Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-01T03:01:49.971Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Review of Kelly S. Mix, Linda B. Smith and Michael Gasser (eds.). The spatial foundations of language and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2010, xviii + 307 pp., ISBN 978-0-19-955324-2.

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2014

Michael Pleyer*
Affiliation:
University of Heidelberg, Germany. E-mail: michael.pleyer@hggs.uni-heidelberg.de

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Reviews
Copyright
Copyright © UK Cognitive Linguistics Association 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldwin, D. A.. 1993. Early referential understanding: Infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology 29. 832843.Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M., Pook, P. K. & Rao, R. P. N.. 1997. Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20. 723767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. 2001. Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology 43. 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V.. 1997. Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition 64. 137.Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Marquez, M., Mittelberg, I., Coulson, S. & Spivey, M. J. (eds.) 2007. Methods in Cognitive Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. 1987. The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kirsh, D. 1995. The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence 73. 3168.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. 1987. Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. & Núñez, R.. 2000. Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W.. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, volume 1: Theoreticalpre-requisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Moll, H. & Meltzoff, A. N.. 2011. Perspective-taking and its foundation in joint attention. In Eilan, N., Lerman, H. & Roessler, J. (eds.), Perception, causation, and objectivity. Issues inphilosophy and psychology, 286304. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B.. 1956. The child's conception of space. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 2000. Towards a cognitive semantics, volume 1: Conceptual structuring systems. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Verhagen, A. 2007. Construal and perspectivization. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics, 4881. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Winter, B. & Bergen, B. K.. 2012. Language comprehenders represent object distance both visually and auditorily: Evidence for the immersed experiencer view. Language and Cognition 4. 116.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A.. 2004. The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, B. H. (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, 3562. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar