Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-09T06:39:25.761Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Analog Simulation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2022

Russell Trenholme*
Affiliation:
Minnesota Center for Philosophy of Science University of Minnesota
*
Send reprint requests to the author, Minnesota Center for Philosophy of Science, University of Minnesota, 315 Ford Hall, 224 Church Street S.E., Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA.

Abstract

The distinction between analog and digital representation is reexamined; it emerges that a more fundamental distinction is that between symbolic and analog simulation. Analog simulation is analyzed in terms of a (near) isomorphism of causal structures between a simulating and a simulated process. It is then argued that a core concept, naturalistic analog simulation, may play a role in a bottom-up theory of adaptive behavior which provides an alternative to representational analyses. The appendix discusses some formal conditions for naturalistic analog simulation.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 1994 by the Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Brooks, R. (1991a), “Challenges for Complete Creature Architectures”, in J. A. Meyer and S. W. Wilson (eds.), pp. 434443.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. (1991b), “Intelligence Without Reason”, in Twelfth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence: Proceedings (IJCAI-91). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann, pp. 569595.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. (1991c), “Intelligence Without Representation”, Artificial Intelligence 47: 139160.10.1016/0004-3702(91)90053-MCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, N. (1983), How the Laws of Physics Lie. Oxford: Clarendon.10.1093/0198247044.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, K. (1967), The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cummins, R. (1989), Meaning and Mental Representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1987), The Intentional Stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. (1981), Knowledge and the Flow of Information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dretske, F. (1988), Explaining Behavior: Reasons in a World of Causes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1990), The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Godfrey-Smith, P. (1989), “Misinformation”, Canadian Journal of Philosophy 19: 533550.10.1080/00455091.1989.10716781CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grandy, R. (1987), “Information-Based Epistemology, Ecological Epistemology and Epistemology Naturalized”, Synthese 70: 191203.10.1007/BF00413935CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanson, S. and Burr, D. (1990), “What Connectionist Models Learn: Learning and Representation in Connectionist Networks”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13: 417518.10.1017/S0140525X00079760CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1987), “An Overview of the Frame Problem”, in Pylyshyn, Z. W. (ed.), The Robot's Dilemma: The Frame Problem in Artificial Intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 7793.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. (1983), Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1971), “Analog and Digital”, Nous 5: 321327.10.2307/2214671CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maes, P. (1990), Designing Autonomous Agents. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.1016/S0921-8890(05)80024-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, J. A. and Wilson, S. (1991), From Animals to Animats: Proceedings of the First International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/3115.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, R. and van Gelder, T. (1991), “Representing Aspects of Language”, in Hammond, K. and Gentner, D. (eds.), Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 487492.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1984), Computation and Cognition: Toward a Foundation for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. (1975), “Form, Formation, and Transformations of Internal Representations”, in Solso, R. (ed.), Information Processing and Cognition: The Lyola Symposium. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Suppes, P. (1970), A Probabilistic Theory of Causality. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
van Fraassen, B. (1980), The Scientific Image. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/0198244274.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiener, N. (1961), Cybernetics: or, Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine. 2d ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar