Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2pzkn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-23T15:56:55.986Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Production of atypical category exemplars in patients with schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2010

GILDAS BRÉBION*
Affiliation:
Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom Unit of Research and Development, Sant Joan de Déu - Serveis de Salut Mental y CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
RODRIGO A. BRESSAN
Affiliation:
Center for Neuroimaging and Cognition, University of Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Brazil
RUTH I. OHLSEN
Affiliation:
Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
LYN S. PILOWSKY
Affiliation:
Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
ANTHONY S. DAVID
Affiliation:
Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Gildas Brébion, Unit of Research and Development, Sant Joan de Déu - Serveis de Salut Mental, C\ Doctor Antoni Pujadas 42, 08830 Sant Boi de Llobregat (Barcelona) Spain. E-mail: gildas.brebion@kcl.ac.uk

Abstract

Previous studies have revealed semantic memory impairments in patients with schizophrenia, and suggested that certain of these impairments were related to thought disorganization. One explanation offered for this is a broadening of the boundaries of semantic categories in schizophrenia. We selected 16 semantic categories, and required a sample of 41 schizophrenia patients and 43 healthy control subjects to produce one exemplar from each category. The typicality of the subjects’ responses was rated. The exemplars produced by the patients were on average less typical than those produced by the healthy controls. No significant association between typicality of the response and thought disorganization was revealed in the patient sample. Affective flattening, alogia, and anhedonia were significantly and inversely associated with the typicality score, that is, higher ratings of these symptoms were associated with more typical responses. Our results suggest that a broadening of semantic category boundaries is observed in patients with schizophrenia, but is unrelated to thought disorganization. This semantic abnormality is not a feature of the patients with high ratings of certain negative symptoms. (JINS, 2010, 16, 822–828.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Battig, W.F., & Montague, W.E. (1969). Category norms for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology (Monograph), 80, 146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brébion, G., Amador, X., Smith, M., Malaspina, D., Sharif, Z., & Gorman, J. (1999). Opposite links of positive and negative symptomatology with memory errors in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 88, 1524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., David, A.S., Bressan, R.A., Ohlsen, R.I., & Pilowsky, L.S. (2009). Hallucinations and two types of free-recall intrusion in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 39, 917926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., David, A.S., Jones, H.M., Ohlsen, R., & Pilowsky, L.S. (2007). Temporal context discrimination in patients with schizophrenia: Associations with auditory hallucinations and negative symptoms. Neuropsychologia, 45, 817823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., David, A.S., Jones, H., & Pilowsky, L.S. (2005). Hallucinations, negative symptoms, and response bias in a verbal recognition task in schizophrenia, Neuropsychology, 19, 612617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brébion, G., David, A.S., Ohlsen, R., Jones, H.M., & Pilowsky, L.S. (2007). Visual memory errors in schizophrenic patients with auditory and visual hallucinations. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 832838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., Gorman, J., Amador, X., Malaspina, D., & Sharif, Z. (2002). Source monitoring impairments in schizophrenia: Characterisation and associations with positive and negative symptomatology. Psychiatry Research, 112, 2739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brébion, G., Ohlsen, R.I., Pilowsky, L.S., & David, A.S. (2008). Visual hallucinations in schizophrenia: Confusion between imagination and perception. Neuropsychology, 22, 383389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, E.Y., Wilkins, A.J., & McKenna, P.J. (1994). Semantic memory is both impaired and anomalous in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 24, 193202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doughty, O.J., & Done, D.J. (2009). Is semantic memory impaired in schizophrenia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 91 studies. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14, 473509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doughty, O.J., Done, D.J., Lawrence, V.A., Al-Mousawi, A., & Ashaye, K. (2008). Semantic memory impairment in schizophrenia – Deficit in storage or access of knowledge? Schizophrenia Research, 105, 4048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elvevåg, B., Storms, G., Heit, E., & Goldberg, T. (2005). Category content and structure in schizophrenia: An evaluation using the instantiation principle. Neuropsychology, 19, 371380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elvevåg, B., Weickert, T., Wechsler, M., Coppola, R., Weinberger, D.R., & Goldberg, T.E. (2002). An investigation of the integrity of semantic boundaries in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 53, 187198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, T.E., Aloia, M.S., Gourovitch, M.L., Missar, D., Pickar, D., & Weinberger, D.R. (1998). Cognitive substrates of thought disorder, I: The semantic system. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 16711676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, N.E., Done, D.J., Anthony, S.H., McKenna, P.J., & Ochocki, M. (2004). Can disorganisation of semantic memory account for the abnormalities of thought in schizophrenia – a controlled experimental study. Schizophrenia Research, 70, 233240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichs, R.W., & McDermid Vaz, S. (2004). Verbal memory errors and symptoms in schizophrenia. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17, 98101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiang, M., & Kutas, M. (2006). Abnormal typicality of responses on a category fluency task in schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 145, 119126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiefer, M., Martens, U., Weisbrod, M., Hermle, L., & Spitzer, M. (2009). Increased unconscious semantic activation in schizophrenia patients with formal thought disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 114, 7983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreher, D.A., Holcomb, P.J., Goff, D., & Kuperberg, G.R. (2008). Neural evidence for faster and further automatic spreading activation in schizophrenic thought disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34, 473482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moelter, S.T., Hill, S.K., Hughett, P., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., & Ragland, J.D. (2005). Organization of semantic category exemplars in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 78, 209217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, S.A., & Asberg, M. (1979). A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 382389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S., Woodward, T.S., Küppers, D., Lausen, A., & Schickel, M. (2002). Increased automatic spreading of activation in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 59, 181186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomarol-Clotet, E., Oh, T.M., Laws, K.R., & McKenna, P.J. (2008). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, 9297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossell, S.L. (2006). Category fluency performance in patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: The influence of affective categories. Schizophrenia Research, 82, 135138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossell, S.L., & David, A.S. (2006). Are semantic deficits in schizophrenia due to problems with access or storage? Schizophrenia Research, 82, 121134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stirling, J., Hellewell, J., Blakey, A., & Deakin, W. (2006). Thought disorder in schizophrenia is associated with both executive dysfunction and circumscribed impairments in semantic function. Psychological Medicine, 36, 475484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turetsky, B.I., Moberg, P.J., Mozley, L.H., Moelter, S.T., Agrin, R.N., Gur, R.C., et al. . (2002). Memory-delineated subtypes of schizophrenia: Relationship to clinical, neuroanatomical, and neurophysiological measures. Neuropsychology, 16, 481490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed