Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-31T20:51:22.940Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“And they had a big, big, very long fight:” The development of evaluative language in preschoolers' oral fictional stories told in a peer-group context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 April 2021

Ageliki NICOLOPOULOU*
Affiliation:
Lehigh University, USA
Hande ILGAZ
Affiliation:
Bilkent University, Turkey
Marta SHIRO
Affiliation:
Universidad Central de Venezuela, Venezuela & Florida Atlantic University, USA
Lisa B. HSIN
Affiliation:
Harvard University, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Ageliki Nicolopoulou, Department of Psychology, Lehigh University, 17 Memorial Drive East, Bethlehem, PA 18015-3068, USA. E-mail: agn3@lehigh.edu

Abstract

This study examined the development of evaluative language in preschoolers’ oral fictional narratives using a storytelling/story-acting practice where children told stories to and for their friends. Evaluative language orients the audience to the teller's cognitive and emotional engagement with a story's events and characters, and we hypothesized that this STSA context might yield new information about the early development of this language, prior to elementary school. We analyzed 60 stories: the first and last story told by 10 children in each of three preschool classrooms (3-, 4-, and 5-year-old classes) that used STSA throughout the school year. Stories were coded for evaluative expressions and evidential expressions. Five-year-olds used significantly more evaluative language than did 3-year-olds, and children at all ages used significantly more evaluative language at the end than at the beginning of the year. The number of stories told throughout the year explained unique variance in children's evaluative language growth.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aksu-Koç, A. (2016). The interface of evidentials and epistemics in Turkish: Perspectives from acquisition. In Güven, M., Akar, D., Öztürk, & M. Kelepir, (Eds), Exploring the Turkish Linguistic Landscape (pp. 143156). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A., & Erciyes, A. A. (2018). Narrative discourse: Developmental perspectives. In Bar-On, A. & Ravid, D. (Eds.), Handbook of communicative disorders: Theoretical, empirical, and applied-linguistic perspectives (pp. 329356). Boston: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S., Skarabella, B., & Hughes, M. (2008). Using corpora to examine discourse effects in syntax. In Behrens, H. (Ed.), Corpora in language acquisition research: Finding structure in Data (pp. 99137). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations on children's narrative competence. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1997). Narrative theory and narrative development: The Labovian impact. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 235244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (Eds.), (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brockmeyer, C., Nicolopoulou, A., , A., & Ilgaz, H. (2009). Finding the best narrative assessment techniques for preschoolers: Elicitation methods compared across ages and over time using multiple narrative measures. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting for Society for Research in Child Development (April 2–4: Denver, CO).Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Carpendale, J. I., & Lewis, C. (2015). The development of social understanding. In Liben, L. S., Ulrich, U., Lerner, R. M. (Eds). Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes, vol. 2, 7 th ed. (pp. 381424). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, B. (1999). Language basis of reading and reading disabilities: Evidence from a longitudinal investigation. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(4), 331361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. (1986). Evidentiality in English conversation and academic writing. In Chafe, W. & Nichols, J. (Eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic code of epistemology (pp. 261312). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. (1994). Discourse, consciousness, and time: The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Choi, S. (2006). Acquisition of modality. In Frawley, W. (Ed.). The expression of modality (pp. 141172). NY: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., Griffin, J. A., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2012). How reading books fosters language development around the world. Child Development Research, Article ID 602807; doi:10.1155/2012/602807CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., Hoffer, K. G., & Rivera, B. L. (2019). The developing language foundation for reading comprehension. In Veneziano, E. & Nicolopoulou, A. (Eds), Narrative, literacy, and other skills (pp. 2141). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dickinson, D. K., & Porche, M. V. (2011). Relation between language experiences in preschool classrooms and children's Kindergarten and Fourth-grade language and reading abilities. Child Development, 82(3), 870886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (2001). Beginning literacy with language. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Field, A. (2016). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. Thousand Oaks, CA: SageGoogle Scholar
Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A. (2004). Test of Narrative Language. Austin, TX: Pro-edGoogle Scholar
Glenn-Applegate, K., Breit-Smith, A., Justice, L. M., & Piasta, S. (2010). Artfulness in young children's spoken narratives. Early Education and Development, 21(3), 468493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graf, E., & Davies, C. (2014). The production and comprehension of referring expressions. In Matthews, D. (Ed.) Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 161182). TILAR, Benjamins.Google Scholar
Griffin, T. M., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. P. (2004). Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First Language, 24(2), 123147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M., & Bassano, D. (2016). Modality and mood in first language acquisition. In Nuyts, J. & van der Auwera, J. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of modality and mood (pp. 430447). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental review, 26(1), 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huberty, C. J., & Morris, J. D. (1989). Multivariate analysis versus multiple univariate analyses. Psychology Bulletin, 105(2), 302308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A. C., & Nakamura, K. (2004). Linguistic strategies serving evaluative functions: A comparison between Japanese and Turkish narratives. In Strömqvist, S. & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.), Relating events in narratives: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 329358). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). The transformation of experience in narrative syntax. In Labov, W. (Ed.), Language in the inner city: Studies in the Black English Vernacular (pp. 354396). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967/1997). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, J. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 1244). Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press. Reprinted in Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 3–38.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project (3rd ed.). Volume I: Tools for Analyzing talk: Transcription Programs. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Malle, B. F. (2020). The tree of social cognition: Hierarchically organized capacities of mentalizing. In Gilead, M & Ochsner, K. (Eds). The neural bases of mentalizing. Springer.Google Scholar
