in terms of requests, number of constraints, stimuli and number of instructions (Appendix A). Task 1 is considered a problem-solving task as the problem itself is well-defined, and the set of solutions is both limited and unique (Alexiou et al. Reference Alexiou, Zamenopoulos, Johnson and Gilbert2009). We extended that experiment by adding an open layout design task to produce a block experiment in order to determine whether the open layout design task produces different results to the semi-closed layout Task 2 (Table 1). Both design tasks have no predetermined final state and the tasks are open-ended. Thus, the block experiment consisted of a sequence of three tasks: problem-solving layout design, basic layout design and open layout design (Figure 1). We added a fourth completely open design task that uses free-hand sketching after Task 3. Task 4 is an ill-defined and fully unconstrained task unrelated to formal problem-solving. Each participant was given two sheets of paper (A3 size) and three instruments, a pencil, graphite and a pen (Figure 1). The three design tasks (Tasks 2–4) require defining the problem and the solution spaces in a successive order of complexity. A neurophysiological subtraction between the problem-solving Task 1 and the design tasks will reveal brain magnetic fields more strongly involved in designing.

Table 1. Description of the problem-solving, basic design and open design tasks.

Figure 1. Depiction of the problem-solving Task 1, layout design Task 2, open layout design Task 3 and open free hand sketching design Task 4.

The set of four tasks was preceded by a pre-task so that participants could get used to the interface and headset. In the pre-task, the participant had to place a set of components for the design of a hair dryer. As with all block experiments, each subsequent task was potentially primed by the previous task. The Mikado pick-up-sticks game was given to the participants to play in the breaks between tasks (Figure 2). Results across the tasks were previously reported on single and two-domain studies (Vieira et al. Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral, Gattol, Fernandes and Fernandes2019a, Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral, Gattol, Fernandes and Fernandes2019b, Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral, Gattol and Fernandes2020a). For this paper, the focus is only on Tasks 1 and 4. Task 4 is an open design task while Task 1 is a well-defined, highly constrained problem, these are the two extremes of the way requirements of design tasks are presented. Task 4 has been selected because it has been used in previous protocol analysis studies of mechanical engineers and industrial designers and results about designers’ cognition are available (e.g. Jiang Reference Jiang2012).

Figure 2. Schematic sequence of the tasks’ procedure given to the participants.

2.3. Setup and procedure

A tangible interface for individual task performance was built based on magnetic material for easy handling (Figure 3). Differently from the original tasks (Alexiou et al. Reference Alexiou, Zamenopoulos, Johnson and Gilbert2009), the magnetic pieces were placed at the top of the vertical magnetic board to reduce signal noise due to eye and head horizontal movements. The pre-task was designed so that participants could familiarize themselves with the use of the EEG headset and with maneuvering the magnetic pieces that make up the physical interface and prevent participants from getting fixated in the problem-solving task, Task 1. This pre-task was also intended to familiarize participants with postural and facial movements and allowed the researcher to correct and fine-tune necessary adjustments before advancing to the block experiment.

Figure 3. Emotiv Epoc+ electrodes highlighted in grey against the 10–10 I.S. layout and experiment setup using the headset.

Participants are instructed to wash their hair prior to attending the experiment session to improve contact quality. One researcher was present in each experiment session to instruct the participant and to check for recording issues. A period of 10 min for setting up and a few minutes for a short introduction was necessary for informing each participant, reading and signing of the consent agreement and setting the room temperature. Each participant was reminded to use the bathroom, spit out any gum and asked to untie their hair before the start of the experiment. The researcher checked metallic accessories for electromagnetic interference and contact lenses. The researcher sat at the desk and checked posture of each participant. In order to reduce excessive signal artifacts, the researcher asked each participant as far as possible to avoid neck movements, blinking, muscle contractions as well avoid rotating their head, horizontal eye movements, pressing the lips and teeth together during the tasks. The researcher followed a script to conduct the experiment so that each participant was presented with the same information and stimuli. Before each task, participants were asked to start by reading the text describing the task which took an average of 10 s. Then the participants performed the sequence of tasks (Figure 2). The participants were asked to stay silent during the tasks and use the breaks for talking and clarifications. If needed, extra time was given to the participants, in particular in Task 4, so they could find a satisfactory solution. Instructions given to the participants are reported in Appendix A.

2.4. Data collection methods

EEG activity was recorded using a portable 14-channel system Emotiv Epoc+. Electrodes are arranged according to the 10–10 I.S standard (Figure 3).

Electromagnetic interference of the room was checked for frequencies below 60 Hz. Each of the Emotiv Epoc+ channels collects continuous signals of electrical activity at their location. The participants performed the tasks on the physical magnetic board, with two video cameras capturing the participant’s face and activity and an audio recorder. All the data captures were streamed using Panopto software (https://www.panopto.com/) that also allows for direct screen capture, Figure 4.

Figure 4. Screen capture depicting audio, video and screen captures streaming in Panopto.

A total of 100 experimental sessions constitute our major dataset of which 90 took place at the University of Porto, between March and July of 2017, and June and September of 2018. Ten sessions took place in the Design Hub of Mouraria, Lisbon, during August 2018 in rooms with the necessary conditions for the experiment, such as natural lighting sufficient for performing experiments between 9:00 and 15:00 and no electromagnetic interference. Only the 55 that involved either mechanical engineers or industrial designers are considered in this paper.

2.5. Data processing methods

The 14 electrodes were disposed according to the 10–10 I.S. (Figure 3) 256 Hz sampling rate, low cutoff 0.1 Hz, high cutoff 50 Hz. Several methods in the literature (Mannan et al. Reference Mannan, Kamran and Jeong2018) seek to split the EEG signal into components, assuming that the measurements are characterized by two different underlying patterns, whose mathematical correlations between similar components and non-correlations between different components can be empirically extracted along the signal measured in time. We adopted the blind source separation (BSS) technique based on canonical correlation analysis (CCA) for the removal of muscle artifacts from EEG recordings (Borga & Knutsson Reference Borga and Knutsson2001; De Clercq et al. Reference De Clercq, Vergult, Vanrumste, Van Paesschen and Van Huffel2006) adapted to remove the short EMG bursts due to articulation of spoken language, attenuating the muscle artifacts contamination on the EEG recordings (Vos et al. Reference Vos, Riès, Vanderperren, Vanrumste, Alario, Huffel and Burle2010). The BSS–CCA algorithm, by using correlation as a criterion to measure independence of signals, takes into account temporal correlation. By establishing an ordering system of the separated singular valued components of the signal, the outputted components are sorted so that the highest correlated sources represent EEG sources and the lowest correlated sources represent noise. By systematically eliminating a subset of the bottom sources, the EEG signals from all subjects used in this study were cleaned. More specifically, by turning the last four sources to zero, the cleaned EEG signal is reconstructed as a combination of the remaining sources identified. Thus, data processing includes the removal of Emotiv specific DC offset with the Infinite Impulse Response (IIR) filter and BSS–CCA. Data analysis included total and band power values on individual and aggregate levels using MatLab and the EEGLab open source software. All the EEG segments of the recorded data were used for averaging throughout the entire tasks, from beginning to end. Unpublished results testing the BSS–CCA procedure’s efficacy on EEG signals from participants performing sketching tasks in three conditions including pen and paper confirm its 99% efficacy. The statistical approach we describe in Section 2.6, compensates for the potential effects on the procedure due to the limitations of the equipment. As Task 4 involves muscular activity given that participants sketched, the procedure described in De Clercq et al. (Reference De Clercq, Vergult, Vanrumste, Van Paesschen and Van Huffel2006) was used. All the tasks of this experiment involve thinking and motion. The motor actions involved in the tasks using the tangible interface versus the free hand sketching and their corresponding EEG signals are of the same source, thus we claim that the BSS–CCA procedure filters the signal.

Each of the Emotiv Epoc+ channel’s continuous electrical signal is processed to produce multiple measures. Here, we report only on two: transformed power (Pow) and task-related power (TRP). The Pow is the transformed power, more specifically the mean of the squared values of microvolts per second (μV/s) for each electrode’s processed signal per task. This measure tells us about the amplitude of the signal per channel, per participant magnified to absolute values. We present Pow values on aggregates of participants’ individual results, for each domain cohort, per total task and for each task deciles for the temporal analysis. The TRP is the task-related power, typically calculated taking the resting state as the reference period per individual (Dickter & Kieffaber Reference Dickter and Kieffaber2014; Schwab et al. Reference Schwab, Benedek, Papousek, Weiss and Fink2014; Rominger et al. Reference Rominger, Papousek, Perchtold, Weber, Weiss and Fink2018). This method that is normally time-locked to fractions of seconds. In this study, we time-locked on a scale of multiple seconds to allow for the design activity to unfold. We make a shift of the focus on time-locking the experiments equally for all participants, to the unfolding of the designing and problem-solving cognitive activities until the solution is produced. Thus, our experiment is time-locked for the complete unfolding of the cognitive activities involved in each task.

Of the 55 sessions recorded, 7 had recording problems that rendered them unsuitable for further analysis and were excluded from further analysis. These problems ranged from failing to switch on all the equipment, equipment failures, participants’ handedness and failure to complete the experiment. We processed the Pow and the TRP measurements for each participant per total task and temporal deciles per task, then after determining the outliers, we calculated the mean and standard deviation of each measure for each cohort. A z-score was conducted in the analysis of Pow across tasks for each of the remaining 48 participants data to determine outliers. The criteria for excluding participants were based on the evidence of 6 or more channel threshold z-score values above 1.96 or below −1.96 and individual measurements above 2.81 or below −2.81. This resulted in a further eight sessions being excluded leaving 40. After the division of the Pow into time deciles (which provides the basis for the analysis of temporal stages) amplitude leading to 2.5 standard deviations from the mean as threshold values were excluded per channel, 4 experiments were further excluded leaving 36, 18 per domain.

2.6. Data analysis methods

As the focus of the study is to determine how well neurophysiological activations during designing can be distinguished from those during problem-solving, we take the problem-solving task, Task 1 as the control/reference period for the TRP calculations. Thus, for each electrode, the following formula was applied taking the log of the Pow of the corresponding electrode i (of 14), in Task 1 as the reference period. By subtracting the log-transformed power of the reference period (Pow i, reference) from the activation period (Pow i, activation) for each trial j (per participant), according to the formula:

(1) $$ {TRP}_i=\mathit{\log}{\left({Pow}_i, activation\right)}_j-\mathit{\log}{\left({Pow}_i, reference\right)}_j $$

By doing this, negative values indicate a decrease of task-related power from the reference (problem-solving Task 1) for the activation period, while positive values indicate a power increase (Pfurtscheller & Lopes da Silva Reference Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva1999) (power and activation refer to brain wave amplitude).

2.7. Statistical approach

We performed standard statistical analyses based on the design of the experiment: always a mixed repeated-measures design with pairwise comparisons to follow up on specific differences with task, hemisphere, electrode and decile as within-subject factors and domain as the between-subjects factor. These analyses were performed for the dependent variable of Pow and for all the within-subject variables. The threshold for significance in all the analyses is p ≤ 0.05. We further describe each statistical comparison performed in relation to the hypotheses and the methods used. To compare the Pow of the tasks within domain we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere and electrode (Appendix B). We then compared the Pow of the tasks between domains and performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the between-subjects factor domain and the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere and electrode (Appendix B). To investigate the temporal behaviour within each domain, we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere, electrode and decile (Appendix C). To compare the Pow for the temporal analysis in deciles across domains, we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the between-subject factors of task, hemisphere, electrode and decile (Appendix D). We used only the results for the two tasks of interest from the analyses of the full data set.

3. Analysis of results

3.1. Task-related power across domains

The TRP results for the two domains and tasks are depicted in Figure 6. The radar plot simulates the two hemispheres by distributing the electrodes (10–10 I.S.) symmetrically around a vertical axis. Total TRP scores per electrode can be considered by comparing with the vertical scale and across the tasks (Figure 5). Once the problem-solving Task 1 (reference task) is subtracted from itself to produce the reference, it shows up as an orange circle with a value of zero for all electrode measurements. The difference is shown as higher or lower activation of the electrodes/regions for Task 4 within or beyond the orange circle border. The sketching Task 4 shows higher neurophysiological activation compared to the Task 1 in both domains.

Figure 5. Radar plot of task-related power (TRP) for the 14 electrodes by taking the problem-solving Task 1 as the reference period for Task 4 for the industrial designers and mechanical engineers. The electrode labels are distributed such that all the electrodes on the left hemisphere are on the left and all those on the right hemisphere are on the right. Their labelling uses the standard 10–10 I.S.

3.2. Analysis of transformed power within domains

Pow results for each domain cohort are depicted in Figures 6 and 7. Pow scores per electrode (average of entire task) can be considered by comparing the vertical scale values and across the two tasks. The open sketching design Task 4 shows higher neurophysiological activation from Task 1 in both domains, Figure 6. Mechanical engineers show higher activations in the problem-solving Task 1. Detailed analysis results from running the 5 × 2 × 7 repeated-measurement ANOVA for each domain are presented in Appendix B. Below, we report on significant (p ≤ 0.05) differences, the channels of significant differences between the two tasks within domain are shown highlighted with a solid circle. The circles indicate significant differences from the pairwise comparisons that were conducted to follow up on the main effects and interaction effects.

Figure 6. Transformed power (Pow) of the two tasks for each domain. The solid circles indicate channels where there are significant differences between the tasks within each domain.

Figure 7. Transformed power (Pow) of the two tasks per domain. The solid circles indicate channels and task where there are significant differences across domains.

From the analysis of the mechanical engineers the pairwise comparisons revealed that the open design sketching Task 4 does not show significantly different neurophysiological activations from problem-solving Task 1 (p = 0.10).

3.2.1. Significant main effects

From the analysis of the industrial designers, the pairwise comparisons revealed that the open design sketching Task 4 shows significantly different neurophysiological activations from problem-solving Task 1 (p < 0.001). From the analysis of each cohort, we found significant main effects per domain (Table 2).

Table 2. Significant main effects from the ANOVA for each domain

These significant main effects related to different tasks provide evidentiary support for Hypothesis 2, as all the factors show main effects for the industrial designers, but not to Hypothesis 1, as only hemisphere and electrode show main effects for the mechanical engineers but not task. Task, hemisphere and electrode are further investigated in the temporal analysis, presented in Section 4.

3.3. Analysis of transformed power across domains

Detailed analysis results from running the 2 × 5 × 2 × 7 mixed repeated-measurement ANOVA are presented in Appendix B. We conducted pairwise comparisons to check for differences comparing the seven electrodes per hemisphere and task across domains. Below, we report on significant (p ≤ 0.05) differences (Figure 7). The channels of significant differences per task between domains are shown highlighted with a solid circle. The circles indicate significant differences from the pairwise comparisons that were conducted to follow up on the main effects and interaction effects. The pairwise comparisons revealed that the open design sketching Task 4 shows significantly different neurophysiological activations from problem-solving Task 1 (p < 0.01).

3.3.1. Significant main effects

From the analysis of the 36 participants, we found significant main effects and significant interaction effects between multiple factors (Table 3). Of particular interest is the significant main effect related to the tasks. No significant main effect was found for the between-subjects factor domain (p = 0.67).

Table 3. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 36 participants

These significant main effects provide evidentiary support for Hypothesis 3 concerning differences between problem-solving and designing across the two domains.

4. Temporal analysis results

Designing is a temporal activity, a single value based on aggregating across an entire session hides how designing unfolds over time. Different cognitive behaviours exhibited when design sessions are divided into deciles show this temporal activity in designing (Jiang et al. Reference Jiang, Gero, Yen and Gero2014; Kan & Gero, Reference Kan and Gero2017). The division of each design session’s data into temporal deciles allows a more detailed analysis of the temporal dimension stages.

4.1. Problem-solving and design tasks of mechanical engineers

To compare the Pow scores for the deciles by domain, we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere, electrode and decile, for each domain (Appendix C).

4.1.1. Significant main effects

From the analysis of the 18 mechanical engineers we found significant main effects and significant interaction effects between the factors shown in Table 4.

Table 4. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 18 mechanical engineers

In addition, we conducted pairwise comparisons for hemisphere, electrode, decile and task.

The significant main effects for mechanical engineers relate to task and to decile and the significant interaction effect relates to task + decile. These provide evidentiary support for Hypothesis 4. For a more detailed analysis, we carried out pairwise comparisons that showed differences comparing hemispheres for the seven electrodes and tasks within each decile.

The significant (p ≤ 0.05) pairwise comparisons found between Task 1 (problem-solving task) and Task 4 (open free hand sketching task) across the deciles (Appendix C) are shown in Figure 8. Solid circles represent the channels with activation of statistically significant differences in Task 4 from Task 1. Problem-solving Task 1 shows increased general activation in some deciles (1, 2 and 5), Task 4 shows higher variation of temporal distributions of activations across deciles.

Figure 8. For mechanical engineers, channels with significant differences from Task 1 to Task 4 by decile, shown as solid circles.

The placement of the electrodes related to each cortex of the brain is shown in Figure 9. Higher activations of channels of the right occipitotemporal cortex and secondary visual cortex occur in Task 4. Statistically significant differences between the open sketching design Task 4 and problem-solving task, Task 1 occur in the prefrontal cortex, right temporal and visual cortices. The channels of significant differences with decreased activation from Task 4 to Task 1 are located in the prefrontal cortex.

Figure 9. Electrodes placement related to each cortex of the brain.

4.2. Problem-solving and design tasks of industrial designers

To compare the Pow scores for the deciles we performed the same analysis as for the mechanical engineers by running a 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere, electrode and decile (Appendix C).

4.2.1. Significant main effects

From the analysis of the 18 industrial designers we found significant main effects and significant interaction effects between the factors as shown in Table 5.

Table 5. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 18 industrial designers.

In addition, we conducted pairwise comparisons for hemisphere, electrode, decile and task. The pairwise comparisons indicate that Task 4 differs significantly from:

  • problem-solving Task 1 (p < .01)

The significant main effects for industrial designers relate to task and the significant interaction effect relates to task + decile. These provide evidentiary support for Hypothesis 5.

The pairwise comparisons showed differences comparing hemispheres for the seven electrodes and tasks within each decile. In Figure 10, we show significant (p ≤ 0.05) pairwise comparisons found between Task 1 (problem-solving task) and Task 4 (open free hand sketching task) across the deciles (Appendix C).

Figure 10. For industrial designers, channels with significant differences from Task 1 to Task 4 by decile, shown as solid circles.

Generally, higher activations of channels of the right occipitotemporal cortex and secondary visual cortex are shown in Task 4. Statistically significant differences between Task 4 and problem-solving Task 1 occur bilaterally in the dorsolateral prefrontal, temporal and visual cortices. All the channels of significant differences show increased activation from Task 1 to Task 4.

4.3. Problem-solving and design tasks across domains

Detailed analysis results from running the 2 × 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 mixed repeated-measurement ANOVA are presented in Appendix D. We conducted pairwise comparisons to check for differences comparing the 7 electrodes per hemisphere, task and decile across domains. Below we report on significant (p ≤ .05) differences.

4.3.1. Significant main effects

From the analysis of the 36 participants we found significant main effects and significant interaction effects between multiple factors, Table 6. Of particular interest is the significant main effect related to deciles, task, significant interaction effects related to task + deciles and task + domain. No significant main effect was found for the between-subjects factor domain (p = .28).

Table 6. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 36 participants

These significant main effects relate to task and decile and the significant interaction effects relate to task and domain and task and decile provide evidentiary support for Hypothesis 6 concerning differences in temporal distributions of activations between problem-solving and designing, between tasks and across domain.

5. Discussion and conclusion

With this controlled experiment we showed evidence for significant differences between two prototypical problem-solving and open design tasks, between two cohorts from the domains of mechanical engineering and industrial design. The neurophysiological differences of task-related power between problem-solving Task 1 and the sketching Task 4, support a number of inferences:

  1. (i) Significant higher amplitudes in the open sketching design Task 4 for both cohorts were measured, meaning that from this study open design sketching, as expected, is the most distinguishable design activity from problem-solving thinking mode. The relevance of sketching while designing has been studied (Goldschmidt Reference Goldschmidt1994; Scrivener, Ball & Tseng Reference Scrivener, Ball and Tseng2000; Kavakli & Gero Reference Kavakli and Gero2001; Bilda, Gero & Purcell Reference Bilda, Gero and Purcell2006). Our preliminary results show higher amplitude for sketching at the aggregate level. Further analysis of the data might reveal other aspects when comparing novices and experienced designers (Vieira et al. Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral, Gattol and Fernandes2020a), categorical stages (Vieira et al. Reference Vieira, Gero, Delmoral and Fernandes2020b) and the conceptual categories of the participants’ sketching solutions.

  2. (ii) Dissimilar amplitude results found in the open sketching design Task 4 between domains could involve differences in the confidence in sketching, as sketching skills might differ between these mechanical engineers and industrial designers. This could have influenced the results in particular, of the channel P8, whose cognitive functions are associated with drawing (Harrington et al. Reference Harrington, Farias, Davis and Buonocore2007).

  3. (iii) Dissimilar adaptive approaches can be inferred as the constraints of the magnetic elements and physical solution space of Task 1 disappeared in Task 4. Industrial designers showed higher change of activations implying a greater adaptation to the open design task. This supports prior cognitive results that show mechanical engineers do not necessarily change nor adapt their approach when constraints are reduced, and more freedom of choice is given (Jiang et al. Reference Jiang, Gero, Yen and Gero2014). Probably due to their education, mechanical engineers show higher amplitudes in channels related to deductive reasoning, F7 (Goel et al. Reference Goel, Gold, Kapur and Houle1997) and lower amplitudes in channels associated with inductive reasoning, F3 and P7 (Goel et al. Reference Goel, Gold, Kapur and Houle1997).

  4. (iv) Hemisphere and electrode significant differences were found between the two tasks for both cohorts for all of the within-domain analyses. Channels of significant differences between Task 1 and Task 4 were found in the occipitotemporal and prefrontal cortices.

We further discuss these channels associated with Brodmann areas’ specific cognitive functions and the hypotheses of this study. EEG measures electromagnetic fields generated by neural cortical activity, as a consequence no direct inferences can be made in relation to cognitive functions activity from deep sources of the brain (Sawyer Reference Sawyer2011; Dickter & Kieffaber Reference Dickter and Kieffaber2014; Hinterberger et al. Reference Hinterberger, Zlabinger and Blaser2014). However, we can infer that each channel is associated with the cognitive functions of the Brodmann area onto which it is mapped in the brain hemispheres. Brodmann’s studies on brain cells’ neuron structure and its cytoarchitecture (Brodmann Reference Brodmann1909) have been refined and correlated to various cortical functions and cognitive activities by measuring blood flow in response to different mental tasks, Figure 11.

Figure 11. Approximate Brodmann areas (numbered) of the 10–10 I.S. placement system.

Research using fMRI over the past decades has contributed to the development of our understanding about the localization of function in the brain and has helped shape a consensus (Dickter & Kieffaber Reference Dickter and Kieffaber2014). Multiple magnetic resonance imaging measurements have resulted in an extended map (Glasser et al. Reference Glasser, Coalson, Robinson, Hacker, Harwell, Yacoub, Ugurbil, Andersson, Beckmann, Jenkinson, Smith and Van Essen2016), with each discrete area containing cells with not only similar structure, but also function and connectivity. Various cognitive functions and connectivity have been identified in studies using fMRI and positron emission tomography (PET).

5.1. Hypotheses

Statistically significant differences were found indicating preliminary evidence for some of the hypotheses. The results of the analysis of the EEG data of the 36 participants showed differences in the neurophysiological activity of these cohorts of mechanical engineers and industrial designers across tasks and provide initial support for five of the six hypotheses.

  • Hypothesis 1: the neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineers when problem-solving (Task 1) and designing (Task 4) are significantly different.

Results from the repeated-measurement ANOVA for this domain do not support Hypothesis 1. For these mechanical engineers, statistically significant differences were not found between the open sketching design Task 4 and problem-solving Task 1. However, main effects were found for hemisphere and electrode. This is consistent with recent results between open-ended and constrained tasks of product design engineers where no significant differences were found (Hay et al. Reference Hay, Duffy, Gilbert, Lyall, Campbell, Coyle and Grealy2020). However other aspects are revealed by the temporal analysis.

  • Hypothesis 2: the neurophysiological activations of industrial designers when problem-solving (Task 1) and designing (Task 4) are significantly different.

Results from the repeated-measurement ANOVA for the industrial designers, show statistically significant differences between the open sketching design Task 4 and the problem-solving Task 1, providing support for Hypothesis 2.

  • Hypothesis 3: the neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineers and industrial designers are significantly different for problem-solving and designing.

From the results of the mixed repeated-measurement ANOVA across domains, no significant main effect was found for domain, meaning that a common design ground is shared by the cohorts. However, an interaction effect between domain and hemisphere was found. These hemispheric differences will be explored in a future paper.

Statistically significant differences between the open design sketching Task 4 and the problem-solving Task 1 were found. Although differences can be found from the analysis within domain, the two cohorts as a whole, show significant differences between designing and problem-solving prototypical tasks, providing support for Hypothesis 3.

The pairwise comparisons show significant differences between the tasks in channel P7 between domains. In the left hemisphere, channel P7 maps on Brodmann area 37 involved in lexico-semantic associations and semantic categorization (Gerlach et al. Reference Gerlach, Law, Gade and Paulson2000), attention to semantic relations (MacDermott et al. 2003), metaphor comprehension (Rapp et al. Reference Rapp, Leube, Erb, Grodd and Kircher2004) and deductive reasoning (Goel et al. Reference Goel, Gold, Kapur and Houle1998). It could be inferred that mechanical engineer’s specific domain training sets them to prioritize some of these cognitive functions over others. The open requests of the task might not correspond to the specific nature of the requests these mechanical engineers are used to responding to. They may not be using the metaphorical and associative nature of exploring within the problem space (Goel & Pirolli Reference Goel and Pirolli1989, Reference Goel and Pirolli1992), nor producing a solution-focussed primary concept (Darke Reference Darke1979; Cross Reference Cross2006; Dorst Reference Dorst2011, Reference Dorst2015) before carrying out an analysis of the proposal. Further research is needed to elucidate this potential explanation.

Pairwise comparisons within domains revealed significant differences in two channels between the tasks. The mechanical engineers exhibit higher amplitude of FC6 in Task 4, and industrial designers start with lower amplitude of this channel in Task 1 and progressively surpass in amplitude that of mechanical engineers in Task 4. The channel FC6 is associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann area 44. In the right hemisphere onto which it is mapped, BA 44 cognitive functions of interest are associated with goal-intensive processing (Fincham et al. Reference Fincham, Carter, Veen, Stenger and Anderson2002) and search for originality (Nagornova Reference Nagornova2007). The open design requests of Task 4 prompt them to explore and plan ahead within the problem and solution space (Dorst & Cross Reference Dorst and Cross2001). The mechanical engineers and the industrial designers also show significant differences in channel O1, associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann area 18. In the left hemisphere onto which it is mapped BA18 is associated with the cognitive functions of visual mental imagery (Platel et al. Reference Platel, Price, Baron, Wise, Lambert, Frackowiak, Lechevalier and Eustache1997) and visual word form (Vorobyev et al. Reference Vorobyev, Alho, Medvedev, Pakhomov, Roudas, Rutkovskaya, Tervaniemi, Van Zuijen and Näätänen2004), such cognitive functions are associated with open designing.

  • Hypothesis 4: the neurophysiological temporal distributions of activations of problem-solving (Task 1) and designing (Task 4) are significantly different for mechanical engineers.

Results from the repeated-measurement ANOVA within domain, show statistically significant difference between the open sketching design Task 4 and the problem-solving task, Task 1, across deciles for the domain of the mechanical engineers. This provides initial support for Hypothesis 4.

In addition to the channels FC6, O1 and P7, the pairwise comparisons show significant differences between the tasks across deciles for the channels AF3 (deciles 1 and 2), O2 (decile 8) and AF4 and T7 (decile 10). Channel AF3, which maps onto the left prefrontal cortex, is associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann area 09, such as deductive reasoning (Goel et al. Reference Goel, Gold, Kapur and Houle1997) and metaphoric comprehension (Shibata et al. Reference Shibata, Abe, Terao and Miyamoto2007). Higher activation of these areas involves semantic and pragmatic processing which differ from those in literal comprehension. The mechanical engineers show significant differences and lower amplitude in channel AF3 in Task 4 for the first two deciles inverting the tendency from decile 4.

Channel O2, which maps onto the right occipitotemporal cortex, is associated with visuo-spatial information processing (Wabersky et al. Reference Waberski, Gobbele, Lamberty, Buchner, Marshall and Fink2008). The mechanical engineers show significant differences for this channel only in decile 8.

In the last deciles, mechanical engineers show significant differences for channel O1 previously mentioned, and channels T7 and AF4. Mapped onto the temporal cortex of the left hemisphere, channel T7’s known associated cognitive functions are not easy to relate to design cognition. However, its higher activation during visual word recognition (Pekkola et al. Reference Pekkola, Ojanen, Autti, Jääskeläinen, Möttönen, Tarkiainen and Sams2005) may suggest that it relates to generating internal representations of speech from reading the design request.

Mapped on the right hemisphere, prefrontal cortex, channel AF4, which is associated with the cognitive functions of coordinating visual spatial memory (Slotnick & Moo Reference Slotnick and Moo2006), planning (Fincham et al. Reference Fincham, Carter, Veen, Stenger and Anderson2002) and decision-making (Rogers et al. Reference Rogers, Owen, Middleton, Williams, Pickard, Sahakian and Robbins1999), shows significant differences in the last decile.

  • Hypothesis 5: the neurophysiological temporal distributions of activations of problem-solving (Task 1) and designing (Task 4) are significantly different for industrial designers.

Results from the repeated-measurement ANOVA within domain, show statistically significant difference between the open sketching design Task 4 and the problem-solving task, Task 1, across deciles for the domain of the industrial designers. This provides initial support for Hypothesis 5.

The pairwise comparisons show significant differences between the tasks for the industrial designers across deciles. Besides channels AF4, FC6, O2, O1, P7, T7, F3, AF3 with their cognitive functions, significant differences were also found for channels F4, F8, T8 and P8, which all map onto the right hemisphere.

Channels F4 and F8, which map onto the right prefrontal cortex, show significant differences in deciles 6 and 10. Channel F4 is associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann area 08, of executive control (Kübler, Dixon & Garavan Reference Kübler, Dixon and Garavan2006) and planning (Crozier et al. Reference Crozier, Sirigu, Lehéricy, van de Moortele, Pillon, Grafman, Agid, Dubois and LeBihan1999). Channel F8 is associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann area 45, such as response inhibition (Marsh et al. Reference Marsh, Zhu, Schultz, Quackenbush, Royal, Skudlarski and Peterson2006), probably reflecting preparing to finish the task. Channel T8, which maps onto the right temporal cortex, is associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann area 21, such as observation of motion (Rizzolatti et al. Reference Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Matelli, Bettinardi, Paulesu, Perani and Fazio1996). Industrial designers’ training skills in sketching develops attention to gestures, and this translates in higher activation. Channel P8, which maps onto the right occipitotemporal cortex, is associated with the cognitive functions of Brodmann area 37, such as monitoring shape (Le, Pardo & Hu Reference Le, Pardo and Hu1998), visual fixation (Richter et al. Reference Richter, Costello, Sponheim, Lee and Pardo2004) and drawing (Harrington et al. Reference Harrington, Farias, Davis and Buonocore2007). Industrial designers show significant differences for this channel in 8 of 10 deciles in which we infer they prioritize some of these cognitive functions in open design requests.

  • Hypothesis 6: the neurophysiological temporal activations of mechanical engineers and industrial designers are significantly different for problem-solving and designing.

Results from the mixed repeated-measurement ANOVA, revealed no significant main effect for domain. However, an interaction effect between domain and task was found and this provides initial support for Hypothesis 6.

Results from this study, in particular the results from the industrial designers, provide preliminary evidence to the initial and core research question whether designing is distinct from problem-solving (Goel & Pirolli Reference Goel and Pirolli1989, Reference Goel and Pirolli1992; Visser Reference Visser2009) at least on the level of neurophysiological activations between a problem-solving layout task and an open sketching design task. The differences found between mechanical engineers and industrial designers in terms of activations match differences found in cognitive studies of mechanical engineers and industrial designers, where mechanical engineers did not change their cognitive behaviour related to the openness of the task, but industrial designers did (Jiang et al. Reference Jiang, Gero, Yen and Gero2014). However, a common design ground was also found for these two design activities as the analyses show no effect for domain, and significant differences between the tasks across deciles were found for both. The results from this EEG study provide an alternate form of evidence than protocol studies to support differences between problem-solving and designing.

This paper has demonstrated, through a detailed analysis of EEG data, that a low-cost EEG device can be used to produce preliminary results in design research, results capable of being used to provide initial testing of hypotheses.

5.2. Limitations of the study

The data set is from a convenience sample of volunteers who responded to the call for participants. The participants’ familiarity with layout tasks and their sketching ability have the potential to influence the results. The statistical approach we described reduced the potential effects on the results of the limitations of using a low-cost EEG device. Due to the low spatial resolution of EEG the results cannot support strong claims related to location, as fields extend across the brain (Sawyer, Reference Sawyer2011). To better identify unique brain regions associated with neural activity a larger number of EEG channels is needed or techniques that provide a higher spatial resolution are required.

5.3. Further work

Analysis of the frequency bands and temporal analysis of the EEG data is in progress to produce a more detailed articulation of the neurophysiological activations of these cohorts of mechanical engineers and industrial designers and to relate them to published results (Nguyen & Zheng Reference Nguyen and Zeng2010; Vecchiato et al. Reference Vecchiato, Jelic, Gaetano, Maglione, Matteis and Babiloni2015; Liu et al. Reference Liu, Zeng and Ben Hamza2016; Liang et al. Reference Liang, Lin, Yao, Chang, Liu and Chen2017).The results we already have allow for the comparison between domains at multiple levels of details. These comparisons will be the subject of a future paper. More data needs to be collected to understand the extent of EEG data for design studies. Think-aloud protocols of individuals and design teams are being collected separately while measuring EEG responses. Further research is needed for the removal of artefacts due to speech and motor skeletal bursts without removing signals that result from cognitive actions. If successful, the cleaned data would allow for the direct temporal matching of design cognition, derived from think-aloud protocol, with neurophysiological measurements, opening up a new research direction for neurocognitive research in design studies.

More research connecting brain region activations and cognitive functions is needed to make design neurocognition a research tool that supersedes such cognitive research tools as protocol analysis.

Acknowledgments and financial support

This research was supported by the Unity of Conceptual and Experimental Validation of INEGI-Feup. We thankfully acknowledge the consultancy of the neurophysiologist Dílio Alves of the local hospital, Prof. Manoel Silva for the hints on magnetism and Prof. João Paulo Cunha of INESC-Feup. The research is funded by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology, grant number SFRH/BPD/104281/2014.

The second author acknowledges the support of the U.S. National Science Foundation, Grant Nos CMMI-1762415 and EEC-1929896. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.

Appendix A

Instructions given to the participants for and in between tasks.

Pre-task

  1. 1.1 Ask participant to: “Please start by reading the text after I say OK.”

  2. 1.2 Ask participant to: “Please touch the orange end button in the board after concluding each task.”

Task request: Please arrange the components so that:

  1. (i) the hairdryer has a complete mechanism,

  2. (ii) each component is in its functional place and

  3. (iii) the hairdryer has a closed surface.

First task break: “we have this game for you to play while I prepare the board for the next task.”

Task 1

  1. 1.3 Ask participant to: “Please start by reading the text after I say OK.”

  2. 1.4 Ask participant to: “Please touch the orange end button in the board after concluding each task.”

Task request: Please arrange the furniture so that:

  1. (i) the bed is in the corner of the room with the head on the west,

  2. (ii) the wardrobe is next to the door and next to the bed and

  3. (iii) the desk is under the window.

Second task break: “please continue playing the game while I prepare the board for the next task.”

Task 2

  1. 1.5 Ask participant to: “Please start by reading the text after I say OK.”

  2. 1.6 Ask participant to: “Please touch the orange end button in the board after concluding each task.”

Task request: Please arrange the furniture so that:

  1. (i) the room is functional,

  2. (ii) the room is comfortable and

  3. (iii) it has at least a bed, a wardrobe and a desk.

Third task break: “please continue playing the game while I prepare the board for the next task.”

Task 3

  1. 1.7 Ask participant to: “Please start by reading the text after I say OK.”

  2. 1.8 “Both boards are available.”

  3. 1.9 Ask participant to: “Please touch the orange end button in the board after concluding each task.”

Task request: Please arrange a space and its furniture so that:

  1. (i) it uses some or all of the wall elements, the door, window and balcony,

  2. (ii) it uses some or all of the furniture and

  3. (iii) privacy is important for the sleeping area.

Interview 1 – Ask participants the following questions:

  1. 1. How satisfied are you with the result of the third task?

  2. 2. Did you experience difficulties?

  3. 3. How did you determine your trajectories in each task performed?

  4. 4. How did you implement your design strategy if you used one?

Fourth task break: “please continue playing the game while I prepare the board for the next task.”

Task 4

  1. 1.10 Ask participant to: “Please start by reading the text after I say OK.”

  2. 1.11 Inform the participant: “You have 10 min for this task. I will let you know 3 min before the end with a hand sign.”

  3. 1.12 Ask participant to: “Please touch the orange end button in the board after concluding each task.”

    Task request: Please propose and represent an outline design for a future personal entertainment system.

Interview 2 – Ask participants the following questions:

  1. 1. How satisfied are you with the result of the fourth task?

  2. 2. Did you experience difficulties?

  3. 3. How did you determine your trajectories in the task performed?

  4. 4. How did you implement your design strategy if you used one?

Appendix B

To compare the Pow of mechanical engineers we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors task, hemisphere and electrode. From the analysis of the 18 participants we found a significant main effect of: hemisphere, F(1, 17) = 16.88, p ≤ 0.001, η2 partial = 0.50; and electrode, F GG(2.85, 48.44) = 3, p < 0.01, η2 partial = .15 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.89). The pairwise comparisons revealed that Task 4 does not differs significantly from Task 1 (p = 0.11). In addiction the pairwise comparisons revealed that some electrodes were significant between Task 1 and Task 4, namely, in the left hemisphere electrode O1(p = 0.02) and in the right hemisphere electrode FC6(p = 0.04).

To compare the Pow of industrial designers we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors task, hemisphere and electrode. From the analysis of the 18 participants we found a significant main effect of: task, F(4, 68) = 5.95, p < 0.001, η2 partial = .26; hemisphere, F(1, 17) = 43.25, p < 0.001, η2 partial = 0.72; and electrode, F(6, 102) = 4.51, p < 0.001, η2 partial = 0.21. The pairwise comparisons revealed that Task 4 differs significantly from Task 1 (p < 0.001). In addiction the pairwise comparisons revealed that some electrodes were significant between Task 1 and Task 4, namely, in the left hemisphere electrodes AF3 (p = 0.03), F3 (p = 0.03), P7 (p < 0.001) and O1 (p < 0.001) and in the right hemisphere electrodes FC6 (p = 0.03), P8 (p = 0.01) and O2 (p < 0.001).

To compare the Pow of industrial designers and mechanical engineers, we performed an analysis by running a 2 × 5 × 2 × 7 mixed repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the between-subjects factor domain and the within-subject factors task, hemisphere and electrode. From the analysis of the 36 participants (18 industrial designers and 18 mechanical engineers) we found a significant main effect of: task, F GG(3.03, 102.87) = 5.50, p < 0.01,η2 partial = 0.14 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.76); hemisphere, F(1, 136) = 59.05, p < 0.001,η2 partial = 0.64 and for electrode, F GG(3.80, 129.35) = 6.49, p < 0.001,η2 partial = 0.16 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.63). Moreover, the ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect between the factors: domain and hemisphere, F(1, 136) = 5.92, p = 0.02,η2 partial = 0.15; and hemisphere and electrode, F GG(3.34, 113.56) = 4.36, p < 0.01,η2 partial = 0.11 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.56). In addition, we conducted pairwise comparisons to check for differences between industrial designers and mechanical engineers comparing the seven electrodes per hemisphere and task. Below we report on significant (p ≤ 0.05) comparisons. The following comparisons were significant in the left hemisphere for Task 1, electrodes P7(p = .03) and in the right hemisphere for Task 3, electrode FC6 (p = 0.03).

Appendix C

To compare the Pow scores for the deciles we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere, electrode and decile, per domain. From the analysis of the 18 mechanical engineers we found a significant main effect of: task, F(4, 68) = 2.95, p = 0.03, η 2partial = 0.15; hemisphere, F(1, 17) = 48.64, p < 0.001, η2partial = 0.74; electrode, F(6, 102) = 2.89, p = 0.01, η 2partial = 0.15) and decile FGG(4.29, 72.94) = 3.09, p = 0.02, η 2partial = 0.15 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.48). Significant interaction effects were found between the factors: hemisphere and electrode, FGG(6, 102) = 6.59, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.27); task and decile, F(36, 612) = 1.65, p = 0.01, η 2partial = 0.09; electrode and decile, F(54, 918) = 1.58, p < 0.01, η 2partial = 0.09. In Table C1 we report on significant (p ≤ .05) pairwise comparisons found between Task 1 and Task 4 (open free hand sketching design) across the deciles.

Table C1. Pow significant differences for electrodes between Tasks 1 and 4 per decile of mechanical engineers (p value)

To compare the Pow scores for the deciles we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere, electrode and decile, per domain. From the analysis of the 18 industrial designers we found a significant main effect of: task, FGG(2.56, 43.49) = 8.67, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.34 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.64); hemisphere, F(1, 17) = 88.77, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.84; and electrode, FGG(3.86, 65.53) = 3.98, p = 0.001, η 2partial = 0.19 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.64). A marginally significant main effect was found for decile, F(9, 153) = 1.83, p = 0.07, η 2partial = 0.10. Significant interaction effects were found between the factors: task and hemisphere, FGG(2.12, 35.98) = 5.09, p = 0.01, η 2partial = 0.23) (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.53); task and electrode, F(24, 408) = 3.44, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.17; task and decile, F(36, 612) = 2.31, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.12); and hemisphere and electrode, F(6, 102) = 3.26, p < 0.01, η 2partial = 0.16. In Table C2, we report on significant (p ≤ 0.05) pairwise comparisons found between Task 1 (problem-solving) and Task 4 (open free hand sketching design) across the deciles.

Table C2. Pow significant differences for electrodes between Tasks 1 and 3 and Tasks 1 and 4 per decile of industrial designers (p value).

Appendix D

To compare the Pow scores for the deciles of industrial designers and mechanical engineers, we performed an analysis by running a 5 × 2 × 7 × 10 repeated-measurement ANOVA, with the between-subjects factor domain and the within-subject factors of task, hemisphere, electrode and decile. From the analysis of the 36 participants (18 industrial designers and 18 mechanical engineers) we found a significant main effect of: task, FGG(3.01, 102.41) = 7.07, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.17 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.75); hemisphere, F(1, 34) = 134.64, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.80; electrode, FGG(4.53, 153.88) = 6.06, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.15 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.75); and decile FGG(6.29, 213.79) = 2.91, p < 0.01, η 2partial = 0.08 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.70). No significant main effect was found for domain, F(1, 34) = 1.16, p = 0.29,η2 partial = 0.03. Moreover, the ANOVA revealed a significant interaction effect between the factors: task and domain, F(4, 136) = 4.39, p < 0.01, η 2partial = 0.11; task and electrode, FGG(9.02, 306.80) = 2.40, p = 0.01, η 2partial = 0.07 (corrected for Greenhouse–Geisser estimates of sphericity, ε = 0.38); task and decile, F(36, 1224) = 2.33, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.06; and hemisphere and electrode, F(6, 204) = 8.62, p < 0.001, η 2partial = 0.20. No other significant main effect nor interaction effect between the factors was found. There was no adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Table D1. Transformed power (Pow) significant differences for electrodes between Tasks 1 and 4 per decile (p value) for the 36 participants.

References

Alexiou, K., Zamenopoulos, T., Johnson, J. & Gilbert, S.J. 2009. Exploring the neurological basis of design cognition using brain imaging: some preliminary results. Design Studies 30 (6), 623647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badcock, N., Preece, K., de Wit, B., Glenn, K., Fieder, N., Thie, J. & McArthur, G. 2015. Validation of the Emotiv EPOC EEG system for research quality auditory event-related potentials in children. PeerJ. doi:10.7717/peerj.907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badcock, N., Mousikou, P., Mahajan, Y., de Lissa, P., Thie, J. & McArthur, G. 2013. Validation of the Emotiv EPOC® EEG gaming system for measuring research quality auditory ERPs. PeerJ. doi:10.7717/peerj.38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bashivan, P., Rish, I. & Heisig, S. 2016. Mental State Recognition via Wearable EEG. Proceedings of 5th NIPS workshop on Machine Learning and Interpretation in Neuroimaging.Google Scholar
Benedek, M., Christensen, A. P., Fink, A. & Beaty, R. E. 2019. Creativity assessment in neuroscience research. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 13 (2), 218226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermudez, J., Krizaj, D., Lipschitz, C., Elliot Bueler, C., Rogowska, J. & Yurgelun, D. 2017. Externally-induced meditative states: an exploratory fMRI study of architects’ responses to contemplative architecture. Frontiers of Architectural Research 6 (2), 123136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilda, Z., Gero, J. S. & Purcell, T. 2006. To sketch or not to sketch? That is the question. Design Studies 27 (5), 587613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borga, M. & Knutsson, H. 2001. A canonical correlation approach to blind source separation. Report LiU-IMT-EX-0062 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Linkoping University.Google Scholar
Borgianni, Y. & Maccioni, L. 2020. Review of the use of neurophysiological and biometric measures in experimental design research. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing 34 (2), 248285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brodmann, K. 1909. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde. Leipzig, Johann Ambrosius Barth.Google Scholar
Cross, N. 2006. Designerly Ways of Knowing. Springer.Google Scholar
Crozier, S., Sirigu, A., Lehéricy, S., van de Moortele, P., Pillon, B., Grafman, J., Agid, Y., Dubois, B. & LeBihan, D. 1999. Distinct prefrontal activations in processing sequence at the sentence and script level: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 37 (13), 14691476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crutcher, R. J. 1994. Telling what we know: The use of verbal report methodologies in psychological research, Psychological Science 5, 241244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darke, J. 1979. The primary generator and the design process. Design Studies 1 (1), 3644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Clercq, W., Vergult, A., Vanrumste, B., Van Paesschen, W. & Van Huffel, S. 2006. Canonical correlation analysis applied to remove muscle artifacts from the electroencephalogram. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 53, 25832587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickter, C. & Kieffaber, P. 2014. EEG Methods for the Psychological Sciences. Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorst, K. & Cross, N. 2001. Creativity in the design process: Co-evolution of problem-solution. Design Studies 22, 425437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorst, K. 2011. The core of ‘design thinking’ and its application. Design Studies 32 (6), 521553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorst, K. 2015. Frame innovation. Create New Thinking by Design. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellamil, M., Dobson, C., Beeman, M. & Christoff, K. 2011. Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. Neuroimage 59, 17831794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. & Simon, H. 1993. Protocol Analysis; Verbal Reports as Data. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincham, J., Carter, C., van Veen, V., Stenger, V. & Anderson, J. 2002. Neural mechanisms of planning: a computational analysis using event-related fMRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99 (5), 33463351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glasser, M., Coalson, T., Robinson, E., Hacker, C., Harwell, J., Yacoub, E., Ugurbil, K., Andersson, J., Beckmann, C., Jenkinson, M., Smith, S. & Van Essen, D. 2016. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536 (7615), 171178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerlach, C., Law, I., Gade, A. & Paulson, O. B. 2000. Categorization and category effects in normal object recognition: a PET study. Neuropsychologia 38 (13), 16931703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gero, J. S. 1990. Design prototypes: a knowledge representation schema for design. AI Magazine, 11 (4), 2636.Google Scholar
Gero, J. S. & Kannengiesser, U. 2004. The situated function-behavior-structure framework. Design Studies 25 (4), 373391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gero, J. S. & Mc Neil, T. 1998. An approach to the analysis of design protocols. Design Studies 19 (1), 2161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goel, V. & Pirolli, P. 1989. Motivating the notion of generic design within information processing theory: the design problem space. AI Magazine 10 (1), 1836.Google Scholar
Goel, V. & Pirolli, P. 1992. The structure of design problem spaces. Cognitive Science 16, 395429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S. & Houle, S. 1998. Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 3, 293302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S. & Houle, S. 1997. The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. Neuroreport 8 (5), 13051310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Göker, M. 1997. The effects of experience during design problem solving. Design Studies 18, 405426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. 2014. Linkography: Unfolding the Design Process. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. 1994. On visual design thinking: the vis kids of architecture. Design Studies 15 (2), 158174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K., Moss, J. & Cagan, J. 2017. Inside the mind: Using neuroimaging to understand moral product preference judgments involving sustainability. ASME Journal of Mechanical Design 139 (4), 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K., Moss, J. & Cagan, J. 2019. A neuroimaging investigation of design ideation with and without inspirational stimuli – understanding the meaning of near and far stimuli. Design Studies 60, 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, G., Farias, D., Davis, C., Buonocore, M. 2007. Comparison of the neural basis for imagined writing and drawing. Human Brain Mapping 28 (5), 450459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hashemi, A. J. Pino, L., Moffat, G., Mathewson, K., Aimone, C., Bennett, P., Schmidt, L. & Sekuler, A. 2016. Characterizing population EEG dynamics throughout adulthood. eNeuro 3 (6): ENEURO.027516. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0275-16.2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatchuel, A. & Weil, B. 2009. CK design theory: an advanced formulation. Research in Engineering Design 19 (4), 181192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, L., Duffy, A., McTeague, C., Pidgeon, L., Vuletic, T. & Grealy, M. 2017. A systematic review of protocol studies on conceptual design cognition: design as search and exploration. Design Science 3 (e10), 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, L., Duffy, A., Gilbert, S., Lyall, L., Campbell, G., Coyle, D. & Grealy, M. 2020. The neural correlates of ideation in product design engineering practitioners. Design Science 5 (e29), 123.Google Scholar
Hinterberger, T., Zlabinger, M., Blaser, K. 2014. Neurophysiological correlates of various mental perspectives. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, H. 2012. Understanding Senior Design Students' Product Conceptual Design Activities, PhD Thesis, National University of Singapore, Singapore.Google Scholar
Jiang, H., Gero, J. S., Yen, C. 2014. Exploring designing styles using Problem-Solution indexes. In Design Computing and Cognition'12 (ed. Gero, J. S.), pp. 85101. Springer.Google Scholar
Jones, J. C. & Thornley, D. G. (eds). 1963. Conference on Design Methods. Oxford UK. Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Kan, J. W. T. & Gero, J. S. 2017. Quantitative Methods for Studying Design Protocols. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavakli, M. & Gero, J. S. 2001. Sketching as mental imagery processing. Design Studies 22 (4), 347364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krigolson, O., Williams, C., Norton, A., Hassall, C., Colino, F., 2017. Choosing MUSE: Validation of a low-cost, portable EEG system for ERP research. Frontiers in Neuroscience 11. doi:10.3389/fnins.2017.00109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruger, C. & Cross, N. 2006. Solution driven versus problem driven design: strategies and outcomes. Design Studies 27 (5), 527548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kübler, A., Dixon, V. & Garavan, H. 2006. Automaticity and reestablishment of executive control-an fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 (8), 13311342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le, T., Pardo, P. & Hu, X. 1998. 4 T-fMRI study of nonspatial shifting of selective attention: cerebellar and parietal contributions. The American Physiological Society 79 (3), 15351548.Google ScholarPubMed
Liang, C., Chang, C. & Liu, Y. 2018. Comparison of the cerebral activities exhibited by expert and novice visual communication designers during idea incubation. International Journal of Design Creativity and Innovation 7 (4), 213236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liang, C., Lin, C., Yao, C., Chang, W., Liu, Y. & Chen, S. 2017. Visual attention and association: an electroencephalography study in expert designers. Design Studies 48, 7695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, L. T., Zeng, Y. & Ben Hamza, A. 2016. Identification of relationships between eectroencephalography (EEG) bands and design activities. In ASME 2016 International Design Engineering Technical Conferences and Computers and Information in Engineering Conference. Volume 7: 28th International Conference on Design Theory and Methodology. Charlotte, NC, August 21–24.Google Scholar
Liu, L., Li, Y., Xiong, Y., Cao, J. & Yuan, P. 2018. An EEG study of the relationship between design problem statements and cognitive behaviors during conceptual design. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing 32 351362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mannan, M., Kamran, M. & Jeong, M. 2018. Identification and removal of physiological artifacts from electroencephalogram signals: a review. IEEE Access 6, 3063030652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martindale, C. & Hines, D. 1975. Creativity and cortical activation during creative, intellectual and EEG feedback tasks. Biological Psychology 3, 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, R., Zhu, H., Schultz, R., Quackenbush, G., Royal, J., Skudlarski, P. & Peterson, B. 2006. A developmental fMRI study of self-regulatory control. Human Brain Mapping 27 (11), 848863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagornova, V. 2007. Changes in the EEG power during tests for nonverbal (figurative) creativity. Human Physiology 33 (3), 277284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, P., Nguyen, T. & Zeng, Y. 2017. Empirical approaches to quantifying effort, fatigue and concentration in the conceptual design process: an EEG study. Research in Engineering Design 29, 393409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, T. A. & Zeng, Y. 2010. Analysis of design activities using EEG signals. In 22nd International Conference on Design Theory and Methodology; Volume 5: Special Conference on Mechanical Vibration and Noise, pp. 277–286.Google Scholar
Park, C. & Alderman, J. 2018. Designing Across Senses: A Multimodal Approach to Product Design. O'Reilly Media.Google Scholar
Pekkola, J., Ojanen, V., Autti, T., Jääskeläinen, I.P., Möttönen, R., Tarkiainen, A. & Sams, M. 2005. Primary auditory cortex activation by visual speech: an fMRI study at 3 T. Neuroreport 16 (2), 125128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfurtscheller, G. & Lopes da Silva, F. 1999. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clinical Neurophysiology 110, 18421857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pidgeon, L., Grealy, M., Duffy, A., Hay, L., McTeague, C., Vuletic, T., Coyle, D. & Gilbert, S. 2016. Functional neuroimaging of visual creativity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain and Behavior 6 (10), 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platel, H., Price, C., Baron, J., Wise, R., Lambert, J., Frackowiak, R., Lechevalier, B. & Eustache, F. 1997. The structural components of music perception. A functional anatomical study. Brain 120 (2), 229243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purcell, T., Williams, P., Gero, J. S. & Colbron, B. 1993. Fixation effects: do they exist in design problem solving? Environment and Planning B 20, 333345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapp, A.M., Leube, D.T., Erb, M., Grodd, W. & Kircher, T.T. 2004. Neural correlates of metaphor processing. Cognitive Brain Research 20 (3), 395402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, H., Costello, P., Sponheim, S., Lee, J. & Pardo, J. 2004. Functional neuroanatomy of the human near/far response to blur cues: eye-lens accommodation/vergence to point targets varying in depth. European Journal of Neuroscience 20 (10), 27222732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D. & Fazio, F. 1996. Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution. Experimental Brain Research 111 (2), 246252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R., Owen, A., Middleton, H., Williams, E., Pickard, J., Sahakian, B. & Robbins, T. 1999. Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 19 (20), 90299038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rominger, C., Papousek, I., Perchtold, C., Weber, B., Weiss, E. & Fink, F. 2018. The creative brain in the figural domain: distinct patterns of EEG alpha power during idea generation and idea elaboration. Neuropsychologia 118, 1319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sawyer, K. 2011. The cognitive neuroscience of creativity: a critical review. Creativity Research Journal, 23 (2), 137154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwab, D., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. & Fink, A. 2014. The time-course of EEG alpha power changes in creative ideation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scrivener, S., Ball, L. & Tseng, S. 2000. Uncertainty and sketching behaviour. Design Studies 21 (5), 465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shealy, T. & Gero, J. S. 2019. The neurocognition of three engineering concept generation techniques. In Proceedings of the 22nd International Conference on Engineering Design (ICED19), Delft, The Netherlands, 5–8 August 2019. doi:10.1017/dsi.2019.189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shealy, T., Hu, M. & Gero, J. S. 2018. Neuro-cognitive differences between brainstorming, morphological analysis and TRIZ, In ASME IDETC. Quebec, Canada. August 26–29.Google Scholar
Shibata, M., Abe, J., Terao, A. & Miyamoto, T. 2007. Neural mechanisms involved in the comprehension of metaphoric and literal sentences: an fMRI study. Brain Research 1166, 92102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slotnick, S. & Moo, L. 2006. Prefrontal cortex hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate visual spatial memory. Neuropsychologia 44 (9), 15601568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vecchiato, G., Jelic, A., Gaetano, T., Maglione, A., Matteis, F. & Babiloni, F. 2015. Neurophysiological correlates of embodiment and motivational factors during the perception of virtual architectural. Cognitive Processing 16 (1), 425429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vieira, S. 2012. Ambiguity, risk and change in designing: a micro-level description for a property-based approach. In Proceedings of the 2nd Inter. Workshop on Modelling and Management Engineering Processes, pp. 1528. Cambridge Engineering Design Center.Google Scholar
Vieira, S., Gero, J. S., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Fernandes, C. & Fernandes, A. 2019a. Comparing the design neurocognition of mechanical engineers and architects: a study of the effect of designer's domain. In Proceedings of the 22nd International Conference on Engineering Design (ICED19), Delft, The Netherlands, 5–8 August 2019. doi:10.1017/dsi.2019.191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vieira, S., Gero, J. S., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Fernandes, C. & Fernandes, A. 2019b. Understanding the design neurocognition of mechanical engineers when designing and problem-solving, ASME IDETC, paper IDETC2019–97838.Google Scholar
Vieira, S., Gero, J. S., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V. & Fernandes, A. 2020a. The neurophysiological activations of novice and experienced professionals when designing and problem-solving. In Design 2020, October 2020, Croatia.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vieira, S., Gero, J. S., Delmoral, J. & Fernandes, A. 2020b. Brain activity in constrained and open design spaces: an EEG study. In International Conference on Design Creativity, August 2020, Finland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, W. 2009. Design: One, but in different forms. Design Studies 30 (3), 187223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vorobyev, V., Alho, K., Medvedev, S., Pakhomov, S., Roudas, M., Rutkovskaya, J., Tervaniemi, M., Van Zuijen, T. & Näätänen, R. 2004. Linguistic processing in visual and modality-nonspecific brain areas: PET recordings during selective attention. Cognitive Brain Research 20, 309322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vos, D., Riès, S., Vanderperren, K., Vanrumste, B., Alario, F., Huffel, V. & Burle, B. 2010. Removal of muscle artifacts from EEG recordings of spoken language production. Neuroinform 8, 135150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waberski, T. Gobbele, R., Lamberty, K., Buchner, H., Marshall, J. & Fink, G. 2008. Timing of visuo-spatial information processing: Electrical source imaging related to line bisection judgements. Neuropsychologia 46, 12011210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Description of the problem-solving, basic design and open design tasks.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Depiction of the problem-solving Task 1, layout design Task 2, open layout design Task 3 and open free hand sketching design Task 4.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Schematic sequence of the tasks’ procedure given to the participants.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Emotiv Epoc+ electrodes highlighted in grey against the 10–10 I.S. layout and experiment setup using the headset.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Screen capture depicting audio, video and screen captures streaming in Panopto.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Radar plot of task-related power (TRP) for the 14 electrodes by taking the problem-solving Task 1 as the reference period for Task 4 for the industrial designers and mechanical engineers. The electrode labels are distributed such that all the electrodes on the left hemisphere are on the left and all those on the right hemisphere are on the right. Their labelling uses the standard 10–10 I.S.

Figure 6

Figure 6. Transformed power (Pow) of the two tasks for each domain. The solid circles indicate channels where there are significant differences between the tasks within each domain.

Figure 7

Figure 7. Transformed power (Pow) of the two tasks per domain. The solid circles indicate channels and task where there are significant differences across domains.

Figure 8

Table 2. Significant main effects from the ANOVA for each domain

Figure 9

Table 3. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 36 participants

Figure 10

Table 4. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 18 mechanical engineers

Figure 11

Figure 8. For mechanical engineers, channels with significant differences from Task 1 to Task 4 by decile, shown as solid circles.

Figure 12

Figure 9. Electrodes placement related to each cortex of the brain.

Figure 13

Table 5. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 18 industrial designers.

Figure 14

Figure 10. For industrial designers, channels with significant differences from Task 1 to Task 4 by decile, shown as solid circles.

Figure 15

Table 6. Significant effects from the ANOVA based on the 36 participants

Figure 16

Figure 11. Approximate Brodmann areas (numbered) of the 10–10 I.S. placement system.

Figure 17

Table C1. Pow significant differences for electrodes between Tasks 1 and 4 per decile of mechanical engineers (p value)

Figure 18

Table C2. Pow significant differences for electrodes between Tasks 1 and 3 and Tasks 1 and 4 per decile of industrial designers (p value).

Figure 19

Table D1. Transformed power (Pow) significant differences for electrodes between Tasks 1 and 4 per decile (p value) for the 36 participants.