Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-12T18:57:41.494Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chronobiology of pupil dilation in design students during idea generation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 May 2024

Samuele Colombo*
Affiliation:
Politecnico di Torino, Italy
John S. Gero
Affiliation:
UNC Charlotte, United States of America
Alessandro Mazza
Affiliation:
University of Turin, Italy
Marco Cantamessa
Affiliation:
Politecnico di Torino, Italy

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Chronobiology studies physiological variations due to the time of day, an unexplored factor in design research. This paper explores the effect of time of day on designers' physiological responses in idea generation. Convergent (CT) and divergent (DT) thinking, as building blocks of designing, are explored using pupil dilation as a proxy for cognitive load. Time of day and educational background are explored for engineering and industrial designers. Results show a larger pupil diameter in the afternoon than in the morning, especially for DT, with higher values for industrial designers.

Type
Human Behaviour and Design Creativity
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
The Author(s), 2024.

References

Agnoli, S., Franchin, L., Rubaltelli, E., & Corazza, G.E. (2019). The emotionally intelligent use of attention and affective arousal under creative frustration and creative success, Personality and individual differences, 142, 242-248, https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, R., Nyström, M., & Holmqvist, K. (2010). Sampling frequency and eye-tracking measures: how speed affects durations, latencies, and more. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3(3). https://dx.doi.org/10.16910/jemr.3.3.6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aschoff, J., & Wever, R. (1976). Human circadian rhythms: A multioscillatory system. Federal Proc., 35, .Google ScholarPubMed
Balters, S., & Steinert, M. (2017). Capturing emotion reactivity through physiology measurement as a foundation for affective engineering in engineering design science and engineering practices. Journal of Intelligent Manufacturing, 28(7), 15851607, https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10845-015-1145-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balters, S., Weinstein, T., Mayseless, N., Auernhammer, J., Hawthorne, G., Steinert, M., Meinel, C., Leifer, L.J. & Reiss, A.L. (2023). Design science and neuroscience: A systematic review of the emergent field of design neurocognition, Design Studies, 84, https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2022.101148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borgianni, Y., & Maccioni, L. (2020). Review of the use of neurophysiological and biometric measures in experimental design research. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing, 34(2), 248-285 https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0890060420000062CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borgianni, Y., Maccioni, L. & Basso, D. (2019). Exploratory study on the perception of additively manufactured end-use products with specific questionnaires and eye-tracking. Int J Interact Des Manuf 13, 743759. https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12008-019-00563-wCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colombo, S., Gero, J.S., & Cantamessa, M. (2023). Chronobiology in divergent thinking: how designers are affected by time of day, Proceedings of the Design Society, International conference of engineering design 2023, ICED23, 3, 887-896, https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/pds.2023.89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daguet, I, Bouhassira, D, Gronfier, C. (2019). Baseline Pupil Diameter Is Not a Reliable Biomarker of Subjective Sleepiness. Front Neurol.;10:108. https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2019.00108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dybvik, H., Abelson, F., Aalto, P., Goucher-Lambert, K., & Steinert, M. (2022). Inspirational stimuli improve idea fluency during ideation: A replication and extension study with eye-tracking. Proceedings of the Design Society, 2, 861-870. https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/pds.2022.88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckstein, M.K., Guerra-Carrillo, B., Miller Singley, A.T., & Bunge, S.A. (2017). Beyond eye gaze: What else can eyetracking reveal about cognition and cognitive development? Dev Cogn Neurosci. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Facer-Childs, ER, Boiling, S, Balanos, GM. (2018) The effects of time of day and chronotype on cognitive and physical performance in healthy volunteers. Sports Med Open. 4(1):47. https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s40798-018-0162-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavas, R., Tripathy, S., Chatterjee, D., & Sinha, A. (2018). Cognitive load and metacognitive confidence extraction from pupillary response. Cognitive Systems Research, 52:325334. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2018.07.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gero, J.S., & Milovanovic, J. (2020). A framework for studying design thinking through measuring designers' minds, bodies and brains. Design Science, 6, E19. https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2020.15CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gero, J.S., & Milovanovic, J. (2022). Creation and characterization of design spaces, in Lockton, D., Lenzi, S., Hekkert, P., Oak, A., Sádaba, J., Lloyd, P. (eds.), DRS2022, Bilbao, Spain. https://dx.doi.org/10.21606/drs.2022.265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goucher-Lambert, K., Moss, J. & Cagan, J. (2019). Inside the mind: Using neuroimaging to understand moral product preference judgements involving sustainability. Journal of Mechanical Design (139). https://dx.doi.org/10.1115/1.4035859Google Scholar
Guilford, J.P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence, McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
Hay, L., Duffy, A.H.B., McTeague, C., Pidgeon, L.M., Vuletic, T., & Grealy, M. (2017). A systematic review of protocol studies on conceptual design cognition, Design Science, 3. https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2017.11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horne, J.A. & Östberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology: 4, 97-100. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2008.01.001Google ScholarPubMed
