Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-15T00:33:08.144Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Can eating harder textured foods slowly influence appetite?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 June 2020

Sara Megan Wallace
Affiliation:
Centre for Public Health, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, United Kingdom
Sinead Watson
Affiliation:
Centre for Public Health, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, United Kingdom
Martin Schimmel
Affiliation:
Division of Gerodontology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland. Division of Gerodontology and Removable Prosthodontics, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Ciaran Forde
Affiliation:
Clinical Nutrition Research Centre, A*STAR Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore, Singapore
Gerry McKenna
Affiliation:
Centre for Public Health, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, United Kingdom
Jayne Woodside
Affiliation:
Centre for Public Health, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, United Kingdom
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.
Introduction

Chewing a greater number of chews per bite has been shown to increase fullness, lower hunger levels and lead to a lower energy intake, when compared to chewing each bite fewer times. Increased levels of fullness and decreased levels of hunger have also been observed after consuming harder textured foods which require more chewing activity and have a longer oro-sensory exposure time. The aim of this study was to investigate whether consumption of foods differing in texture, classified as ‘fast’ or ‘slow’ foods, combined with differences in oral processing behaviours (chewing normally or more slowly), has an impact on self-reported hunger, fullness and satiety outcomes.

Materials and Methods

This is an interim analysis of a randomised crossover experiment designed to test the impact of both oral processing behaviours and food texture on self-reported hunger, fullness and satiety. Participants consumed two breakfasts of different texture (a ‘fast’ yoghurt and fruit compote option, and a ‘slow’ granola option) twice; once at their normal rate of eating and once after being instructed to eat slowly. Measures of self-reported appetite (hunger, fullness, satiety, desire to eat and prospective food consumption) were assessed using a visual analogue scale at three time points; baseline (minimum 20 minutes before consumption), immediately pre-consumption and post-consumption. Statistical analysis was conducted in SPSS; paired sample t tests and one-way analysis of variance was conducted to measure whether change in appetite ratings between the time points differed between the four breakfast options.

Results

Data were collected from 16 healthy volunteers (study aiming to recruit n = 21; mean age 32.1 y, female n = 13, 81.3%). For all four breakfast options, levels of hunger, desire to eat and prospective food consumption significantly decreased, and levels of fullness and satiety significantly increased between both baseline and post-consumption, (p < 0.05) and between pre-consumption and post-consumption (p < 0.05). However, amongst the four breakfast options, no significant differences were observed in change in these appetite measures between baseline and post-consumption and pre- and post-consumption (p > 0.05).

Discussion

This interim analysis demonstrates no differences in mean change in appetite measures for breakfast options of different texture chewed normally or more slowly. Analysis of the full study will determine the effect of oral processing behaviours and food texture on self-reported hunger, fullness and satiety outcomes.

Type
Abstract
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2020