Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-06T02:20:47.546Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cortical volume alterations in the limbic network in adolescents with high reactive aggression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2024

Johannah Bashford-Largo*
Affiliation:
Child and Family Translational Research Center, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Center for Brain, Biology and Behavior, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA
R. James R. Blair
Affiliation:
Child and Adolescent Mental Health Centre, Mental Health Services, Capital Region of Denmark, Copenhagen, Denmark
Karina S. Blair
Affiliation:
Child and Family Translational Research Center, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA
Matthew Dobbertin
Affiliation:
Child and Family Translational Research Center, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Inpatient Center, Boys Town National Research Hospital, Boys Town, NE, USA
Jaimie Elowsky
Affiliation:
Clinical Psychology Department, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA
Ahria Dominguez
Affiliation:
Clinical Health, Emotion, and Neuroscience (CHEN) Laboratory, Department of Neurological Sciences, College of Medicine, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
Melissa Hatch
Affiliation:
Mind and Brain Health Laboratories (MBHL), Department of Neurological Sciences, College of Medicine, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA
Sahil Bajaj
Affiliation:
Department of Cancer Systems Imaging, MD Anderson Center, University of Texas, Houston, TX, USA
*
Corresponding author: J. Bashford-Largo; Email: johannah.bashford-largo@boystown.org
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Previous studies show aggression-related structural alterations in frontal and limbic brain regions. Most studies have focused on overall aggression, instead of its subtypes, and on specific regions instead of networks. This study aims to identify both brain networks and regions that are associated with reactive and proactive subtypes of aggression. Structural MRI data were collected from 340 adolescents (125 F/215 M) with a mean age of 16.29 (SD = 1.20). Aggression symptomology was indexed via the Reactive Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ). Freesurfer was used to estimate Cortical Volume (CV) from seven networks and regions within specific networks associated with aggression. Two multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs) were conducted on groups for low versus higher reactive and proactive RPQ scores. Our reactive aggression MANCOVA showed a main effect in CV [F(14,321) = 1.935, p = 0.022,ηp2 = 0.078] across all the 7-Networks. Unpacking this main effect revealed significant volumetric differences in the right Limbic Network (LN) (p = 0.029) and the Temporal Pole (p = 0.011), where adolescents in the higher reactive aggression group showed higher cortical volumes. Such findings are consistent with region/voxel-specific analyses that have associated atypical structure within the LN and reactive aggression. Moreover, the temporal pole is highly interconnected with regions important in the regulation and initiation of reactive aggression.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© Boys Town National Research Hospital, 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

High levels of aggression can have significant costs to society and are one of the leading causes for youth seeking referrals to mental health (Magalotti et al., Reference Magalotti, Neudecker, Zaraa and McVoy2019). While aggression is not atypical during childhood development, maladaptive levels of aggression (i.e., overly frequent and intense) can lead to impaired social relationships, incarceration, and even death (Hendricks & Liu, Reference Hendricks and Liu2012). An increased risk for aggression is transdiagnostic with a variety of psychiatric diagnoses, including major depressive disorder (MDD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), conduct disorder (CD), and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) (Buchmann et al., Reference Buchmann, Hohmann, Brandeis, Banaschewski and Poustka2014; Liu & Cole, Reference Liu and Cole2021; Saylor & Amann, Reference Saylor and Amann2016). Given the poor prognosis for aggressive individuals, there is a considerable need to determine reliable trait variables that might aid in clinical decision-making.

Substantial neuroimaging work has pointed to an association between an increased risk for aggression and structural and functional disruptions within regions of the fronto-limbic-striatal systems (Blair, Reference Blair2016; Ducharme et al., Reference Ducharme, Hudziak, Botteron, Ganjavi, Lepage, Collins, Albaugh, Evans and Karama2011; Sukhodolsky et al., Reference Sukhodolsky, Ibrahim, Kalvin, Jordan, Eilbott and Hampson2021). However, it is important to note that acts of aggression are not homogenous and have different subtypes and etiologies. A commonly made distinction is drawn between reactive and proactive aggression. Reactive aggression is unplanned and made in response to threat or social provocation, whereas the less common proactive aggression is goal-oriented and is seen as more callous (Blair, Reference Blair2018; Blair et al., Reference Blair, Zhang, Bashford-Largo, Bajaj, Mathur, Ringle, Schwartz, Elowsky, Dobbertin, Blair and Tyler2021; Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1996).

There are a lack of studies distinguishing between these two subtypes of aggression, though there are indications that they can be differentiated at the neural level (Cima & Raine, Reference Cima and Raine2009; Naaijen et al., Reference Naaijen, Mulder, Ilbegi, de Bruijn, Kleine-Deters, Dietrich, Hoekstra, Marsman, Aggensteiner, Holz, Boettinger, Baumeister, Banaschewski, Saam, M E Schulze, Santosh, Sagar-Ouriaghli, Mastroianni, Castro Fornieles, Bargallo, Rosa, Arango, Penzol, Werhahn, Walitza, Brandeis, Glennon, Franke, Zwiers and Buitelaar2020). Most neuroimaging studies focus on frontal regions involved in response control and reinforcement-based decision-making as well as limbic structures, such as the amygdala and hippocampus. Overall, high aggression, especially reactive aggression, is associated with a decrease in activity in the ventral medial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and an increase in activity in the amygdala (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Zhang, Bashford-Largo, Bajaj, Mathur, Ringle, Schwartz, Elowsky, Dobbertin, Blair and Tyler2021; Choe et al., Reference Choe, Shaw and Forbes2015; Coccaro et al., Reference Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald and Phan2007; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Chan and Raine2008; Sukhodolsky et al., Reference Sukhodolsky, Ibrahim, Kalvin, Jordan, Eilbott and Hampson2021). Structural and functional connectivity between the amygdala and vmPFC and/or orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has also been considered to be important in regulating aggression (Sukhodolsky et al., Reference Sukhodolsky, Ibrahim, Kalvin, Jordan, Eilbott and Hampson2021; White et al., Reference White, VanTieghem, Brislin, Sypher, Sinclair, Pine, Hwang and Blair2016). However, one DTI study in a healthy sample showed no structural differences within OFC-amygdala connectivity between participants with high and low physical aggressiveness (Beyer et al., Reference Beyer, Münte, Wiechert, Heldmann, Krämer and Siegel2014).

There are few neuroimaging studies that have examined the differences in reactive and proactive aggression. Functional studies tend to agree on a few key findings regarding the two subtypes. The response to threat involves increased function within the amygdala, hypothalamus, and periaqueductal gray (Coker-Appiah et al., Reference Coker-Appiah, White, Clanton, Yang, Martin and Blair2013; Haller, Reference Haller2018). These regions are shown to be associated with reactive aggression, which is mediated by a threat response circuit involving these regions and the vmPFC (Blair, Reference Blair2016). Proactive aggression has been shown to be associated with not only the amygdala but also regions implicated in goal setting and reward, such as the dorsolateral PFC and striatum (Belfry & Kolla, Reference Belfry and Kolla2021; Blair, Reference Blair2016).

Of the very few structural studies that have looked at the subtypes, there have been mixed results. There have been reports that increased anterior cingulate cortex volume (Farah et al, Reference Farah, Ling, Raine, Yang and Schug2018) but decreased thickness (Romero-Martínez et al., Reference Romero-Martínez, Sarrate-Costa and Moya-Albiol2022; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Joshi, Jahanshad, Thompson and Baker2017) is selectively associated with proactive aggression. One study using youth with conduct disorder saw significant decreases in gyrification in the bilateral superior parietal cortex within individuals with high proactive aggression scores (Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Gao, Dong, Sun, Situ and Yao2022). Alternatively, though, there is at least one report that both proactive and reactive aggression were associated with increased right OFC volume and thickness of the left paracentral areas (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Joshi, Jahanshad, Thompson and Baker2017). Amygdala volumes have been reported to negatively correlate with proactive aggression (Naaijen et al., Reference Naaijen, Mulder, Ilbegi, de Bruijn, Kleine-Deters, Dietrich, Hoekstra, Marsman, Aggensteiner, Holz, Boettinger, Baumeister, Banaschewski, Saam, M E Schulze, Santosh, Sagar-Ouriaghli, Mastroianni, Castro Fornieles, Bargallo, Rosa, Arango, Penzol, Werhahn, Walitza, Brandeis, Glennon, Franke, Zwiers and Buitelaar2020) but positively with reactive aggression (Farah et al., Reference Farah, Ling, Raine, Yang and Schug2018). Reactive aggression has also been reported to negatively correlate with insula volume (Naaijen et al., Reference Naaijen, Mulder, Ilbegi, de Bruijn, Kleine-Deters, Dietrich, Hoekstra, Marsman, Aggensteiner, Holz, Boettinger, Baumeister, Banaschewski, Saam, M E Schulze, Santosh, Sagar-Ouriaghli, Mastroianni, Castro Fornieles, Bargallo, Rosa, Arango, Penzol, Werhahn, Walitza, Brandeis, Glennon, Franke, Zwiers and Buitelaar2020).

Few studies have specifically looked at networks implicated in aggression severity. Studies have seen that alterations in connectivity within and/or between the default-mode network (DMN) and other networks/regions were predictive of aggression (Dailey et al., Reference Dailey, Smith, Vanuk, Raikes and Killgore2018; Ibrahim et al., Reference Ibrahim, Noble, He, Lacadie, Crowley, McCarthy, Scheinost and Sukhodolsky2022; Weathersby et al., Reference Weathersby, King, Fox, Loret and Anderson2019). Other studies have seen disruptions in activity and connectivity within the DMN in individuals who are prone to aggression (Broulidakis et al., Reference Broulidakis, Fairchild, Sully, Blumensath, Darekar and Sonuga-Barke2016; Dalwani et al., Reference Dalwani, Tregellas, Andrews-Hanna, Mikulich-Gilbertson, Raymond, Banich, Crowley and Sakai2014; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Zhang, Zhou and Wang2022; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Liao, Song, Gao, Zhou, Tan, Liu, Tang, Chen, Chen and Zhan2013; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Yao, Fairchild, Cao, Zhang, Xiang, Zhang and Wang2016). Another network commonly seen in aggression literature is the limbic network (LN), where structural and connectivity alterations relative to typical developing participants have been reported in individuals presenting with higher aggression (Ducharme et al., Reference Ducharme, Hudziak, Botteron, Ganjavi, Lepage, Collins, Albaugh, Evans and Karama2011; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Joshi, Jahanshad, Thompson and Baker2017). The LN is comprised of the OFC and temporal pole (Yeo et al., Reference Thomas Yeo, Krienen, Sepulcre, Sabuncu, Lashkari, Hollinshead, Roffman, Smoller, Zöllei, Polimeni, Fischl, Liu and Buckner2011) and involved in important frontal-limbic connections commonly seen in those with aggression (Gan et al., Reference Gan, Preston-Campbell, Moeller, Steinberg, Lane, Maloney, Parvaz, Goldstein and Alia-Klein2016).

This present study will look at the differences in cortical volume (CV) between high and low aggression groups (both reactive and proactive aggression groups) within seven different networks: Visual Network, Somatomotor Network; Dorsal Attention Network, Ventral Attention Network, Limbic Network, Frontoparietal Network, and Default-Mode Network.

Previous literature has shown alterations in regions within the DMN and LN in individuals at increased risk for aggression (De Brito et al., Reference De Brito, Mechelli, Wilke, Laurens, Jones, Barker, Hodgins and Viding2009; Ducharme et al., Reference Ducharme, Hudziak, Botteron, Ganjavi, Lepage, Collins, Albaugh, Evans and Karama2011; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Joshi, Jahanshad, Thompson and Baker2017). Because of these previous findings, we adopted an exploratory approach and hypothesized that proactive and reactive aggression would be associated with alterations in CV within these regions in these networks (specifically orbitofrontal cortices and superior temporal gyri).

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from a residential care facility in the Midwest and from the surrounding community. Participants recruited from the residential facility had been referred for behavioral and mental health problems, whereas participants from the community were recruited through flyers or social media. Structural MRI data were collected from 340 adolescents (125 F/215 M) with a mean age of 16.29 (SD = 1.20, 14–18 years), and IQ of 103.91 (SD = 10.73).

Exclusion criteria included braces, claustrophobia, active substance dependence, pervasive developmental disorder, Tourette’s syndrome, lifetime history of psychosis, neurological disorder, head trauma, non-English speaking, and presence of active safety concerns. Clinical characterization was done through psychiatric interviews by licensed and board-certified child and adolescent psychiatrists with the participants and their parents to adhere closely to common clinical practice. All participants and their parents provided written informed assent/consent prior to enrollment. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Boys Town National Research Hospital (BTNRH).

Demographics characteristics

Group differences in sex, age, IQ, intracranial volume (ICV) (Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Ridgway, Bartlett, Henley, Lehmann, Hobbs, Clarkson, MacManus, Ourselin and Fox2010), and RPQ scores were examined via chi square and independent sample t-tests. These variables were used as covariates in the following analyses.

Data collection

Neuroanatomical data

High resolution structural MRI (T1-weighted) data were collected using a 3-Tesla Siemens MRI scanner located at BTNRH. Whole-brain anatomical data for each participant were acquired using a 3D magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo sequence, which consisted of 176 axial slices (slice thickness = 1 mm, voxel resolution = 0.9 × 0.9 × 1 mm3, repetition time = 2200ms; echo time = 2.48 ms; matrix size = 256 × 208; field of view (FOV) = 230 mm, and flip angle = 8o).

General intelligence (IQ)

The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence II (WASI-II) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2011) was used to estimate IQ in the domains of perceptual reasoning, verbal comprehension, and Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ). FSIQ scores have high reliability (α = 0.98) and strong correlations (r = 0.92) with scores on the full Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997, Reference Wechsler1999) and were used in the current context.

Reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire

The Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire (RPQ; Raine et al., Reference Raine, Dodge, Loeber, Gatzke‐Kopp, Lynam, Reynolds, Stouthamer‐Loeber and Liu2006) is a 23-item questionnaire which has shown to be a validated measure of both proactive aggression (11 items; α = 0.87) and reactive (12 items; α = 0.83) aggression in youth (Cima et al., Reference Cima, Raine, Meesters and Popma2013).

Image preprocessing

The “recon-all” pipeline from the FreeSurfer toolbox (Version 6.0; https:// surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) was used to process the anatomical brain images (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999; Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Sereno and Dale1999) and for estimating CV measures. Structural image processing included head motion-correction, brain extraction, automated transformation to the standard MNI template space, volumetric segmentation into cortical and sub-cortical matter, intensity correction, and parcellation of the cerebral cortex into gyral and sulcal matter (Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker, Buckner, Dale, Maguire, Hyman, Albert and Killiany2006). See (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999; Fischl, Reference Fischl2004; Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Sereno and Dale1999) for full details. Steps to ensure preprocessing accuracy included a careful visual inspection of raw structural images, skull-stripped brain volumes, and pial surfaces via FreeSurfer (Version 6.0; https:// surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu).

Data analysis

Behavioral analysis

A correlation between reactive and proactive aggression scores was conducted to look at a possible association between these subtypes of aggression.

Network volume analysis

FreeSurfer was used to parcellate the whole brain into seven networks using Yeo’s atlas (Yeo et al., Reference Thomas Yeo, Krienen, Sepulcre, Sabuncu, Lashkari, Hollinshead, Roffman, Smoller, Zöllei, Polimeni, Fischl, Liu and Buckner2011) (N1: Visual Network; N2: Somatomotor Network; N3: Dorsal Attention Network; N4: Ventral Attention Network; N5: Limbic Network; N6: Frontoparietal Network; and N7: Default-Mode Network). A network cortical volume analysis was conducted (sex, age, IQ, and intracranial volume [ICV] were used as covariates) using network-wise CV data to look at potential differences between high and low aggression groupings. Two multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs) were conducted on the seven bilateral networks (14 total) for each subscale RPQ score group (i.e., one examining groups with higher vs. lower reactive aggression and a second examining groups differing in proactive aggression levels).

Region-based analysis

Our steps for region-based analysis are as follows: 1. Identify any significant networks (as described above). 2. If any significant networks were discovered using the above analysis, we then extracted regions from the networks. 3. We then looked at associations of CV of the extracted regions and aggression groups by performing a MANCOVA– again with sex, age, IQ, and ICV as covariates.

Follow-up analyses

Potential confounds: impact of other major psychopathologies and prescribed medications

Several of our participants were diagnosed with different psychiatric disorders including Major Depressive Disorder (N = 45), Social Anxiety Disorder (N = 77), Generalized Anxiety Disorder (N = 86), Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (N = 38), Conduct Disorder (N = 145), Oppositional Defiant Disorder (N = 169), and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (N = 175). In addition, several of our youth were on psychiatric medications (N = 124) during the time of the study, including SSRIs, stimulants, and antipsychotics. Table 1 shows demographic characteristics of both comorbidities and medications. Given the potential confounds, the MANCOVA described above (individual networks) was repeated, first, with the inclusion of psychiatric diagnoses and, second, with the inclusion of prescribed medications as covariates.

Table 1. Demographics of sample

Key to table. ns: Non-Significant; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001; SD = Standard Deviation; IQ = Intelligent Quotient; ICV = Intercranial Volume; MDD = Major Depressive Disorder; SAD = Social Anxiety Disorder; GAD = Generalized Anxiety Disorder; PTSD = Post Traumatic Stress Disorder; CD = Conduct Disorder; ODD = Oppositional Defiant Disorder; ADHD = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; SSRIs=Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors. There was a 43.5% overlap between the two high aggression groupings.

Potential confounds

In order to control for the effects of proactive aggression when looking at reactive aggression and vice versa, a follow-up MANCOVA with the other aggression subtype as a covariate will be run if either of the groups showed significant network cortical volume differences. Another follow-up MANCOVA will be run after removing participants that had high scores in both reactive and proactive aggression as well.

Results

Bivariate analysis

Reactive aggression scores and proactive aggression scores from the RPQ were significantly correlated across the entire sample (r = 0.63, p < 0.001).

Demographics characteristics

Table 1 shows the total number of participants in each group. There was some overlap (n = 148, 43.5%) between groups (meaning these youth had high scores in both reactive and proactive aggression). There were no significant differences in sex (χ 2(1) = 0.016, p = 0.90; χ 2(1) = 0.642, p = 0.42) or age (t(338) = 0.302, p = 0.76; t (248.52) = 1.23, p = 0.22) between high/low reactive and high/low proactive aggression groups respectively. ICV was not significantly different between high/low reactive aggression groups, [t(338) = 1.452, p = 0.15], but was significantly different between high/low proactive aggression groups, [t(338) = 0.81, p = 0.04], where those with low proactive aggression had higher ICV. There were significant differences in IQ between high and low reactive [t(300.378) = 3.081, p = 0.002] and high and low proactive [t(247.64) = 2.36, p = 0.019] aggression groups, where lower aggression groups had higher IQ scores.

Group differences in CV

Our MANCOVAs showed a significant main effect across the 7 networks in CV for reactive aggression [F(14,321) = 1.935, p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.078; Wilk’s lambda = 0.922] but no significant effects for the MANCOVA on proactive aggression [F(14,321) = 0.666, p = 0.807, ηp2 = 0.028; Wilk’s lambda = 0.972]. This particularly reflected group differences driven by the right Limbic Network [F(1,334) = 4.802, p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.014], where adolescents in the higher reactive aggression group showed higher cortical volumes (Figures 1 and 2).

Follow-up exploratory analysis of the MANCOVA main effect

Our ROI-specific MANCOVA for the right Limbic Network showed significant group differences in ROI volume [F(2,333) = 3.471, p = 0.032,ηp2 = 0.020; Wilk’s lambda = 0.980]. Specifically, we saw significant difference in CV of the Temporal Pole [F(1,334) = 6.466, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.019], where adolescents in the higher reactive aggression group showed higher cortical volumes (Figure 3).

Follow-up to reactive aggression analysis

Diagnoses

Our follow-up MANCOVA analysis with the addition of seven psychiatric diagnoses (see Table 1 for full list) continued to show a significant equation that mirrored the results of the main analysis [F(14,314) = 2.145, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.087], still showing strongest significant differences in the right Limbic Network [F(1,327) = 7.116, p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.021].

Medication

Our follow-up MANCOVA analysis with the addition of the three medications (Antipsychotics, stimulants, and SSRIs) continued to show a significant equation that mirrored the results of the main analysis [F(14,318) = 2.023, p = 0.016, ηp2 = 0.082], again showing strongest significant differences in the right Limbic Network [F(1,331) = 5.126, p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.015].

Subtype

Our follow-up MANCOVA analysis with the addition of proactive aggression as a covariate in our regression analysis, continued to show a significant equation that mirrored the results of the main analysis [F(14, 320) = 1.805, p = 0.037, ηp2 = 0.073], again showing strongest significant differences in the right Limbic Network [F(1,333) = 5.557, p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.016]. When adding reactive aggression as a covariate to our MANCOVA looking at proactive aggression, we still saw a non-significant result (p = 0.879).

Removal of participants in both high score groups

Our next follow-up MANCOVA was the same as the main analysis (7 Network), however, with the removal of individuals that had both high reactive aggression scores and high proactive aggression scores (43.5% who had both). The removal of these participants made our results no longer significant within the reactive aggression groupings [F(14, 173) = 1.423, p = 0.147, ηp2 = 0.103].

Discussion

The goal of this study was to examine differences in cortical volumes (CV) in brain networks within high and low aggression groups (both reactive and proactive aggression). We found that CV of the right Limbic Network (LN) was significantly greater in those with higher vs. lower reactive aggression scores. In addition, region-specific analysis showed that within the right LN, CV of the temporal pole was significantly increased in the higher reactive aggression score group. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not see any significant differences in CV within networks in proactive aggression groups.

Previous work has typically taken a region/voxel-focused approach to analysis and not focused on network-level structural alterations in individuals prone to higher levels of aggression (Chester et al., Reference Chester, Lynam, Milich and DeWall2017; Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Gao, Dong, Sun, Situ and Yao2022; Naaijen et al., Reference Naaijen, Mulder, Ilbegi, de Bruijn, Kleine-Deters, Dietrich, Hoekstra, Marsman, Aggensteiner, Holz, Boettinger, Baumeister, Banaschewski, Saam, M E Schulze, Santosh, Sagar-Ouriaghli, Mastroianni, Castro Fornieles, Bargallo, Rosa, Arango, Penzol, Werhahn, Walitza, Brandeis, Glennon, Franke, Zwiers and Buitelaar2020). However, and consistent with the current findings, there are several previous results which indicate atypical LN function/structure relating to increased aggression risk. Indeed, accounts of reactive aggression have stressed the importance of dysfunction in components of this system for some time (Bertsch et al., Reference Bertsch, Florange and Herpertz2020; Blair, Reference Blair2004). Individuals at increased risk of reactive aggression frequently show poor emotion regulation (Nikolic et al., Reference Nikolic, Pezzoli, Jaworska and Seto2022). Notably, the role of OFC in emotion regulation has long been recognized (Blair, Reference Blair2004; Christiansen et al., Reference Christiansen, Hirsch, Albrecht and Chavanon2019; Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Wang, Liu, Xi, Li, Zhang, Li, Yin, Tan, Lu and Li2018). Empirical work has associated atypical connectivity between the amygdala/temporal cortex and OFC with an increased risk for aggression, particularly reactive aggression (Sukhodolsky et al., Reference Sukhodolsky, Ibrahim, Kalvin, Jordan, Eilbott and Hampson2021; White et al., Reference White, VanTieghem, Brislin, Sypher, Sinclair, Pine, Hwang and Blair2016). These regions have also been implicated in mediating reactive aggression (Coccaro et al., Reference Coccaro, McCloskey, Fitzgerald and Phan2007; Gan et al., Reference Gan, Preston-Campbell, Moeller, Steinberg, Lane, Maloney, Parvaz, Goldstein and Alia-Klein2016). Moreover, atypical structure of OFC and amygdala volumes have been associated with an increased risk of reactive aggression (Farah et al., Reference Farah, Ling, Raine, Yang and Schug2018; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Joshi, Jahanshad, Thompson and Baker2017), and lesions within the LN can lead to increased impulsivity and aggression (Berlin et al., Reference Berlin, Rolls and Kischka2004; Kuniishi et al., Reference Kuniishi, Ichisaka, Matsuda, Futora, Harada and Hata2016; Potegal, Reference Potegal2012; Shiba et al., Reference Shiba, Kim, Santangelo and Roberts2015).

It should be noted that within the LN, it was particularly atypical temporal pole structure that was associated with increased risk for reactive aggression. The temporal pole is a dominant hub in the processing of semantic and socioemotional information (Guo et al., Reference Guo, Hu, Jiang, Zheng, Mo, Cao, Zhu and Zhong2022; Pehrs et al., Reference Pehrs, Zaki, Schlochtermeier, Jacobs, Kuchinke and Koelsch2017) and is considerably interconnected with other regions very important in the regulation and initiation of reactive aggression: the amygdala (Li et al., Reference Li, Cui, Zhu, Kong, Guo, Zhu, Hu, Zhang, Li, Li, Jiang, Meyers, Li, Wang, Yang and Li2016), as well as OFC (Novitskaya et al., Reference Novitskaya, Dümpelmann, Vlachos, Reinacher and Schulze-Bonhage2020; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Plotzker and Ezzyat2007). It could be speculated that larger volumes are associated with strong emotional reactions and disruptions in the perception of provocations, leading to outbursts or disrupted emotional processing. Previous work has tended to report reductions in thickness and volume in the temporal pole (rather than the increased volume seen here) in groups of aggressive individuals (Cope et al., Reference Cope, Ermer, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2014; Ermer et al., Reference Ermer, Cope, Nyalakanti, Calhoun and Kiehl2012; Gregory et al., Reference Gregory, ffytche, Simmons, Kumari, Howard, Hodgins and Blackwood2012; Ly et al., Reference Ly, Motzkin, Philippi, Kirk, Newman, Kiehl and Koenigs2012). However, most of that work has been conducted in individuals with psychopathy which was not a predominant feature of the sample with higher reactive aggression studied here. It is notable that one previous finding, in a slightly more similar study, also observed increased temporal pole volumes in those exhibiting higher reactive aggression (Breitschuh et al., Reference Breitschuh, Schöne, Tozzi, Kaufmann, Strumpf, Fenker, Frodl, Bogerts and Schiltz2018).

In contrast to predictions, there was no association of CV within the DMN and either reactive or proactive aggression. Previous work has reported alterations in connectivity within and/or between the DMN and other networks/regions were predictive of aggression (Dailey et al., Reference Dailey, Smith, Vanuk, Raikes and Killgore2018; Ibrahim et al., Reference Ibrahim, Noble, He, Lacadie, Crowley, McCarthy, Scheinost and Sukhodolsky2022; Weathersby et al., Reference Weathersby, King, Fox, Loret and Anderson2019), disruptions in activity and connectivity within the DMN in individuals who are prone to aggression (Broulidakis et al., Reference Broulidakis, Fairchild, Sully, Blumensath, Darekar and Sonuga-Barke2016; Dalwani et al., Reference Dalwani, Tregellas, Andrews-Hanna, Mikulich-Gilbertson, Raymond, Banich, Crowley and Sakai2014; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Zhang, Zhou and Wang2022; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Liao, Song, Gao, Zhou, Tan, Liu, Tang, Chen, Chen and Zhan2013; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Yao, Fairchild, Cao, Zhang, Xiang, Zhang and Wang2016) and structural alterations within the DMN in individuals at increased risk for aggression (De Brito et al., Reference De Brito, Mechelli, Wilke, Laurens, Jones, Barker, Hodgins and Viding2009; Ducharme et al., Reference Ducharme, Hudziak, Botteron, Ganjavi, Lepage, Collins, Albaugh, Evans and Karama2011; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Joshi, Jahanshad, Thompson and Baker2017). The reason for the absence of comparable findings in the current cohort are unclear but it is possible that it represents a Type II error.

Also, in contrast to predictions, we found no networks showing atypical structure in the group of participants showing higher levels of proactive aggression. Relatively little previous work has focused on individuals showing higher levels of proactive aggression as opposed to focusing on samples who are at increased for proactive aggression (but also reactive aggression), such as individuals with psychopathy (Blair, Reference Blair2010; Garofalo et al., Reference Garofalo, Neumann and Velotti2021; Hofhansel et al., Reference Hofhansel, Weidler, Votinov, Clemens, Raine and Habel2020). It has been argued that proactive aggression is a chosen behavior reflecting the individual’s decision-making (Blair, Reference Blair2019). This may result in socially undesirable choices because of the economic realities of the individual or because the individual’s representations of potential costs (e.g., the distress of others) is disrupted (Blair, Reference Blair2019). As such, it is possible that structural differences may be less commonly seen in those at increased risk for proactive rather than reactive aggression.

Despite the strengths of our study, including large sample size and diverse range of clinical symptomatology, there were some caveats. First, 72% of the adolescents in our sample had at least one psychiatric diagnosis, and it could be argued that we are seeing results from specific disorders instead of aggression severity. Our follow-up analysis with diagnoses as covariates showed results approximate to the main analysis, with the LN still showing highest significance. Second, multiple adolescents were on medications including SSRIs, stimulants, and antipsychotics. Our follow-up analysis with the inclusion of the medications as covariates also revealed LN showing significant differences between the two groups. Third, our reactive aggression group was not matched in IQ scores; those in the lower reactive aggression group had higher IQ scores than those with higher reactive aggression. However, we did use IQ as a covariate in our analysis (in addition to age, sex, and ICV). As such, it is unlikely that our findings can be considered to reflect group differences in IQ. Fourth, multiple participants were in both the high proactive aggression group as well as the high reactive aggression group. A post hoc analysis was ran removing individuals in both groups, which made our results no longer significant (see Table 1 for breakdown of groups). Fifth, the group-based analysis approach chosen here due to test-retest reliability concerns runs the risk of data loss by dichotomizing the variable of interest (Dawson & Weiss, Reference Dawson and Weiss2012) Our goal would be to extend the current study in future work where the test-retest reliability of core measures is known and satisfactory.

In conclusion, our study revealed that CV of the right LN, particularly the temporal pole region within the LN, was significantly greater in those with higher reactive aggression scores. These findings broaden our knowledge of the neurobiology of reactive aggression and can inform future imaging work.

Figure 1. Limbic network and temporal pole locations: location of the limbic network (via Yeo’s 7 network parcellation) and temporal pole (via clustering).

Figure 2. Limbic network: identified cortical network, limbic network (a), and identified region of significance, temporal pole (b). Cortical volume of the right limbic network was significantly different between those with high and low reactive aggression (high reactive aggression > low reactive aggression). * = p < 0.05.

Figure 3. Temporal pole. Within the right limbic network, cortical volume of the temporal pole was also significantly different between those with high and low reactive aggression (high reactive aggression > low reactive aggression) * = p < 0.05.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424000750.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. The data are not publicly available due to IRB restrictions.

Funding statement

This research was in part supported by the National Institute of Mental Health under award number K22-MH109558 (RJRB). We would like to thank Ron Copsey, Kim VanHorn, Michael Wright, Mark Timm, Michelle Kelly, and Sarah Johnson for their contributions to data collection.

Competing interests

None.

References

Barnes, J., Ridgway, G. R., Bartlett, J., Henley, S. M. D., Lehmann, M., Hobbs, N., Clarkson, M. J., MacManus, D. G., Ourselin, S., & Fox, N. C. (2010). Head size, age and gender adjustment in MRI studies: A necessary nuisance? NeuroImage, 53(4), 12441255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belfry, K. D., & Kolla, N. J. (2021). Cold-blooded and on purpose: A review of the biology of proactive aggression. Brain Sciences, 11(11), 1412. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111412 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, H. A., Rolls, E. T., & Kischka, U. (2004). Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 127(Pt 5), 11081126. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh135 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertsch, K., Florange, J., & Herpertz, S. C. (2020). Understanding brain mechanisms of reactive aggression. Current Psychiatry Reports, 22(12), 81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-01208-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beyer, F., Münte, T. F., Wiechert, J., Heldmann, M., Krämer, U. M., & Siegel, A. (2014). Trait aggressiveness is not related to structural connectivity between orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala. PLOS ONE, 9(6), e101105. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 198208. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00276-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2010). Neuroimaging of psychopathy and antisocial behavior: A targeted review. Current Psychiatry Reports, 12(1), 7682. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-009-0086-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2018). Traits of empathy and anger: Implications for psychopathy and other disorders associated with aggression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 373(1744), 20170155. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0155 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J., Zhang, R u, Bashford-Largo, J., Bajaj, S., Mathur, A., Ringle, J., Schwartz, A., Elowsky, J., Dobbertin, M., Blair, K. S., & Tyler, P. M. (2021). Reduced neural responsiveness to looming stimuli is associated with increased aggression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, nsab058(10), 10911099. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab058 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (2016). The neurobiology of impulsive aggression. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26(1), 49. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2015.0088 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2019). Dysfunctional neurocognition in individuals with clinically significant psychopathic traits. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 21(3), 291299. https://doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2019.21.3/rblair CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breitschuh, S., Schöne, M., Tozzi, L., Kaufmann, Jörn, Strumpf, H., Fenker, D., Frodl, T., Bogerts, B., & Schiltz, K. (2018). Aggressiveness of martial artists correlates with reduced temporal pole grey matter concentration. Psychiatry Research. Neuroimaging, 281, 2430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broulidakis, M. J., Fairchild, G., Sully, K., Blumensath, T., Darekar, A., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2016). Reduced default mode connectivity in adolescents with conduct disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(9), 800808.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.05.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchmann, A., Hohmann, S., Brandeis, D., Banaschewski, T., & Poustka, L. (2014). Aggression in children and adolescents. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 17, 421442. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2013_261 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chester, D. S., Lynam, D. R., Milich, R., & DeWall, C. N. (2017). Physical aggressiveness and gray matter deficits in ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 97, 1722. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choe, D. E., Shaw, D. S., & Forbes, E. E. (2015). Maladaptive social information processing in childhood predicts young men’s atypical amygdala reactivity to threat. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 56(5), 549557. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12316 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, H., Hirsch, O., Albrecht, B., & Chavanon, M.-L. (2019). Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and emotion regulation over the life span. Current Psychiatry Reports, 21(3), 17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-019-1003-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cima, M., & Raine, A. (2009). Distinct characteristics of psychopathy relate to different subtypes of aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 835840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.031 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cima, M., Raine, A., Meesters, C., & Popma, A. (2013). Validation of the dutch reactive proactive questionnaire (RPQ): Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression from childhood to adulthood: Reactive and proactive aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 39(2), 99113. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coccaro, E. F., McCloskey, M. S., Fitzgerald, D. A., & Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biological Psychiatry, 62(2), 168178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coker-Appiah, D. S., White, S. F., Clanton, R., Yang, J., Martin, A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2013). Looming animate and inanimate threats: The response of the amygdala and periaqueductal gray. Social Neuroscience, 8(6), 621630. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.839480 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cope, L. M., Ermer, E., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2014). Paralimbic gray matter reductions in incarcerated adolescent females with psychopathic traits. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(4), 659668. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9810-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67(3), 993. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131875 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dailey, N. S., Smith, R., Vanuk, J. R., Raikes, A. C., & Killgore, W. D. S. (2018). Resting-state functional connectivity as a biomarker of aggression in mild traumatic brain injury. NeuroReport, 29(16), 14131417. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000001127 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction. NeuroImage, 9(2), 179194. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0395 Google ScholarPubMed
Dalwani, M. S., Tregellas, J. R., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Mikulich-Gilbertson, S. K., Raymond, K. M., Banich, M. T., Crowley, T. J., & Sakai, J. T. (2014). Default mode network activity in male adolescents with conduct and substance use disorder. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 134, 242250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.10.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, N. V., & Weiss, R. (2012). Dichotomizing continuous variables in statistical analysis: A practice to avoid. Medical Decision Making, 32(2), 225226. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X12437605 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Brito, S. A., Mechelli, A., Wilke, M., Laurens, K. R., Jones, A. P., Barker, G. J., Hodgins, S., & Viding, E. (2009). Size matters: Increased grey matter in boys with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Brain, 132(4), 843852. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, R. S., Ségonne, F., Fischl, B., Quinn, B. T., Dickerson, B. C., Blacker, D., Buckner, R. L., Dale, A. M., Maguire, R. P., Hyman, B. T., Albert, M. S., & Killiany, R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968980. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ducharme, S., Hudziak, J. J., Botteron, K. N., Ganjavi, H., Lepage, C., Collins, D. L., Albaugh, M. D., Evans, A. C., & Karama, S. (2011). Right anterior cingulate cortical thickness and bilateral striatal volume correlate with CBCL aggressive behavior scores in healthy children. Biological Psychiatry, 70(3), 283290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ermer, E., Cope, L. M., Nyalakanti, P. K., Calhoun, V. D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2012). Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in criminal psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(3), 649658. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026371 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, T., Ling, S., Raine, A., Yang, Y., & Schug, R. (2018). Alexithymia and reactive aggression: The role of the amygdala. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 281, 8591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.09.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14(1), 1122. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhg087 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B, Sereno, MI, & Dale, AM (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage, 9, 195–207. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0396 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gan, G., Preston-Campbell, R. N., Moeller, S. J., Steinberg, J. L., Lane, S. D., Maloney, T., Parvaz, M. A., Goldstein, R. Z., & Alia-Klein, N. (2016). Reward vs. Retaliation-the role of the mesocorticolimbic salience network in human reactive aggression. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 179. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garofalo, C., Neumann, C. S., & Velotti, P. (2021). Psychopathy and aggression: The role of emotion dysregulation. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(23-24), NP12640NP12664. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260519900946 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, S., ffytche, D., Simmons, A., Kumari, V., Howard, M., Hodgins, S., & Blackwood, N. (2012). The antisocial brain: Psychopathy matters. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(9), 962972. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, P., Hu, S., Jiang, X., Zheng, H., Mo, D., Cao, X., Zhu, J., & Zhong, H. (2022). Associations of neurocognition and social cognition with brain structure and function in early-onset schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 798105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haller, J. (2018). The role of central and medial amygdala in normal and abnormal aggression: A review of classical approaches. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 85, 3443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendricks, K., & Liu, J. (2012). Childbearing depression and childhood aggression: Literature review. MCN: The American Journal of Maternal/Child Nursing, 37(4), 253261. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMC.0b013e3182482c5e Google ScholarPubMed
Hofhansel, L., Weidler, C., Votinov, M., Clemens, B., Raine, A., & Habel, U. (2020). Morphology of the criminal brain: Gray matter reductions are linked to antisocial behavior in offenders. Brain Structure & Function, 225(7), 20172028. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02106-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. IBM Corp.Google Scholar
Ibrahim, K., Noble, S., He, G., Lacadie, C., Crowley, M. J., McCarthy, G., Scheinost, D., & Sukhodolsky, D. G. (2022). Large-scale functional brain networks of maladaptive childhood aggression identified by connectome-based predictive modeling. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(2), 985999. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01317-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, Y., Gao, Y., Dong, D., Sun, X., Situ, W., & Yao, S. (2022). Brain anatomy in boys with conduct disorder: Differences among aggression subtypes. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 55(1), 313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-022-01360-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuniishi, H., Ichisaka, S., Matsuda, S., Futora, E., Harada, R., & Hata, Y. (2016). Chronic inactivation of the orbitofrontal cortex increases anxiety-like behavior and impulsive aggression, but decreases depression-like behavior in rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 250. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00250 Google ScholarPubMed
Lee, T. M. C., Chan, S. C., & Raine, A. (2008). Strong limbic and weak frontal activation to aggressive stimuli in spouse abusers. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(7), 655656. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.46 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, W., Cui, H., Zhu, Z., Kong, L., Guo, Q., Zhu, Y., Hu, Q., Zhang, L., Li, H., Li, Q., Jiang, J., Meyers, J., Li, J., Wang, J., Yang, Z., & Li, C. (2016). Aberrant functional connectivity between the amygdala and the temporal pole in drug-free generalized anxiety disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 549. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00549 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Q., & Cole, D. A. (2021). Aggressive outbursts among adults with major depressive disorder: Results from the collaborative psychiatric epidemiological surveys. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 135, 325331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ly, M., Motzkin, J. C., Philippi, C. L., Kirk, G. R., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K. A., & Koenigs, M. (2012). Cortical thinning in psychopathy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(7), 743749. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11111627 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magalotti, S. R., Neudecker, M., Zaraa, S. G., & McVoy, M. K. (2019). Understanding chronic aggression and its treatment in children and adolescents. Current Psychiatry Reports, 21(12), 123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-019-1105-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naaijen, J., Mulder, L. M., Ilbegi, S., de Bruijn, S., Kleine-Deters, R., Dietrich, A., Hoekstra, P. J., Marsman, J.-B. C., Aggensteiner, P. M., Holz, N. E., Boettinger, B., Baumeister, S., Banaschewski, T., Saam, M. C., M E Schulze, U., Santosh, P. J., Sagar-Ouriaghli, I., Mastroianni, M., Castro Fornieles, J., Bargallo, N., Rosa, M., Arango, C., Penzol, M. J., Werhahn, J. E., Walitza, S., Brandeis, D., Glennon, J. C., Franke, B., Zwiers, M. P., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2020). Specific cortical and subcortical alterations for reactive and proactive aggression in children and adolescents with disruptive behavior. NeuroImage: Clinical, 27, 102344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102344 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nikolic, M., Pezzoli, P., Jaworska, N., & Seto, M. C. (2022). Brain responses in aggression-prone individuals: A systematic review and meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of anger- and aggression-eliciting tasks (pp. 2022.01.11.475895), bioRxiv.Google ScholarPubMed
Novitskaya, Y., Dümpelmann, M., Vlachos, A., Reinacher, P. C., & Schulze-Bonhage, A. (2020). In vivo-assessment of the human temporal network: Evidence for asymmetrical effective connectivity. NeuroImage, 214, 116769. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116769 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, I. R., Plotzker, A., & Ezzyat, Y. (2007). The enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain, 130(7), 17181731. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm052 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pehrs, C., Zaki, J., Schlochtermeier, L. H., Jacobs, A. M., Kuchinke, L., & Koelsch, S. (2017). The temporal pole top-down modulates the ventral visual stream during social cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 27(1), 777792. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv226 Google ScholarPubMed
Potegal, M. (2012). Temporal and frontal lobe initiation and regulation of the top-down escalation of anger and aggression. Behavioural Brain Research, 231(2), 386395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.10.049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke‐Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., Stouthamer‐Loeber, M., & Liu, J. (2006). The reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32(2), 159171. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20115 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero-Martínez, Á., Sarrate-Costa, C., & Moya-Albiol, L. (2022). Reactive vs proactive aggression: A differential psychobiological profile? Conclusions derived from a systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 136, 104626. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104626 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saylor, K. E., & Amann, B. H. (2016). Impulsive aggression as a comorbidity of attention-deficit/Hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26(1), 1925. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2015.0126 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiba, Y., Kim, C., Santangelo, A. M., & Roberts, A. C. (2015). Lesions of either anterior orbitofrontal cortex or ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in marmoset monkeys heighten innate fear and attenuate active coping behaviors to predator threat. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 250. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00250 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Ibrahim, K., Kalvin, C. B., Jordan, R. P., Eilbott, J., & Hampson, M. (2021). Increased amygdala and decreased frontolimbic resting-state functional connectivity in children with aggressive behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 17(7), 634644. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab128 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, Q., Zhang, Y., Zhou, J., & Wang, X. (2022). Altered resting-state functional connectivity in the default mode network in male juvenile violent offenders. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 16(2), 608616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-021-00535-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, J., Liao, Y., Song, M., Gao, J.-H., Zhou, B., Tan, C., Liu, T., Tang, Y., Chen, J., Chen, X., & Zhan, W. (2013). Aberrant default mode functional connectivity in early onset schizophrenia. PloS One, 8(7), e71061. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071061 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weathersby, F. L., King, J. B., Fox, J. C., Loret, A., & Anderson, J. S. (2019). Functional connectivity of emotional well-being: Overconnectivity between default and attentional networks is associated with attitudes of anger and aggression. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 291, 5262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2019.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale®—third edition (WAIS®-III),. Pearson Assessment Inc.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual for the Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence-second edition (WASI-II). NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
White, S. F., VanTieghem, M., Brislin, S. J., Sypher, I., Sinclair, S., Pine, D. S., Hwang, S., & Blair, R. J. R. (2016). Neural correlates of the propensity for retaliatory behavior in youths with disruptive behavior disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(3), 282290. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15020250 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Joshi, S. H., Jahanshad, N., Thompson, P. M., & Baker, L. A. (2017). Neural correlates of proactive and reactive aggression in adolescent twins: Neural correlates of adolescent aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 43(3), 230240. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21683 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., Roffman, J. L., Smoller, J. W., Zöllei, L., Polimeni, J. R., Fischl, B., Liu, H., & Buckner, R. L. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 11251165. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zheng, K.‐Z., Wang, H.‐N., Liu, J., Xi, Y.‐B., Li, L., Zhang, X., Li, J.‐M.,Yin, H., Tan, Q.‐R., Lu, H.‐B., & Li, B.‐J. (2018). Incapacity to control emotion in major depression may arise from disrupted white matter integrity and OFC-amygdala inhibition. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics, 24(11), 10531062. https://doi.org/10.1111/cns.12800 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, J., Yao, N., Fairchild, G., Cao, X., Zhang, Y., Xiang, Y.-T., Zhang, L., & Wang, X. (2016). Disrupted default mode network connectivity in male adolescents with conduct disorder. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10(4), 9951003. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9465-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics of sample

Figure 1

Figure 1. Limbic network and temporal pole locations: location of the limbic network (via Yeo’s 7 network parcellation) and temporal pole (via clustering).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Limbic network: identified cortical network, limbic network (a), and identified region of significance, temporal pole (b). Cortical volume of the right limbic network was significantly different between those with high and low reactive aggression (high reactive aggression > low reactive aggression). * = p < 0.05.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Temporal pole. Within the right limbic network, cortical volume of the temporal pole was also significantly different between those with high and low reactive aggression (high reactive aggression > low reactive aggression) * = p < 0.05.

Supplementary material: File

Bashford-Largo et al. supplementary material

Bashford-Largo et al. supplementary material
Download Bashford-Largo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 15.8 KB