Jauk, E., Benedek, M. & Neubauer, A.C. (2012). Tackling creativity at its roots: Evidence for different patterns of EEG alpha activity related to convergent and divergent modes of task processing, International journal of psychophysiology, 84(2), 219-225, https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.02.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J.H. & Ostwald, M.J. (2022). The relationship between divergent thinking and ideation in conceptual design process, Design studies, 79, https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2022.101089CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S., Becattini, N., & Cascini, G. (2021). Correlating design performance to EEG activation: Early evidence from experimental data. Proceedings of the Design Society, 1, 771780, https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/pds.2021.77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahanama, B., Jayawardana, Y., Rengarajan, S., Jayawardena, G., Chukoskie, L., Snider, J., & Jayarathna, S. (2022). Eye movement and pupil measures: a review, Frontiers in Computer Science, (3), https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fcomp.2021.733531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, C. P. (1999). Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 142147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazza, A., Dal Monte, O., Schintu, S., Colombo, S., Michielli, N., Sarasso, P., Törlind, P., Cantamessa, M., Montagna, F. and Ricci, R. (2023). Beyond alpha-band: the neural correlate of creative thinking, Neuropsychologia, 179(10), 1-10, https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitre-Hernandez, H, Covarrubias Carrillo, R, Lara-Alvarez, C. (2021). Pupillary responses for cognitive load measurement to classify difficulty levels in an educational video game: Empirical study. JMIR Serious Games, 9(1), https://dx.doi.org/10.2196/21620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Münch, M., Léon, L., Crippa, S.V., & Kawasaki, A. (2012). Circadian and wake-dependent effects on the pupil light reflex in response to narrow-bandwidth light pulses, Investigative ophthalmology & Visual Science, 53, 4546-4555, https://dx.doi.org/10.1167/iovs.12-9494CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, T. A., & Zeng, Y. (2014). A physiological study of relationship between designer's mental effort and mental stress during conceptual design. Computer-Aided Design, 54, 318. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cad.2013.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, P., Nguyen, T. A., & Zeng, Y. (2019). Segmentation of design protocol using EEG. Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing, 33(1), 1123 https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0890060417000622CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petkar, H., Dande, S., Yadav, R., Zeng, Y., & Nguyen, T. A. (2009). A pilot study to assess designer's mental stress using eye gaze system and electroencephalogram, 899-909. https://dx.doi.org/10.1115/DETC2009-86542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodemer, M., Karch, J., and Bernholt, S. (2023). Pupil dilation as cognitive load measure in instructional videos on complex chemical representations, Front. Educ., 8, https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2023.1062053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roenneberg, T., Wirz-Justice, A., & Merrow, M. (2003). Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. Journal of Biological Rhythms. 18, 8090. https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0748730402239679CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabaoui, I., Lotfi, S., & Talbi, M. (2023). Circadian fluctuations of cognitive and psychomotor performance in medical students: the role of daytime and chronotype patterns, Chronobiology in Medicine, 5(3), 127-137, https://dx.doi.org/10.33069/cim.2023.0018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 755-789, https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, C., & Bao, Y. (2017). Chronobiological research for cognitive science: a multifaceted view, Psych Journal, 6(4), 249-252, https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pchj.203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Self, J.A. (2019). Communication through design sketches: Implications for stakeholder interpretation during concept design. Design Studies, 63, 136. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2019.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shealy, T., Gero, J., Hu, M., & Milovanovic, J. (2020). Concept generation techniques change patterns of brain activation during engineering design. Design Science, 6, e31. https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2020.30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitschan, M. (2021). Time-varying light exposure in chronobiology and sleep research experiments, Frontiers in Neurology, 12, https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2021.654158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, L., Xiang, W., Chai, C., Yang, Z., & Zhang, K. (2014). Designers' perception during sketching: An examination of Creative Segment theory using eye movements. Design Studies, 35(6), 593613. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2014.04.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vieira, S., Gero, J.S., Delmoral, J., Gattol, V., Fernandes, C., & Parente, M. (2020). The Neurophysiological activations of mechanical engineering and industrial designers while designing and problem-solving. Design Science, 6, 1-35. https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2020.26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vieira, S., Benedek, M., Gero, J., Li, S., & Cascini, G. (2022). Design spaces and EEG frequency band power in constrained and open design. International Journal of Design Creativity and Innovation 1e28, https://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21650349.2022.2048697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, R., & Gero, J.S. (2020). The effect of digital design representation on designers' visual attention, Boess, S, Cheung, M and Cain, R, Proceedings DRS2020, Design Research Society, 2234-2244, https://dx.doi.org/10.21606/drs.2020.311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, S., Akioma, M. & Yuan, Z. (2021). Relationship between circadian rhythm and brain cognitive functions. Frontiers of Optoelectronics, 14, 278-287. https://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12200-021-1090-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed