Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-12T12:05:13.663Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploring the Linguistic, Cognitive, and Social Skills Underlying Lexical Processing Efficiency as Measured by the Looking-while-Listening Paradigm

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 March 2021

Samuel RONFARD*
Affiliation:
University of Toronto Mississauga, Canada
Ran WEI*
Affiliation:
Harvard University, USA
Meredith L. ROWE
Affiliation:
Harvard University, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Samuel Ronfard University of Toronto Mississauga 3359 Mississauga Road CCT Building, Room 4059 Mississauga, Ontario, L5L 1C6, Canada, E-mail: samuel.ronfard@utoronto.ca
Ran Wei Harvard University 20 University Road Room 608 Cambridge, MA 02138, United States, Email: ran_wei@g.harvard.edu

Abstract

The looking-while-listening (LWL) paradigm is frequently used to measure toddlers’ lexical processing efficiency (LPE). Children's LPE is associated with vocabulary size, yet other linguistic, cognitive, or social skills contributing to LPE are not well understood. It also remains unclear whether LPE measures from two types of LWL trials (target-initial versus distractor-initial trials) are differentially associated with the abovementioned potential correlates of LPE. We tested 18- to 24-month-olds and found that children's word learning on a fast-mapping task was associated with LPE measures from all trials and distractor-initial trials but not target-initial trials. Children's vocabulary and pragmatic skills were both associated with their fast-mapping performance. Executive functions and pragmatic skills were associated with LPE measures from distractor-initial but not target-initial trials. Hence, LPE as measured by the LWL paradigm may reflect a constellation of skills important to language development. Methodological implications for future studies using the LWL paradigm are discussed.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Joint First and Corresponding Authors

References

Adams, K. A., Marchman, V. A., Loi, E. C., Ashland, M. D., Fernald, A., & Feldman, H. M. (2018). Caregiver talk and medical risk as predictors of language outcomes in full term and preterm toddlers. Child Development, 89(5), 16741690. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12818CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bavin, E. L., Kidd, E., Prendergast, L., Baker, E., Dissanayake, C., & Prior, M. (2014). Severity of autism is related to children's language processing. Autism Research, 7(6), 687694. doi: 10.1002/aur.1410CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Gunn, D. M., & Leigh, E. (2005). Mapping the developmental constraints on working memory span performance. Developmental Psychology, 41(4), 579597. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.579CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2012). At 6-9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 32533258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113380109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergelson, E., & Swingley, D. (2018). Young infants’ word comprehension given an unfamiliar talker or altered pronunciations. Child Development, 89(5), 15671576. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12888CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernard, S., & Deleau, M. (2007). Conversational perspective-taking and false belief attribution: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 443460. doi: 10.1348/026151006X171451CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children's executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bion, R. A. H., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126(1), 3953. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P., & Markson, L. (1998). Capacities underlying word learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(2), 6773. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01121-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2016). Praat (Version 6.0.39) [Computer software]: Doing phonetics by computer. Retrieved from http://www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language (1st ed.). New York, NY: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Taylor, E., Drew, A., Cockerill, H., Brown, J.-A., & Baird, G. (2005). Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(5), 500513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (2003). First language acquisition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
De Rosnay, M., Fink, E., Begeer, S., Slaughter, V., & Peterson, C. (2014). Talking theory of mind talk: young school-aged children's everyday conversation and understanding of mind and emotion. Journal of Child Language, 41(5), 11791193. doi: 10.1017/S0305000913000433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eiteljoerge, S. F. V., Adam, M., Elsner, B., & Mani, N. (2019). Consistency of co-occurring actions influences young children's word learning. Royal Society Open Science, 6(8), 190097. doi: 10.1098/rsos.190097CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ELAN (Version 5.0) [Computer software]. (2016). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, The Language Archive. Retrieved from https://archive.mpi.nl/tla/elanGoogle Scholar
Ellis, E. M., Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., & Evans, J. L. (2015). Novel word learning: An eye-tracking study. Are 18-month-old late talkers really different from their typical peers? Journal of Communication Disorders, 58, 143157. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2015.06.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis Weismer, S., Haebig, E., Edwards, J., Saffran, J., & Venker, C. E. (2016). Lexical processing in toddlers with ASD: Does weak central coherence play a role? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(12), 37553769. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2926-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5, Serial No. 242).Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User's Guide and Technical Manual, 2nd Ed. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. E., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. A. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken language comprehension by infants and young children. In Sekerina, I. A., Fernández, E. M., & Clahsen, H. (Eds.), Developmental Psycholinguistics: On-line methods in children's language processing (pp. 97135). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. doi: 10.1075/lald.44.06ferCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically developing and late-talking toddlers: Lexical processing and vocabulary growth. Child Development, 83(1), 203222. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01692.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234248. doi: 10.1111/desc.12019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 98116. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.98CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Pinto, J. P., Swingley, D., Weinberg, A., & McRoberts, G. W. (2001a). Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. In Tomasello, M. & Bates, E. (Eds.), Language development: The essential readings (pp. 4956). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Swingley, D., & Pinto, J. P. (2001b). When half a word is enough: Infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information. Child Development, 72(4), 10031015. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Thorpe, K., & Marchman, V. A. (2010). Blue car, red car: Developing efficiency in online interpretation of adjective–noun phrases. Cognitive Psychology, 60(3), 190217. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freed, J., Lockton, E., & Adams, C. (2012). Short-term and working memory skills in primary school-aged children with specific language impairment and children with pragmatic language impairment: Phonological, linguistic and visuo-spatial aspects: Memory skills in pragmatic language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(4), 457466. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00148.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Cauley, K. M., & Gordon, L. (1987). The eyes have it: Lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm. Journal of Child Language, 14(1), 2345. doi: 10.1017/S030500090001271XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heibeck, T. H., & Markman, E. M. (1987). Word learning in children: An examination of fast mapping. Child Development, 58(4), 1021. doi: 10.2307/1130543CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2007). Spoken word recognition by Latino children learning Spanish as their first language. Journal of Child Language, 34(2), 227249. doi: 10.1017/S0305000906007896CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31F39. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00768.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122149. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.1.122CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lany, J. (2017). Lexical-processing efficiency leverages novel word learning in infants and toddlers. Developmental Science, e12569. doi: 10.1111/desc.12569Google ScholarPubMed
Law, F., & Edwards, J. R. (2015). Effects of vocabulary size on online lexical processing by preschoolers. Language Learning and Development, 11(4), 331355. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2014.961066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law, F., Mahr, T., Schneeberg, A., & Edwards, J. (2017). Vocabulary size and auditory word recognition in preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(1), 89125. doi: 10.1017/S0142716416000126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Ellis Weismer, S., Miller, C. A., Francis, D. J., Tomblin, J. B., & Kail, R. V. (2007). Speed of processing, working memory, and language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(2), 408428. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/029)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 18(3), 193198. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01871.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucca, K., & Wilbourn, M. P. (2018). Communicating to learn: Infants’ pointing gestures result in optimal learning. Child Development, 89(3), 941960. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12707CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9F16. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchman, V. A., Loi, E. C., Adams, K. A., Ashland, M., Fernald, A., & Feldman, H. M. (2018). Speed of language comprehension at 18 months old predicts school-relevant outcomes at 54 months old in children born preterm. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 1. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000541Google Scholar
Markson, L., & Bloom, P. (1997). Evidence against a dedicated system for word learning in children. Nature, 385(6619), 813815. doi: 10.1038/385813a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K. (2018). Individual Differences in Children's Pragmatic Ability: A Review of Associations with Formal Language, Social Cognition, and Executive Functions. Language Learning and Development, 14(3), 186223. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMurray, B., Horst, J. S., & Samuelson, L. K. (2012). Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychological Review, 119(4), 831877. doi: 10.1037/a0029872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category (N3C) principle. Child Development, 65(6), 16461662. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00840.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newbury, J., Klee, T., Stokes, S. F., & Moran, C. (2015). Exploring expressive vocabulary variability in two-year-olds: The role of working memory. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 58(6), 17611772. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-15-0018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newbury, J., Klee, T., Stokes, S. F., & Moran, C. (2016). Interrelationships between working memory, processing speed, and language development in the age range 2–4 years. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 59(5), 1146. doi: 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0322CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ninio, A., & Snow, C. E. (1996). Pragmatic development. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
O'Neill, D. K. (2007). The Language Use Inventory for young children: A parent-report measure of pragmatic language development for 18- to 47-month-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(1), 214228. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/017)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pesco, D., & O'Neill, D. K. (2012). Predicting later language outcomes from the language use inventory. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 55(2), 421. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0273)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peter, M. S., Durrant, S., Jessop, A., Bidgood, A., Pine, J. M., & Rowland, C. F. (2019). Does speed of processing or vocabulary size predict later language growth in toddlers? Cognitive Psychology, 115, 101238. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.101238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poll, G. H., Miller, C. A., Mainela-Arnold, E., Adams, K. D., Misra, M., & Park, J. S. (2013). Effects of children's working memory capacity and processing speed on their sentence imitation performance. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 48(3), 329342. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Puccini, D., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). 15-month-old infants fast map words but not representational gestures of multimodal labels. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajan, V., Konishi, H., Ridge, K., Houston, D. M., Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., … Schwartz, R. G. (2019). Novel word learning at 21 months predicts receptive vocabulary outcomes in later childhood. Journal of Child Language, 1–15. doi: 10.1017/S0305000918000600Google Scholar
Rollins, P., & Snow, C. E. (1998). Shared attention and grammatical development in typical children and children with autism. Journal of Child Language, 25, 653673. doi:10.1017/ S0305000998003596CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuelson, L. L., & McMurray, B. (2017). What does it take to learn a word? Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science, 8(1–2), 116. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1421CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2018). Discontinuity of reference hinders children's learning of new words. Child Development. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13189Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, L., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition, 106(3), 15581568. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E., Pan, B. A., Imbens-Bailey, A., & Herman, J. (1996). Learning how to say what one means: A longitudinal study of children's speech act use. Social Development, 5(1), 5684. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1996.tb00072.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swingley, D., Pinto, J. P., & Fernald, A. (1999). Continuous processing in word recognition at 24 months. Cognition, 71(2), 73108. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00021-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H., Rogers, S., Cooper, J., Landa, R., Lord, C., Paul, R., Rice, M., Stoel-Gammon, C., Wetherby, A., & Yoder, P. (2009). Defining spoken language benchmarks and selecting measures of expressive language development for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(3), 113. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0136)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2000). The social-pragmatic theory of word learning. Pragmatics, 10(4), 401413. doi: 10.1075/prag.10.4.01tomCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wei, R., Ronfard, S., Leyva, D., & Rowe, M. L. (2019). Teaching a novel word: Parenting styles and toddlers’ word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 187, 104639. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2019.05.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 21432152. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Ronfard et al. supplementary material

Ronfard et al. supplementary material

Download Ronfard et al. supplementary material(File)
File 715.1 KB