Matsui, T. (2014). Children's understanding of linguistic expressions of certainty and evidentiality. In Matthews, D. (Ed.) Pragmatic development in first language acquisition (pp. 295316). TILAR, Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A. (1996). Narrative development in social context. In Slobin, D., Gerhardt, J., Guo, J., & Kyratzis, A. (Eds.), Social Interaction, Social Context, and Language: Essays in Honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 369390). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A. (1997). Worldmaking and identity formation in children's narrative play-acting. In Cox, B.D. & Lightfoot, C. (Eds.), Sociogenetic Perspectives on Internalization (pp. 157187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A. (2002). Peer-group culture and narrative development. In Blum-Kulka, S. & Snow, C.E. (Eds.), Talking to Adults: The Contribution of Multiparty Discourse to Language Acquisition (pp. 117 152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A. (2017). Promoting oral narrative skills through a peer-oriented storytelling and story-acting practice. In Cremin, T., Flewitt, R., Mardell, B., & Swann, J. (Eds). Storytelling in Early Childhood: Enriching Language, Literacy, and Classroom Culture (pp. 4966). London & New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A. (2019). Using a storytelling/story-acting practice to promote narrative and other decontextualized language skills in disadvantaged children. In Veneziano, E. & Nicolopoulou, A. (Eds). Narrative, Literacy, and Other Skills: Studies in Intervention (pp. 163184).Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., Cates Brockmeyer, C., de Sá, A., & Ilgaz, H. (2014). Ch. 3: Narrative performance, peer-group culture, and narrative development in a preschool classroom. In Cakaite, A., Blum-Kulka, S., Aukrust, V., & Teubal, E. (Eds.). Children's peer talk: Learning from each other (pp. 4262). NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., Cortina, K. S., Ilgaz, H, Cates Brockmeyer, C., & de Sá, A. (2015). Using a narrative- and play-based activity to promote low-income preschoolers' oral language, emergent literacy, and social competence. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 31, 147162. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolopoulou, A., & Richner, E. S. (2007). From actors to agents to persons: The development of character representation in young children's narratives. Child Development, 78, 412429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicolopoulou, A., , A., Ilgaz, H., & Brockmeyer, C. (2010). Using the transformative power of play to educate hearts and minds: From Vygotsky to Vivian Paley and beyond. Mind, Culture, and Activity: An International Journal, 17, 117.Google Scholar
Nilsen, E. S., & Fecica, A. M. (2011). A model of communicative perspective-taking for typical and atypical populations of children. Developmental Review, 31, 55.78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, D. K. (2007). The language use inventory for young children: A parent-report measure of pragmatic language development of 18- to 47-month-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 216228.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Neill, D. K., Pearce, M. J., & Pick, J. L. (2004). Preschool children's narratives and performance on the Peabody Individualized Achievement Test–Revised: Evidence of a relationship between early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Language, 24(2), 149183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özyürek, A., & Trabasso, T. (1997). Evaluation during the understanding of narratives. Discourse Processes, 23, 305335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paley, V. G. (1986). Mollie is three: Growing up in school. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paley, V. G. (1990). The boy who would be a helicopter: The uses of storytelling in the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. NY: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Suggate, S., Lang, J., & Shaughency, E. (2010). Children's oral narrative and reading skills in the first 3 years of reading instruction. Reading and Writing, 23, 627644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sénéchal, M., & Lever, R. (2014). Young children's narrative abilities: Links to syntax comprehension and reading. In Robson, S. & Quinn, S. R. (Eds.), The Routledge International Handbook of young children's thinking and understanding (pp. 96108). Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Shiro, M. (2003). Genre and evaluation in narrative development. Journal of Child Language, 30, 165195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiro, M. (2004). Expressions of epistemic modality and the construction of narrative stance in Venezuelan children's stories. Psychology of Language and Communication, 8(2), 3556.Google Scholar
Shiro, M., Diez-Itza, E., & Fernández-Urquiza, M. (2019). Evaluative language and component structure of oral narratives in Williams Syndrome. In Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., Buil-Legaz, L., López-Penadés, R., Sanchez-Azanza, V., & Adrover-Roig, D. (Eds) Atypical language development in Romance Languages (pp. 235257). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., Porche, M. V., Tabors, P. O., & Harris, S. R. (2007). Is literacy enough? Pathways to academic success for adolescents. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Spinillo, A. G., & Pinto, G. (1994). Children's narratives under different conditions: A comparative study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 177193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suggate, S., Schaughency, E., McAnnaly, H., & Reese, E. (2018). From infancy to adolescence: The longitudinal links between vocabulary, early literacy skills, oral narrative, and reading comprehension. Cognitive Development, 47, 8295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, G., & Hunston, S. (2000). Evaluation: An introduction. In Hunston, S. & Thompson, G. (Eds.), Evaluation in text: Authorial stance and the construction of discourse (pp. 127). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ukrainetz, T. A., Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Eisenberg, S. L., Gillam, R. B., & Harm, H. M. (2005). The development of expressive elaboration in fictional narratives. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 13631377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ünal, E., & Papafragou, A. (2018). Evidentials, information sources, and cognition. In Aikhenvald, A. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook on evidentiality. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wellhousen, K. (1993). Eliciting and examining young children's storytelling. Journal of Research in Child Education, 7, (2), 6266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar