Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-19T11:28:40.752Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Alcohol milestones and internalizing, externalizing, and executive function: longitudinal and polygenic score associations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 May 2024

Sarah E. Paul*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
David A.A. Baranger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
Emma C. Johnson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Joshua J. Jackson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
Aaron J. Gorelik
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
Alex P. Miller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Alexander S. Hatoum
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
Wesley K. Thompson
Affiliation:
Population Neuroscience and Genetics (PNG) Center, Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA
Michael Strube
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
Danielle M. Dick
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, USA Rutgers Addiction Research Center, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, USA
Chella Kamarajan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
John R. Kramer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Carver College of Medicine, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
Martin H. Plawecki
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Indiana University, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Grace Chan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Carver College of Medicine, The University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Connecticut, Farmington, CT, USA
Andrey P. Anokhin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
David B. Chorlian
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
Sivan Kinreich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
Jacquelyn L. Meyers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
Bernice Porjesz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University, Brooklyn, NY, USA
Howard J. Edenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Medical and Molecular Genetics, School of Medicine, Indiana University, Indianapolis, IN, USA Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, School of Medicine, Indiana University, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Arpana Agrawal
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Kathleen K. Bucholz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Ryan Bogdan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA
*
Corresponding author: Sarah E. Paul; Email: spaul24@wustl.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Although the link between alcohol involvement and behavioral phenotypes (e.g. impulsivity, negative affect, executive function [EF]) is well-established, the directionality of these associations, specificity to stages of alcohol involvement, and extent of shared genetic liability remain unclear. We estimate longitudinal associations between transitions among alcohol milestones, behavioral phenotypes, and indices of genetic risk.

Methods

Data came from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (n = 3681; ages 11–36). Alcohol transitions (first: drink, intoxication, alcohol use disorder [AUD] symptom, AUD diagnosis), internalizing, and externalizing phenotypes came from the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. EF was measured with the Tower of London and Visual Span Tasks. Polygenic scores (PGS) were computed for alcohol-related and behavioral phenotypes. Cox models estimated associations among PGS, behavior, and alcohol milestones.

Results

Externalizing phenotypes (e.g. conduct disorder symptoms) were associated with future initiation and drinking problems (hazard ratio (HR)⩾1.16). Internalizing (e.g. social anxiety) was associated with hazards for progression from first drink to severe AUD (HR⩾1.55). Initiation and AUD were associated with increased hazards for later depressive symptoms and suicidal ideation (HR⩾1.38), and initiation was associated with increased hazards for future conduct symptoms (HR = 1.60). EF was not associated with alcohol transitions. Drinks per week PGS was linked with increased hazards for alcohol transitions (HR⩾1.06). Problematic alcohol use PGS increased hazards for suicidal ideation (HR = 1.20).

Conclusions

Behavioral markers of addiction vulnerability precede and follow alcohol transitions, highlighting dynamic, bidirectional relationships between behavior and emerging addiction.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Excessive alcohol use is a major global health concern, typically beginning in adolescence and accounting for over 5% of global death and disability and 20% of deaths of those aged 20–49 in the U.S. (Esser et al., Reference Esser, Leung, Sherk, Bohm, Liu, Lu and Naimi2022; Forouzanfar et al., Reference Forouzanfar, Afshin, Alexander, Anderson, Bhutta, Biryukov and Murray2016). Alcohol use disorder (AUD) is among the most prevalent (29.1% lifetime) and comorbid psychopathologies (Castillo-Carniglia, Keyes, Hasin, & Cerdá, Reference Castillo-Carniglia, Keyes, Hasin and Cerdá2019). At the same time, few receive treatment (7.6%–19.8%; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang and Hasin2015; Olfson, Blanco, Wall, Liu, & Grant, Reference Olfson, Blanco, Wall, Liu and Grant2019), and there are high relapse rates among those who do (Moos & Moos, Reference Moos and Moos2006). Despite appreciable advances in AUD research (Koob & Volkow, Reference Koob and Volkow2016; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020), differentiation between the causes and consequences of AUD as it progresses is incomplete. Given the substantial burden of alcohol involvement, it is of critical public health interest to better understand the developmental course of alcohol use and AUD in relationship to genetic liability and behavioral precipitants and/or sequelae.

The neurobiological stage-based model of addiction postulates that alcohol-induced alterations in neural circuits promote three addiction stages—Binge/Intoxication, Withdrawal/Negative Affect, and Preoccupation/Anticipation (Koob & Volkow, Reference Koob and Volkow2010, Reference Koob and Volkow2016)—that broadly correspond with impulsivity/incentive salience, negative emotionality, and reduced executive function, respectively. Stimulation of neural reward circuits positively reinforces escalating use and potentiates impulsive behaviors (Binge/Intoxication). Following continued heavy use, alcohol intake becomes predominantly negatively reinforcing and compulsive, functioning to alleviate negative affect and withdrawal (Withdrawal/Negative Affect). Repeated alcohol-reward and alcohol-relief pairings beget craving and preoccupation with alcohol use (Preoccupation/Anticipation) and a loss of control over alcohol use arising from impaired prefrontal control of circuits that drive reward- and affect-related processes (Heather, Reference Heather2020; Koob & Volkow, Reference Koob and Volkow2010, Reference Koob and Volkow2016).

Other models of addiction emphasize the predispositional role of traits aligned with these addiction stages (i.e. impulsivity, negative emotionality, and EF) in the initial use of alcohol and development of problems. The neurodevelopmental model theorizes that typical patterns of uneven brain development during adolescence and young adulthood drive developmentally normative increases in impulsivity, negative affect, and urgency, as well as reduced executive control, that confer vulnerability to the positive and negative reinforcing properties of alcohol and to preoccupation (Casey, Heller, Gee, & Cohen, Reference Casey, Heller, Gee and Cohen2019; Casey & Jones, Reference Casey and Jones2010; Heller, Cohen, Dreyfuss, & Casey, Reference Heller, Cohen, Dreyfuss and Casey2016; Rapuano, Rosenberg, & Maza, Reference Rapuano, Rosenberg and Maza2020). Broadly, models of AUD suggest that trait-like differences in impulsivity, negative affect, and EF may indicate predispositional risk for AUD development and/or consequences of alcohol exposure (Bogdan, Hatoum, Johnson, & Agrawal, Reference Bogdan, Hatoum, Johnson and Agrawal2023; Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Dick, King, Ray, Sher, Vena and Acion2020; Kwako, Momenan, Litten, Koob, & Goldman, Reference Kwako, Momenan, Litten, Koob and Goldman2016; Littlefield, Stevens, & Sher, Reference Littlefield, Stevens and Sher2014; Rawls, Kummerfeld, & Zilverstand, Reference Rawls, Kummerfeld and Zilverstand2021). However, few studies have evaluated longitudinal bidirectional relationships between AUD stages and these ‘stage-based’ behavioral indicators.

Bidirectional associations between behavior and alcohol involvement

Extensive longitudinal research shows that elevated impulsivity and related clinical phenotypes (e.g. ADHD) prospectively predict alcohol involvement (e.g. initiation, consumption, problematic use; Chassin, Pitts, & Prost, Reference Chassin, Pitts and Prost2002; Defoe, Khurana, Betancourt, Hurt, & Romer, Reference Defoe, Khurana, Betancourt, Hurt and Romer2022; Elkins, King, McGue, & Iacono, Reference Elkins, King, McGue and Iacono2006; Farmer et al., Reference Farmer, Gau, Seeley, Kosty, Sher and Lewinsohn2016; Fergusson, Horwood, & Ridder, Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Ridder2007; King, Iacono, & McGue, Reference King, Iacono and McGue2004; Kuperman et al., Reference Kuperman, Chan, Kramer, Wetherill, Bucholz, Dick and Schuckit2013; Quinn & Harden, Reference Quinn and Harden2013; Stautz & Cooper, Reference Stautz and Cooper2013; Wright & Jackson, Reference Wright and Jackson2023), in line with neurodevelopmental theories that impulsivity poses vulnerability to the initial seeking and rewarding aspects of alcohol use. A smaller body of work in large, prospective samples of youth and young adults suggests that alcohol use may be linked with increases in impulsive and externalizing phenotypes, possibly reflecting alcohol-induced neurocognitive impairment, reward learning, and/or shared genetic predisposition, consistent with the neurobiological stage-based model (Quinn, Stappenbeck, & Fromme, Reference Quinn, Stappenbeck and Fromme2011; Staff, Maggs, Bucci, & Mongilio, Reference Staff, Maggs, Bucci and Mongilio2019).

The relationship between internalizing phenotypes and alcohol involvement is more nuanced. With few exceptions (Ning, Gondek, Patalay, & Ploubidis, Reference Ning, Gondek, Patalay and Ploubidis2020), internalizing has not been strongly linked to early alcohol involvement (e.g. initiation, intoxication) but has been prospectively linked to more severe alcohol engagement (e.g. AUD, relapse), with evidence that alcohol involvement may also potentiate internalizing symptoms (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017; Elkins et al., Reference Elkins, King, McGue and Iacono2006; Farmer et al., Reference Farmer, Gau, Seeley, Kosty, Sher and Lewinsohn2016; Flórez-Salamanca et al., Reference Flórez-Salamanca, Secades-Villa, Hasin, Cottler, Wang, Grant and Blanco2013; King et al., Reference King, Feil, Halvorson, Kosterman, Bailey and Hawkins2020; McCabe, Brumback, Brown, & Meruelo, Reference McCabe, Brumback, Brown and Meruelo2023; Menary, Corbin, & Chassin, Reference Menary, Corbin and Chassin2017). Interestingly, some evidence suggests that internalizing symptoms and behavioral inhibition may initially protect against alcohol use in early adolescence (e.g. via reduced social use opportunities), but then confer risk for more rapid progression of use later when more opportunities to drink arise (Hussong, Ennett, Cox, & Haroon, Reference Hussong, Ennett, Cox and Haroon2017; Ning et al., Reference Ning, Gondek, Patalay and Ploubidis2020).

Deficits in executive function (EF) often act as premorbid vulnerability factors that may promote initial alcohol-seeking and problematic use, and worsen alongside escalating severity of AUD, as proposed in the neurobiological theory. For example, EF deficits (e.g. in response inhibition) may hamper regulation of impulsive urges to engage in risky drinking (Jones, Meier, Corbin, & Chassin, Reference Jones, Meier, Corbin and Chassin2021). In a large longitudinal study of adolescents, associations between alcohol use and impairments in working memory and inhibitory control were attributable to common vulnerability rather than neurotoxicity (Morin et al., Reference Morin, Afzali, Bourque, Stewart, Séguin, O'leary-Barrett and Conrod2019). Although performance in some domains of cognition shows evidence of recovery following abstinence, impairments in working memory and inhibitory control appear to persist (Nowakowska-Domagała, Jabłkowska-Górecka, Mokros, Koprowicz, & Pietras, Reference Nowakowska-Domagała, Jabłkowska-Górecka, Mokros, Koprowicz and Pietras2017; Stavro, Pelletier, & Potvin, Reference Stavro, Pelletier and Potvin2013; but see Pitel et al., Reference Pitel, Rivier, Beaunieux, Vabret, Desgranges and Eustache2009). It may be that executive dysfunction confers vulnerability to escalating use following initiation and worsens in some domains as use becomes chronic and problematic.

In sum, prominent theories of AUD development endeavor to explain its associations with impulsivity, negative affect, and EF. Existing longitudinal studies provide some evidence of bidirectional associations, with some specificity (e.g. internalizing more strongly linked to later milestones). Longitudinal studies that not only measure these traits both before and after relevant alcohol milestones but also incorporate indicators of genetic liability can be further used to parse the directionality and potential shared etiology of these relationships, especially when considering potentially censored observations (e.g. later expression of internalizing symptoms, limited phenotypic expression of high genetic risk).

Genetic contributions to associations between behavioral phenotypes and alcohol involvement

There is robust support for heritable influences on alcohol use and AUD (twin heritability ~30–40% and 50%, respectively; Koopmans, Slutske, van Baal, & Boomsma, Reference Koopmans, Slutske, van Baal and Boomsma1999; Verhulst, Neale, & Kendler, Reference Verhulst, Neale and Kendler2015; Viken, Kaprio, Koskenvuo, & Rose, Reference Viken, Kaprio, Koskenvuo and Rose1999), as well as for impulsivity, risk-taking, negative urgency, depression, and EF (Anokhin, Grant, Mulligan, & Heath, Reference Anokhin, Grant, Mulligan and Heath2015; Flint & Kendler, Reference Flint and Kendler2014; Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008; Gustavson et al., Reference Gustavson, Friedman, Fontanillas, Elson, Palmer and Sanchez-Roige2020), with well-powered genome-wide association studies (GWAS) identifying loci that contribute to this heritable variation (Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Morrison, Mitchell, Lam, Benca-Bachman, Reineberg and Friedman2023; Karlsson Linnér et al., Reference Karlsson Linnér, Biroli, Kong, Meddens, Wedow, Fontana and Beauchamp2019; Levey et al., Reference Levey, Stein, Wendt, Pathak, Zhou, Aslan and Gelernter2021; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020). Incorporating estimates of this genetic liability into theoretical frameworks and analytic models of the relationships between these phenotypes can help clarify the extent to which their association is explained by shared genetic predisposition. Indeed, the Genetically Informed Neurobiological model of Addiction (GINA; Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Hatoum, Johnson and Agrawal2023) posits that genetically influenced individual differences in impulsivity/risk-taking, negative urgency and depression, and EF confer differential vulnerability to the initial seeking and acutely rewarding aspects of substances, heightened negative affect with chronic use, and difficulty engaging in goal-directed behavior when salient substance-related cues elicit craving.

There is some evidence that genetic correlations and associations with polygenic scores (PGS) show stronger genetic relationships between externalizing and alcohol initiation/consumption and between internalizing and problematic use. Problematic alcohol use, but not consumption, is genetically correlated with indices of psychopathology (Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Palmer, Fontanillas, Elson, Adams, Howard and Wilson2019; Walters et al., Reference Walters, Adams, Adkins, Aliev, Bacanu, Batzler and Lichtenstein2018; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020). Evidence further shows opposing genetic correlations in which alcohol frequency and quantity are associated with reduced and increased risk for psychopathology, respectively (Mallard et al., Reference Mallard, Savage, Johnson, Huang, Edwards, Hottenga and Sanchez-Roige2022; Marees et al., Reference Marees, Smit, Ong, Macgregor, An, Denys and Derks2020; Sanchez-Roige et al., Reference Sanchez-Roige, Palmer, Fontanillas, Elson, Adams, Howard and Wilson2019). PGS for risk-taking is associated with future alcohol consumption (Ksinan, Su, Aliev, Spit for Science Workgroup, & Dick, Reference Ksinan, Su, Aliev and Dick2019), while PGS for major depression is related to problematic use and dependence (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Pietrzak, Kranzler, Ma, Zhou, Liu and Han2017; Casey et al., Reference Casey, Heller, Gee and Cohen2019). Despite the currently limited clinical utility of PGS (Rodrigue et al., Reference Rodrigue, Mathias, Knowles, Mollon, Almasy, Schultz and Glahn2023), investigating whether patterns of association between behavioral stage-based PGS and alcohol milestones, and, reciprocally, between alcohol PGS and stage-based symptom onsets, mirror phenotypic associations would yield important insight into the nature of these relationships. For instance, identifying PGS associations in the context of no symptoms or subthreshold symptoms poorly captured by categorical assessment would highlight the plausibility of shared genetic liability that may not yet be phenotypically expressed at the level of measurement.

Current study

We assessed whether non-substance psychiatric representations of stage-based addiction phenotypes confer risk for progression of alcohol milestones and/or arise following these milestones. We leverage the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) Prospective Study, a large (n = 3681), deeply phenotyped, and prospectively assessed cohort of individuals aged 12–21 at wave 1 to examine prospective and bidirectional associations between stage-based phenotypes (i.e. externalizing, internalizing, and EF) and progression across alcohol milestones (i.e. first drink, first intoxication, first AUD symptom(s), first AUD diagnosis; Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017). We use psychopathology symptom onsets as proxies for stage-based phenotypes for several reasons:

  1. (1) Patterns of comorbidity between AUD and externalizing and internalizing disorders parallel links between stages of alcohol involvement and the core constructs of impulsivity and negative affect, respectively, implicated in these disorders;

  2. (2) Retrospectively reported onsets of such symptoms are carefully defined in this cohort, whereas continuous measures of impulsivity and negative affect may not be assessed before alcohol milestones;

  3. (3) Whether distinct subtypes of internalizing and externalizing disorders are specifically and bidirectionally related to alcohol involvement in ways consistent with the stage-based model has not yet been comprehensively explored; and

  4. (4) Focusing on these clinical phenotypes facilitates translation to clinical practice and integration with genetic evidence of shared liability revealed from GWAS of clinical phenotypes.

Consistent with the last point, we also examine whether PGS of psychiatric stage-based phenotypes are related to specific alcohol milestones and whether PGS of alcohol consumption and problematic use are differentially related to these stage-based phenotypes. We hypothesize that externalizing phenotypes will be broadly and reciprocally associated with all alcohol milestones, and that internalizing and EF will be primarily bidirectionally associated with later/more severe alcohol stages.

Methods

Sample

The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) is an ongoing multi-site, family-based study designed to identify genes and traits associated with AUD (total N~17 762; Dick et al., Reference Dick, Balcke, McCutcheon, Francis, Kuo, Salvatore and Bucholz2023; Edenberg, Reference Edenberg2002; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Salvatore, Lai, Merikangas, Nurnberger, Tischfield and Agrawal2023; Reich et al., Reference Reich, Edenberg, Goate, Williams, Rice, Van Eerdewegh and Begleiter1998). The COGA Prospective (COGA-P) Study (n = 3715) is a longitudinal sub-study of adolescent and young adult offspring with at least one parent who participated in the original COGA study and provided permission for offspring participation (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017). All adult participants provided written informed consent, and minors verbal assent, to a protocol approved by each site's institutional review board. Following quality control (Supplemental Methods), our final analytic sample included up to 15 463 observations from 3681 participants from 2186 nuclear families in 910 extended family pedigrees (51.3% female, 63.9% white, 27.4% Black, mean ± s.d. age at wave 1 = 16.02 ± 3.31 years; Table 1). Of these, 1935 and 918 are of European and African-American ancestry, respectively.

Table 1. Participant characteristics

AUD, alcohol use disorder.

a Native American, Asian, Pacific Islander, or ‘any other identified group.’.

Note: Characterization of COGA prospective study participants (total n = 3681).

Measures

Substance involvement and psychiatric assessment

Alcohol transitions and psychopathology onsets were derived from the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism interview (SSAGA; Bucholz, Cadoret, & Cloninger, Reference Bucholz, Cadoret and Cloninger1994), which was administered approximately every 2 years (4.19 ± 2.07 times over 7.36 ± 4.40 years) to assess alcohol and substance use, problems, and disorders, as well as other psychiatric disorders (Table 2).

Table 2. Participant SSAGA and executive function characteristics

SSAGA, semi-structured assessment for the genetics of alcoholism. Major depressive episode, age most severe MDE OR first MDE; social anxiety disorder, first time symptoms made the individual upset with themselves; panic disorder, first time panic attack with 4 + symptoms; agoraphobia, first time fear with several other problems; suicidal ideation, first time had thoughts of taking own life; ADHD inattentive, first time 5 + ADHD inattentive symptoms with functional impairment; ADHD hyperactive, first time 5 + ADHD hyperactive symptoms with functional impairment; oppositional defiant disorder, first period of 6 + months with several symptoms; conduct disorder, first age at which individual experienced 3 + conduct disorder symptoms; cigarette use, first full cigarette; marijuana use, first marijuana use; ToL, Tower of London Test; VST, Visual Span Test.

a These n's and descriptive statistics are for executive function measured at wave 1 (i.e. baseline wave).

Note: Characterization of stage-based and alcohol phenotypes. All numbers reflect lifetime status within the study timespan.

Alcohol Milestones/Transitions. Alcohol milestones were defined as the age of the first: whole drink, intoxication, DSM-5 AUD symptom(s), and DSM-5 AUD diagnosis (Supplemental Methods). Consistent with prior COGA-P work (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017; McCutcheon et al., Reference McCutcheon, Agrawal, Kuo, Su, Dick, Meyers and Bucholz2018), transitions between these milestones (dichotomously coded 0/1 at each year within-person) were defined as follows: age 0 to first drink, first drink to first intoxication, first drink to first AUD symptom, and first drink to first AUD diagnosis (Table 2).

Given that age of first intoxication may be a better predictor of mental health outcomes (Newton-Howes, Cook, Martin, Foulds, & Boden, Reference Newton-Howes, Cook, Martin, Foulds and Boden2019), supplemental analyses defined the origin as first intoxication when predicting first AUD symptom and diagnosis (Supplemental Methods).

Externalizing and Internalizing. The SSAGA inquires about ages of the first experienced impairing symptoms for each disorder examined. Externalizing (EXT) phenotypes were defined as the onset of: ADHD symptoms before age 12 (i.e. inattentive, hyperactive), persistent Oppositional Defiant Disorder symptoms, and ⩾3 Conduct Disorder symptoms. Internalizing (INT) phenotypes included the onsets of: a Major Depressive Episode, distressing Social Anxiety symptoms, Panic Attack accompanied by ⩾4 symptoms, core agoraphobia fear accompanied by additional agoraphobia symptoms, and suicidal ideation (Supplemental Methods). All EXT and INT phenotypes were dichotomous variables coded as 0/1 at each year, with ‘1’ indicating the onset of symptoms.

Executive function

The Tower of London Test (Shallice, Reference Shallice1982) and Visual Span Test (Milner, Reference Milner1971) were used to assess executive function (i.e. planning and problem solving and visuospatial short-term memory, respectively; Supplemental Methods). Tower of London variables included: total excess moves made (errors), average pickup time (initial planning time), and average total time (total planning time). Visual Span Test variables included: highest sequences attained and total correct going forward and backward. For analyses predicting alcohol transitions, EF data came from the neuropsychological assessment conducted most recently prior to the alcohol transition (Supplement). No other neuropsychologically based cognitive assessments were collected in COGA-P.

Polygenic scores (PGS)

Details regarding the genomic data processing pipeline are provided in Supplemental Methods and in (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Salvatore, Lai, Merikangas, Nurnberger, Tischfield and Agrawal2023; Lai, Wetherill, & Bertelsen, Reference Lai, Wetherill and Bertelsen2019). PGS were computed in the COGA-P European ancestry (EA) subsample (n = 1935) using PRS-CS (Ge, Chen, Ni, Feng, & Smoller, Reference Ge, Chen, Ni, Feng and Smoller2019), and in the African-American ancestry (AFR) subsample (n = 918) using PRS-CSx (Ruan et al., Reference Ruan, Lin, Feng, Chen, Lam, Guo and Ge2022; Supplemental Methods).

Ancestry-specific discovery GWAS were used to generate PGS for the following phenotypes (Table 3): alcohol phenotypes (i.e. Drinks/Week (Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Wang, Chen, Jang, Liu, Wang and Vrieze2022), Problematic Alcohol Use (Kranzler et al., Reference Kranzler, Zhou, Kember, Vickers Smith, Justice, Damrauer and Gelernter2019; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020)), internalizing (i.e. major depressive disorder (Howard et al., Reference Howard, Adams, Clarke, Hafferty, Gibson, Shirali and McIntosh2019; Levey et al., Reference Levey, Stein, Wendt, Pathak, Zhou, Aslan and Gelernter2021), generalized anxiety disorder (Levey et al., Reference Levey, Gelernter, Polimanti, Zhou, Cheng, Aslan and Stein2020), neuroticism (Nagel et al., Reference Nagel, Jansen, Stringer, Watanabe, de Leeuw, Bryois and Posthuma2018)), externalizing (i.e. risk tolerance (Karlsson Linnér et al., Reference Karlsson Linnér, Biroli, Kong, Meddens, Wedow, Fontana and Beauchamp2019), ADHD (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Martin, Mattheisen, Als, Agerbo and Neale2019), common executive function (Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Morrison, Mitchell, Lam, Benca-Bachman, Reineberg and Friedman2023)). Because the present study examines stage-based behavioral phenotypes in relation to alcohol milestones, we do not use the recent, large externalizing GWAS (Karlsson Linnér et al., Reference Karlsson Linnér, Mallard, Barr, Sanchez-Roige, Madole, Driver and Dick2021) that includes alcohol and substance-related indicators. GWAS were selected based on sample size, relevance to phenotypes implicated in the neurobiological stage-based model, and similarity to the COGA-P phenotypes where possible (Supplemental Methods).

Table 3. Description of Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) used for polygenic score computation

h 2SNP, SNP-based heritability; AA, African Ancestry; EA, European Ancestry.

a Sample size excluding the COGA cohort from the discovery GWAS (n = 4386).

Covariates

All models (statistical analysis) covaried for: reported biological sex, highest parent educational attainment, parent household income, parent separation between the ages of 12 and 17, AUD case/control family status, parent AUD, birth cohort, and study site (Table 1), following prior work in this cohort (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017). The following analysis-specific covariates were included in relevant models: White, Black, and those endorsing neither (self-reported) and Hispanic/not Hispanic (self-reported) in non-genetic analyses (Table 1); cigarette and cannabis use initiation and dependence in non-genetic analyses; first 10 ancestral principal components in genetic analyses; age of first drink and first intoxication in models analyzing survival from these milestones to adjust for any variability in risk period (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017; Supplemental Methods).

Statistical analysis

Cox proportional hazards models with mixed effects (Cox, Reference Cox1972; Therneau, Reference Therneau2022) were used to generate hazard ratios (HR) for each predictor/outcome combination while including fixed effect covariates (above) and nuclear family as a random intercept to account for data non-independence. Models were run predicting: alcohol transitions from earlier EXT, INT, and EF phenotypes; EXT and INT symptom onsets from earlier alcohol milestones; alcohol transitions from EXT, INT, and EF PGS; and EXT and INT onsets from PGS for drinks/week and problematic alcohol use. Hazard ratio estimates required for 80% power ranged from 1.16 to 1.93 given sample sizes for models estimating survival to later stage-based symptom onsets excluding ADHD symptoms (see online Supplementary Methods; Supplementary Tables S14–S15). Due to insufficient numbers of events following alcohol milestones for agoraphobia and ADHD symptoms, these outcomes were excluded from analyses. Proportional hazards violations were investigated and resolved (Supplemental Methods).

Relationships between alcohol milestones and continuously assessed executive function (EF) development were assessed with linear mixed effects models (lme4 R package; Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015). Time-in-study was person-mean-centered and used to model linear and quadratic within-person trajectories of EF. Main effects of alcohol milestones (Supplemental Methods) and interactions with time-in-study were explored with respect to each EF dependent variable. Random slopes allowed for varying rates of (linear) change across individuals. Statistically significant omnibus tests for the overall interaction effects were followed by post hoc pairwise comparison tests. The same analytic process was repeated for PGS, with PGS of drinks per week and problematic alcohol use replacing the ‘milestone’ variable (Supplemental Methods).

Multiple testing correction

False discovery rate (FDR) correction (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) was used to adjust for multiple testing. p values were adjusted across all predictors within EXT, INT, and EF separately, and separately across all alcohol milestone predictors of stage-based outcomes. Similarly, FDR correction was used across all INT PGS, all EXT PGS, and the EF PGS in predicting alcohol transitions, and across all alcohol PGS in predicting stage-based symptom onsets. For models examining EF trajectories, FDR correction was employed for the omnibus tests for the main effect of milestone and milestone * age interactions (14 tests), and for the 14 omnibus tests for the main effect of alcohol PGS and PGS * age interactions.

Post hoc severity analyses

Given heterogeneity in AUD severity, post hoc analyses examined whether stage-based phenotypes were associated with hazards for the onset of mild (2–3 criteria), moderate (4–5 criteria), and/or severe (6 + criteria) AUD (Supplemental Methods).

Post hoc analyses stratified by ancestry

Given that PGS analyses were conducted within each ancestral group (due to poor portability of PGS prediction across ancestries; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Gignoux, Walters, Wojcik, Neale, Gravel and Kenny2017), phenotypic analyses were repeated in each ancestry group to facilitate comparison across phenotypic and PGS-based models (Supplemental Methods).

Results

Phenotypic associations

Ext, INT, and EF predicting alcohol transitions

Externalizing. Conduct disorder symptoms were associated with increased hazards for all subsequent alcohol transitions (HR = 1.19–1.39, p ⩽ 1.30×10−3, p FDR ⩽ 4.12×10−3; online Supplementary Table S1). ADHD hyperactive symptoms were also prospectively associated with increased hazards for all alcohol transitions (HR = 1.16–1.92, p ⩽ 0.014, p FDR ⩽ 0.027; online Supplementary Table S1) except time from initiation to first intoxication (HR = 1.02–1.15, p ⩾ 0.19). ADHD inattentive symptoms were not significantly associated with hazards for any alcohol transition (0.89⩽HR⩽1.05, p ⩾ 0.13). Oppositional defiant disorder symptoms were associated with increased hazards for subsequent alcohol initiation transition to first AUD diagnosis (HR⩾1.24, p ⩽ 3.80 × 10−3, p FDR = 9.03 × 10−3) but not other transitions (HR = 0.96–1.05, p ⩾ 0.18; Figure 1a; online Supplementary Table S1). Post hoc analyses of AUD severity showed that conduct disorder symptoms were linked with increased hazards of transitioning to moderate and severe AUD, hyperactive ADHD symptoms with transition to mild AUD, and oppositional defiant symptoms with transition to severe AUD (online Supplementary Fig. S1; Supplementary Table S2).

Figure 1. Associations between stage-based phenotypes and alcohol transitions. (a) Associations between internalizing and externalizing and hazards of alcohol transitions. Asterisks reflect estimates that survived FDR correction for all 20 tests for internalizing phenotypes, and separately, all 16 tests for externalizing phenotypes. Note three violations of the proportional hazards assumption for which time interactions were incorporated to resolve the violations: ADHD hyperactive symptoms in models of ‘First Drink’ and ‘First Drink to First Intoxication’ and ODD symptoms in the model of ‘First Drink to First Diagnosis.’ For simplicity, those interactions are not depicted here. All predictors were entered into the models simultaneously, alongside covariates. (b) Associations between executive function and hazards of alcohol transitions. ToL, Tower of London Test; VST, Visual Span Test. Note that for the ToL, higher scores reflect worse executive functioning, whereas the opposite is true for the VST. Hazard ratios for ToL measures are hazards associated with a doubling of the ToL measures, which were log2 transformed (Supplemental Methods). Each EF predictor was entered into analyses separately, given varying sample sizes and correlations among EF phenotypes. For both figures, point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Internalizing. INT was not significantly associated with hazards for any alcohol transitions (HR = 0.82–1.18, p ⩾ 0.043, p FDR ⩾ 0.53; Figure 1a; online Supplementary Table S1). However, post hoc analyses revealed that social anxiety symptoms and suicidal ideation were significantly associated with increased hazards for progression from first drink to first severe AUD (online Supplementary Fig. 1; Supplementary Table S2).

Executive function. EF was not significantly associated with any alcohol transition (HR = 0.97–1.28, p ⩾ 0.023, p FDR ⩾ 0.32; Figure 1b, online Supplementary Table S3).

Post hoc analyses. Supplementary analyses examining transitions from first intoxication to first AUD symptom and diagnosis were broadly consistent (online Supplementary Table S4).

Alcohol milestones predicting later EXT, INT, and EF

Alcohol initiation was significantly associated with increased hazards of subsequent onsets of INT and EXT (HR⩾1.44, p ⩽ 2.10×10−4, p FDR ⩽ 1.68×103), except for panic, social anxiety, and oppositional defiant disorder symptoms (HR⩽1.45, p ⩾ 0.047, pFDR ⩾ 0.147). Neither alcohol intoxication nor AUD symptom(s) was significantly associated with risk for any subsequent symptom onsets (0.42⩽HR⩽1.54, p ⩾ 0.031, pFDR ⩾ 0.12). AUD diagnosis was associated with increased hazards of a subsequent major depressive episode and suicidal ideation (HR⩾1.38, p ⩽ 5.10×10−3, pFDR = 0.024) but no other outcomes (HR = 0.81–1.38, p ⩾ 0.20; Figure 2; online Supplementary Table S5).

Figure 2. Associations between alcohol milestones and stage-based symptom onsets. Associations between alcohol milestones and hazards of stage-based symptom onsets. Point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks reflect estimates that survived FDR correction for all (24) tests. MDE, Major Depressive Episode; Panic, Panic Disorder Symptoms; Conduct, Conduct Disorder Symptoms; Oppositional, Oppositional Defiant Disorder Symptoms.

Executive function. No significant omnibus effects were detected between alcohol milestones and EF trajectories (online Supplementary Table S6).

Genomic ancestry subsamples: phenotypic associations

Among both the European- and African-American Ancestry subsamples, phenotypic associations were broadly consistent with pattern seen in the full sample (Supplementary Tables S7–S8).

Genetic associations

Stage-based phenotypes

No PGS was significantly associated with alcohol transitions (HR = 0.95–1.08, p ⩾ 0.035, p FDR ⩾ 0.29; Fig. 3; online Supplementary Table S9). Post hoc analyses showed that generalized anxiety disorder PGS was associated with faster progression from first intoxication to first AUD symptoms in the European ancestry subsample (HR = 1.10, p = 4.10 × 10−3, p FDR = 0.025; online Supplementary Table S10).

Figure 3. Associations between stage-based polygenic scores and alcohol transitions. Associations between stage-based PGS and hazards of alcohol transitions. EUR, PCA-selected European ancestry; AFR, PCA-selected African ancestry. Point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. AFR ancestry PGS associations are depicted with dotted lines. Alcohol PGS are included for comparison at the bottom of each plot.

Post hoc analyses of AUD severity showed that no PGS was significantly associated with hazards for AUD at any severity level in either ancestry (HR = 0.82–1.25, p ⩾ 6.60 × 10−3, p FDR ⩾ 0.059; online Supplementary Fig. S2, Supplementary Table S11).

Alcohol involvement PGS

Problematic Alcohol Use (PAU; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng and Gelernter2020) PGS was associated with increased hazards for suicidal ideation onset among individuals of European ancestry (HR = 1.20, p = 9.10 × 10−4, p FDR = 0.011) but not with other phenotypes (HR = 0.94–1.29, p ⩾ 0.081). Drinks/week PGS showed no significant associations (HR = 0.92–1.18, p ⩾ 0.023, p FDR ⩾ 0.14; Figure 4; online Supplementary Table S12). No significant omnibus effects were detected between alcohol involvement PGS and EF trajectories (online Supplementary Table S13). No significant omnibus effects were detected between alcohol involvement PGS and EF trajectories (online Supplementary Table S13).

Figure 4. Associations between alcohol polygenic scores and stage-based outcomes. Associations between alcohol PGS and hazards of stage-based outcomes. Point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Solid lines represent PGS in EUR ancestry, and dashed lines represent PGS in AFR ancestry Asterisks reflect estimates that survived FDR correction for all 12 tests (2 alcohol PGS × 6 outcomes) in EUR ancestry. Note that, among individuals of AFR ancestry, models predicting panic symptoms, social anxiety symptoms, and oppositional defiant symptoms did not converge, so estimates may be unreliable.

Discussion

Our study estimated longitudinal relationships between alcohol milestones/transitions (i.e. first drink, intoxication, AUD symptom, AUD diagnosis) and internalizing (INT), externalizing (EXT), and executive function (EF) phenotypes. By examining reciprocal time-varying associations, these findings critically extend the literature by clarifying the directionality and specificity of alcohol-psychopathology associations from early adolescence through young adulthood and the extent to which genetic liability maps onto phenotypic associations. Four broad findings emerged. First, EXT (i.e. ADHD hyperactive, oppositional defiant, and/or conduct disorder symptoms) was prospectively associated with increased hazards for all alcohol transitions. In contrast, only alcohol initiation prospectively predicted later EXT. Second, some INT phenotypes (i.e. social anxiety, suicidal ideation) were prospectively associated only with subsequent transitions to severe AUD. Alcohol milestones were prospectively associated with an accelerated risk for depression and suicidal ideation, but not panic or social anxiety symptoms. Third, EF (i.e. Tower of London Test, Visual Span Test) was not associated with hazards for any alcohol transition, nor were alcohol milestones associated with EF trajectories. Fourth, greater problematic alcohol use PGS was associated with increased hazards for suicidal ideation and the onset of conduct disorder symptoms; however, PGS for behavioral phenotypes were not significantly associated with alcohol transition hazards. Although the timing of typical developmental onsets and progressions of EXT, INT, and AUD may influence these longitudinal associations, these findings suggest that EXT symptomatology may promote accelerations across all stages of alcohol involvement while early INT may drive progression only to severe AUD and INT is more likely to arise following AUD.

Externalizing symptoms are bidirectionally associated with alcohol milestones

As expected, EXT was robustly associated with all subsequent alcohol transitions, and earlier alcohol involvement was prospectively associated with increased hazards for EXT onset. Conduct disorder symptoms exhibited the most consistent associations with all alcohol transitions, and alcohol initiation increased hazards for conduct disorder symptoms. ADHD hyperactive—but not inattentive—symptoms were associated with all transitions except progression from initiation to intoxication and from intoxication to first AUD symptom. Oppositional defiant disorder symptoms were linked with increased hazards for future alcohol initiation, but not vice versa. These findings align with prior work showing that ‘disruptive behavior disorders'’ are associated with shorter time to alcohol problems and AUD onset (Edwards, Gardner, Hickman, & Kendler, Reference Edwards, Gardner, Hickman and Kendler2016; Farmer et al., Reference Farmer, Gau, Seeley, Kosty, Sher and Lewinsohn2016), and that childhood/adolescent conduct symptoms and hyperactive—but not inattentive—ADHD symptoms are prospectively associated with later substance involvement (Elkins et al., Reference Elkins, Saunders, Malone, Keyes, McGue and Iacono2018; Fergusson et al., Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Ridder2007). Studies in young adults have documented associations between inattentive symptoms, which tend to be stable over time, and alcohol use and AUD (Glass & Flory, Reference Glass and Flory2012; Grazioli et al., Reference Grazioli, Gmel, Rougemont-Bücking, Baggio, Daeppen and Studer2019; Roberts, Peters, Adams, Lynam, & Milich, Reference Roberts, Peters, Adams, Lynam and Milich2014). Hyperactivity typically decreases with age (Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, Reference Biederman, Mick and Faraone2000). In childhood, hyperactivity may show stronger associations with alcohol milestones/transitions, but in late adolescence or adulthood, inattention may appear more relevant. These findings suggest that externalizing is a more robust risk factor for alcohol initiation and escalation than the reverse, and that associations between alcohol milestones and subsequent EXT are restricted to early alcohol involvement. Of course, this pattern might be influenced by the typical development of EXT at earlier ages than problematic alcohol use (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005), or these associations might be due to shared underlying disposition that has variable expression across time (Hicks, Foster, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Hicks, Foster, Iacono and McGue2013; Karlsson Linnér et al., Reference Karlsson Linnér, Mallard, Barr, Sanchez-Roige, Madole, Driver and Dick2021).

Internalizing symptoms predict transitions to severe AUD, but not other alcohol transitions

Unlike associations with externalizing, internalizing symptoms were not associated with hazards for subsequent alcohol transitions. These results diverge from prior work in COGA-P showing that the presence of any internalizing disorder is associated with time to first AUD diagnosis (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, McCutcheon, Agrawal, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer and Porjesz2017). Sample sizes for certain individual disorders may have resulted in underpowered analyses, or perhaps an overarching internalizing domain confers more risk than specific disorders. However, post-hoc analyses painted a more nuanced picture; suicidal ideation and social anxiety were associated with increased hazards for progression from initiation to severe—but not mild or moderate—AUD. This pattern of results is consistent with recent analyses showing that AUD criteria mapping onto the Withdrawal/Negative Affect and Preoccupation/Anticipation stages are more frequently endorsed by those with severe AUD (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Kuo, Johnson, Tillman, Brislin, Dick and Agrawal2023) and that anxiety and depression are linked with alcohol dependence symptoms more strongly than with initiation or quantity/frequency measures (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Nasim, Edwards, Salvatore, Cho, Adkins and Kendler2014). Suicidal ideation has been cross-sectionally linked to more severe problematic alcohol use (Crossin et al., Reference Crossin, Cleland, McLeod, Beautrais, Witt and Boden2022), and suicide attempt has been longitudinally linked to future DSM-IV alcohol dependence in the COGA prospective cohort (Agrawal et al., Reference Agrawal, Tillman, Grucza, Nelson, McCutcheon, Few and Bucholz2017); present findings suggest that in a high-risk sample, suicidal ideation may escalate progression of AUD.

Social anxiety disorder symptoms were associated with a substantial increase in hazards for severe AUD, possibly attesting to vulnerability to high levels of alcohol-related problems once drinking is initiated and/or to strong negatively reinforcing effects of alcohol use on social anxiety. Prior work suggests that social anxiety may be uniquely, and possibly causally, related to AUD (Torvik et al., Reference Torvik, Rosenström, Gustavson, Ystrom E, Kendler, Bramness and Reichborn-Kjennerud2019), and that it is more strongly related to alcohol dependence or related problems than to DSM-IV abuse or consumption (Buckner et al., Reference Buckner, Schmidt, Lang, Small, Schlauch and Lewinsohn2008; Schry & White, Reference Schry and White2013).

Alcohol milestones potentiate internalizing onsets

The accelerated development of depression and suicidal ideation following alcohol initiation and AUD onset, coupled with the absence of the reverse relationship, may be attributed to several factors. First, initial alcohol use and escalation may increase negative affect as alcohol use becomes more compulsive and non-alcohol rewards become less reinforcing, consistent with the stage-based model (Koob, Reference Koob2015; Koob & Volkow, Reference Koob and Volkow2010, Reference Koob and Volkow2016). Prior work showed that the risk of depression onset following alcohol dependence is higher than the risk of alcohol dependence following depression (Fergusson, Boden, & Horwood, Reference Fergusson, Boden and Horwood2009; Gilman & Abraham, Reference Gilman and Abraham2001; but see Polimanti et al., Reference Polimanti, Peterson, Ong, MacGregor, Edwards, Clarke and Derks2019). Second, internalizing may be unrelated to or protective against alcohol use, depending on developmental timing and/or levels of concurrent externalizing symptoms. Internalizing has been linked with future problematic alcohol use in adulthood but not adolescence (King et al., Reference King, Feil, Halvorson, Kosterman, Bailey and Hawkins2020), and internalizing symptoms may confer more risk at low levels of EXT (Colder et al., Reference Colder, Scalco, Trucco, Read, Lengua, Wieczorek and Hawk2013, Reference Colder, Shyhalla, Frndak, Read, Lengua, Hawk and Wieczorek2017). Third, early initiation and development of AUD may be linked with negative social, academic, and functional outcomes that increase risk for depression and suicidality (Miller, DiBello, Merrill, Neighbors, & Carey, Reference Miller, DiBello, Merrill, Neighbors and Carey2020; Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Clark, Marshall, Buka, Carey, Shepardson and Carey2018). Fourth, gender differences may obscure relationships between internalizing and subsequent alcohol transitions, with prior work suggesting that early depressive symptoms may be associated with increased risk for later alcohol use and related harms more strongly in girls than boys (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Heron, Dick, Hickman, Lewis, Macleod and Kendler2014). Finally, we note that the onset of a major depressive episode in the SSAGA was not defined to be independent of drug or alcohol use, which is important to distinguish in future work (Schuckit, Smith, & Kalmijn, Reference Schuckit, Smith and Kalmijn2013).

Polygenic scores for stage-based phenotypes are not associated with alcohol transitions

In contrast to phenotypic associations, and despite evidence for shared genetic liability to externalizing and AUD (Karlsson Linnér et al., Reference Karlsson Linnér, Mallard, Barr, Sanchez-Roige, Madole, Driver and Dick2021; Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, Reference Kendler, Prescott, Myers and Neale2003), PGS for externalizing phenotypes were not significantly linked to hazards for alcohol transitions. GWAS of externalizing phenotypes (excluding substance-related variables) may be underpowered for the detection of cross-trait relationships with PGS (Carey, Agrawal, & Bucholz, Reference Carey, Agrawal and Bucholz2016; Kember et al., Reference Kember, Merikangas, Verma, Verma, Judy, Damrauer and Mitnaul2021). Perhaps other stage-based phenotypes (e.g. negative urgency, inhibitory control), for which there are not yet GWAS, would exhibit more predictive utility.

Executive function measures are not associated with alcohol milestones

Although impairments in executive function are thought to be a consequence of prolonged alcohol use and AUD (Koob & Volkow, Reference Koob and Volkow2010, Reference Koob and Volkow2016), the present study found that performance on two executive function tasks was unassociated with alcohol milestones as both predictor and outcome. Few studies have previously explored the relationship between alcohol consumption or AUD and performance on these tasks, with mixed findings (Hendriks, van de Rest, Snippe, Kieboom, & Hogenelst, Reference Hendriks, van de Rest, Snippe, Kieboom and Hogenelst2020; Meyers et al., Reference Meyers, Chorlian, Johnson, Pandey, Kamarajan, Salvatore and Porjesz2019; Pandey et al., Reference Pandey, Ardekani, Kamarajan, Zhang, Chorlian, Byrne and Porjesz2018). Explanations for the present findings include the restriction of EF analyses to individuals with EF assessments that preceded milestone onsets (i.e. those that experienced milestones later, on average, may exhibit a lower constellation of risk factors) and the lack of measures of inhibitory control and other EF components more relevant to AUD.

Strengths and limitations

The present study has notable strengths. Important alcohol transitions were examined, from first drink to the development of AUD. Alongside these transitions, behavioral indicators were examined both phenotypically and using PGS. Internalizing and externalizing phenotypes were measured by multiple discrete symptoms, allowing for fine-grained analysis that shows that social anxiety, suicidal ideation, conduct disorder, and hyperactive ADHD symptoms may be uniquely related to alcohol milestones. By examining bidirectional associations, a truly developmental model was adopted that begins to capture the dynamic process by which AUD and comorbid symptoms develop over time, with implications for intervention and etiologic understanding.

Some limitations are worth noting. First, although high-risk family-based samples have several benefits including enriched endorsement for AUD and other psychopathology, they may not generalize. Second, the PGS analyses were largely constrained to individuals of European ancestry due to the dearth of GWAS of these phenotypes in individuals of non-European ancestry and the poor portability of PGS prediction across ancestries (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Gignoux, Walters, Wojcik, Neale, Gravel and Kenny2017). Despite our efforts to include PGS for those of African–American ancestry, the present null findings may be partially attributable to reduced power arising from smaller discovery GWASs in African ancestry populations. Our European ancestry-specific findings may thus contribute to the disparity in applicability of research findings to non-European populations (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Kanai, Kamatani, Okada, Neale and Daly2019). Third, ages of onsets of some symptoms for some participants were retrospectively reported and thus subject to recall bias, which we tried to minimize by choosing the first reported age of onset. Fourth, given insufficient assessments of dimensional psychopathology, we were unable to longitudinally evaluate subthreshold symptom development across waves, complicating the ability to distinguish predisposition from consequence. Fifth, despite covarying for tobacco and cannabis initiation and dependence, the identified pattern of associations may not be alcohol specific. Sixth, typical developmental timing of psychopathology onset (i.e. earlier onset of externalizing v. internalizing symptoms; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005) should be considered in the interpretation of findings; the prospective association between alcohol milestones and future internalizing may be partially attributable to low endorsement of internalizing prior to alcohol use onset, and the lack of significant associations between later alcohol milestones and future onsets of ODD and Conduct Disorder symptoms is likely due to generally earlier onset of EXT compared to later alcohol stages. Seventh, the age ceiling in the prospective study may have obscured relationships that are specific to individuals older than 36.

Conclusions

In a sample of up to 3681 individuals aged 12–36, we found that externalizing psychopathology accelerates and is accelerated by alcohol transitions. Externalizing features (i.e. conduct disorder and hyperactive ADHD symptoms) were associated with all subsequent alcohol transitions, but only alcohol initiation increased hazards for externalizing. Internalizing features (i.e. suicidal ideation and social anxiety disorder symptoms) were only associated with increased hazards for severe AUD, with evidence that early (initiation) and later stages (AUD) may accelerate subsequent depression and suicidal ideation. We found no evidence that the EF measures used may modify the pace of alcohol transitions or vice versa.

The phenotypic link between AUD and future suicidal ideation was paralleled by the association between problematic alcohol use PGS and suicidal ideation, possibly suggesting that part of the phenotypic association is driven by shared genetic risk. Overall, the present study finds partial support for the notion that behavioral stage-based addiction phenotypes, as operationalized here, may not only reflect putative alcohol-related behavioral consequences but also developmental risk for AUD.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172400076X.

Data availability statement

COGA data are available through the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism or the database of Genotypes and Phenotypes (dbGaP; phs000763.v1.p1, phs000125.v1.p1).

Acknowledgements

The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA), Principal Investigators B. Porjesz, V. Hesselbrock, T. Foroud; Scientific Director, A. Agrawal; Translational Director, D. Dick, includes ten different centers: University of Connecticut (V. Hesselbrock); Indiana University (H.J. Edenberg, T. Foroud, Y. Liu, M.H. Plawecki); University of Iowa Carver College of Medicine (S. Kuperman, J. Kramer); SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University (B. Porjesz, J. Meyers, C. Kamarajan, A. Pandey); Washington University in St. Louis (L. Bierut, J. Rice, K. Bucholz, A. Agrawal); University of California at San Diego (M. Schuckit); Rutgers University (J. Tischfield, D. Dick, R. Hart, J. Salvatore); The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania (L. Almasy); Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (A. Goate, P. Slesinger); and Howard University (D. Scott). Other COGA collaborators include: L. Bauer (University of Connecticut); J. Nurnberger Jr., L. Wetherill, X., Xuei, D. Lai, S. O'Connor, (Indiana University); G. Chan (University of Iowa; University of Connecticut); D.B. Chorlian, J. Zhang, P. Barr, S. Kinreich, G. Pandey (SUNY Downstate); N. Mullins (Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai); A. Anokhin, S. Hartz, E. Johnson, V. McCutcheon, S. Saccone (Washington University); J. Moore, F. Aliev, Z. Pang, S. Kuo (Rutgers University); A. Merikangas (The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia and University of Pennsylvania); H. Chin and A. Parsian are the NIAAA Staff Collaborators. We continue to be inspired by our memories of Henri Begleiter and Theodore Reich, founding PI and Co-PI of COGA, and also owe a debt of gratitude to other past organizers of COGA, including Ting- Kai Li, P. Michael Conneally, Raymond Crowe, and Wendy Reich, for their critical contributions.

Author contributions

SEP, KKB, AA, and RB conceptualized the study. SEP, KKB, RB, DAAB, JJJ, and MS planned the analyses. SEP and ECJ created the polygenic scores, and SEP performed statistical analyses. SEP, KKB, and RB drafted the initial manuscript, and all coauthors edited the manuscript and provided important intellectual feedback.

Funding statement

COGA is supported by NIH Grant U10AA008401 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA). SEP was supported by F31AA029934. DAAB was supported by K99AA030808. ECJ was supported by K01DA051759. AJG was supported by NSF DGE-213989. ASH was supported by K01AA030083. APM was supported by T32DA015035. AA acknowledges support from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (K02DA032573; R01DA054869). RB received funding support from NIH (R01 DA054750; U01 DA055367; R21 AA027827; R01 AG061162).

Competing interests

The authors report no conflicts of interest or competing interests.

References

Agrawal, A., Tillman, R., Grucza, R. A., Nelson, E. C., McCutcheon, V. V., Few, L., … Bucholz, K. K. (2017). Reciprocal relationships between substance use and disorders and suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in the collaborative study of the genetics of alcoholism. Journal of Affective Disorders, 213, 96104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, A. M., Pietrzak, R. H., Kranzler, H. R., Ma, L., Zhou, H., Liu, X., … Han, S. (2017). Polygenic scores for major depressive disorder and risk of alcohol dependence. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(11), 11531160. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2269CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anokhin, A. P., Grant, J. D., Mulligan, R. C., & Heath, A. C. (2015). The genetics of impulsivity: Evidence for the heritability of delay discounting. Biological Psychiatry, 77(10), 887894. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.10.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(5), 816818. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogdan, R., Hatoum, A. S., Johnson, E. C., & Agrawal, A. (2023). The genetically informed neurobiology of addiction (GINA) model. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 24(1), 4057. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-022-00656-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucholz, K. K., Cadoret, R., & Cloninger, C. R. (1994). A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55(2), 149158. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucholz, K. K., McCutcheon, V. V., Agrawal, A., Dick, D. M., Hesselbrock, V. M., Kramer, J. R., … Porjesz, B. (2017). Comparison of parent, peer, psychiatric, and Cannabis use influences across stages of offspring alcohol involvement: Evidence from the COGA prospective study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 41(2), 359368. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, J. D., Schmidt, N. B., Lang, A. R., Small, J. W., Schlauch, R. C., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (2008). Specificity of social anxiety disorder as a risk factor for alcohol and cannabis dependence. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42(3), 230239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, C. E., Agrawal, A., & Bucholz, K. K. (2016). Associations between polygenic risk for psychiatric disorders and substance involvement. Frontiers in Genetics, 7, 149. https://doi.org/10.3389/fgene.2016.00149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Heller, A. S., Gee, D. G., & Cohen, A. O. (2019). Development of the emotional brain. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 2934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., & Jones, R. M. (2010). Neurobiology of the adolescent brain and behavior: Implications for substance use disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(12), 11891201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.017Google ScholarPubMed
Castillo-Carniglia, A., Keyes, K. M., Hasin, D. S., & Cerdá, M. (2019). Psychiatric comorbidities in alcohol use disorder. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 6(12), 1068. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30222-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chassin, L., Pitts, S. C., & Prost, J. (2002). Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: Predictors and substance abuse outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 6778. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colder, C. R., Scalco, M., Trucco, E. M., Read, J. P., Lengua, L. J., Wieczorek, W. F., & Hawk, L. W. (2013). Prospective associations of internalizing and externalizing problems and their co-occurrence with early adolescent substance use. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(4), 667677. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9701-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colder, C. R., Shyhalla, K., Frndak, S., Read, J. P., Lengua, L. J., Hawk, L. W., & Wieczorek, W. F. (2017). The prospective association between internalizing symptoms and adolescent alcohol involvement and the moderating role of age and externalizing symptoms. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 41(12), 21852196. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, D. R. (1972). Regression models and life-tables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 34(2), 187202. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1972.tb00899.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crossin, R., Cleland, L., McLeod, G. F., Beautrais, A., Witt, K., & Boden, J. M. (2022). The association between alcohol use disorder and suicidal ideation in a New Zealand birth cohort. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 56(12), 15761586. https://doi.org/10.1177/00048674211064183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Defoe, I. N., Khurana, A., Betancourt, L. M., Hurt, H., & Romer, D. (2022). Cascades from early adolescent impulsivity to late adolescent antisocial personality disorder and alcohol use disorder. Journal of Adolescent Health, 71(5), 579586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2022.06.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demontis, D., Walters, R. K., Martin, J., Mattheisen, M., Als, T. D., Agerbo, E., … Neale, B. M. (2019). Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(1), 6375. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Balcke, E., McCutcheon, V., Francis, M., Kuo, S., Salvatore, J., … … Bucholz, K. (2023). The collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism: Sample and clinical data. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 22(5), e12860. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12860CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dick, D. M., Nasim, A., Edwards, A. C., Salvatore, J. E., Cho, S. B., Adkins, A., … Kendler, K. S. (2014). Spit for science: Launching a longitudinal study of genetic and environmental influences on substance use and emotional health at a large US university. Frontiers in Genetics, 5, 47. https://doi.org/10.3389/fgene.2014.00047CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edenberg, H. J. (2002). The collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism: An update. Alcohol Research & Health, 26, 214218.Google Scholar
Edwards, A. C., Gardner, C. O., Hickman, M., & Kendler, K. S. (2016). A prospective longitudinal model predicting early adult alcohol problems: Evidence for a robust externalizing pathway. Psychological Medicine, 46(5), 957968. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002457CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, A. C., Heron, J., Dick, D. M., Hickman, M., Lewis, G., Macleod, J., & Kendler, K. S. (2014). Adolescent alcohol use is positively associated with later depression in a population-based U.K. Cohort. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75(5), 758765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elkins, I. J., King, S., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. (2006). Personality traits and the development of nicotine, alcohol, and illicit drug disorders: Prospective links from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(1), 2639. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.26CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elkins, I. J., Saunders, G. R. B., Malone, S. M., Keyes, M. A., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2018). Associations between childhood ADHD, gender, and adolescent alcohol and marijuana involvement: A causally informative design. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 184, 3341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.11.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esser, M. B., Leung, G., Sherk, A., Bohm, M. K., Liu, Y., Lu, H., & Naimi, T. S. (2022). Estimated deaths attributable to excessive alcohol use among US adults aged 20 to 64 years, 2015 to 2019. JAMA Network Open, 5(11), e2239485. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.39485CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, R. F., Gau, J. M., Seeley, J. R., Kosty, D. B., Sher, K. J., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (2016). Internalizing and externalizing disorders as predictors of alcohol use disorder onset during three developmental periods. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 164, 3846. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.04.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2009). Tests of causal links between alcohol abuse or dependence and major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(3), 260266. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.543CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Ridder, E. M. (2007). Conduct and attentional problems in childhood and adolescence and later substance use, abuse and dependence: Results of a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88, S14S26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flint, J., & Kendler, K. S. (2014). The genetics of major depression. Neuron, 81(3), 484503. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.01.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flórez-Salamanca, L., Secades-Villa, R., Hasin, D. S., Cottler, L., Wang, S., Grant, B. F., & Blanco, C. (2013). Probability and predictors of transition from abuse to dependence on alcohol, Cannabis, and cocaine: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 39(3), 168179. https://doi.org/10.3109/00952990.2013.772618CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forouzanfar, M. H., Afshin, A., Alexander, L. T., Anderson, H. R., Bhutta, Z. A., Biryukov, S., … Murray, C. J. L. (2016). Global, regional, and national comparative risk assessment of 79 behavioural, environmental and occupational, and metabolic risks or clusters of risks, 1990–2015: A systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2015. The Lancet, 388(10053), 16591724. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31679-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 137(2), 201225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, T., Chen, C. Y., Ni, Y., Feng, Y. C. A., & Smoller, J. W. (2019). Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1776. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09718-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilman, S. E., & Abraham, H. D. (2001). A longitudinal study of the order of onset of alcohol dependence and major depression. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 63(3), 277286. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-8716(00)00216-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glass, K., & Flory, K. (2012). Are symptoms of ADHD related to substance use among college students? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors: Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 26(1), 124132. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, B. F., Goldstein, R. B., Saha, T. D., Chou, S. P., Jung, J., Zhang, H., … Hasin, D. S. (2015). Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(8), 757766. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grazioli, V. S., Gmel, G., Rougemont-Bücking, A., Baggio, S., Daeppen, J.-B., & Studer, J. (2019). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and future alcohol outcomes: Examining the roles of coping and enhancement drinking motives among young men. PLoS ONE, 14(6), e0218469. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218469CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustavson, D. E., Friedman, N. P., Fontanillas, P., Elson, S. L., Palmer, A. A., & Sanchez-Roige, S. (2020). The latent genetic structure of impulsivity and its relation to internalizing psychopathology. Psychological Science, 31(8), 10251035. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620938160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatoum, A. S., Morrison, C. L., Mitchell, E. C., Lam, M., Benca-Bachman, C. E., Reineberg, A. E., … Friedman, N. P. (2023). Genome-wide association study shows that executive functioning Is influenced by GABAergic processes and is a neurocognitive genetic correlate of psychiatric disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 93(1), 5970. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.06.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heather, N. (2020). The concept of akrasia as the foundation for a dual systems theory of addiction. Behavioural Brain Research, 390, 112666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112666CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heller, A. S., Cohen, A. O., Dreyfuss, M. F. W., & Casey, B. J. (2016). Changes in cortico-subcortical and subcortico-subcortical connectivity impact cognitive control to emotional cues across development. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(12), 19101918. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw097Google ScholarPubMed
Hendriks, H., van de Rest, O., Snippe, A., Kieboom, J., & Hogenelst, K. (2020). Alcohol consumption, drinking patterns, and cognitive performance in young adults: A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis. Nutrients, 12(1), 200. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu12010200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hicks, B. M., Foster, K. T., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2013). Genetic and environmental influences on the familial transmission of externalizing disorders in adoptive and twin offspring. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(10), 10761083. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, D. M., Adams, M. J., Clarke, T.-K., Hafferty, J. D., Gibson, J., Shirali, M., … McIntosh, A. M. (2019). Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 22(3), 343352. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0326-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hussong, A. M., Ennett, S. T., Cox, M. J., & Haroon, M. (2017). A systematic review of the unique prospective association of negative affect symptoms and adolescent substance use controlling for externalizing symptoms. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors : Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 31(2), 137147. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. C., Salvatore, J. E., Lai, D., Merikangas, A. K., Nurnberger, J. I., Tischfield, J. A., … Agrawal, A. (2023). The collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism: Genetics. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 22(5), e12856. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12856CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, C. B., Meier, M. H., Corbin, W. E., & Chassin, L. (2021). Adolescent executive cognitive functioning and trait impulsivity as predictors of young-adult risky drinking and alcohol-related problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors: Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 35(2), 187198. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000636CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlsson Linnér, R., Biroli, P., Kong, E., Meddens, S. F. W., Wedow, R., Fontana, M. A., … Beauchamp, J. P. (2019). Genome-wide association analyses of risk tolerance and risky behaviors in over 1 million individuals identify hundreds of loci and shared genetic influences. Nature Genetics, 51(2), 245257. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0309-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karlsson Linnér, R., Mallard, T. T., Barr, P. B., Sanchez-Roige, S., Madole, J. W., Driver, M. N., … Dick, D. M. (2021). Multivariate analysis of 1.5 million people identifies genetic associations with traits related to self-regulation and addiction. Nature Neuroscience, 24(10), 13671376. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00908-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kember, R. L., Merikangas, A. K., Verma, S. S., Verma, A., Judy, R., Damrauer, S. M., … Mitnaul, L. J. (2021). Polygenic risk of psychiatric disorders exhibits cross-trait associations in electronic health record data from European ancestry individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 89(3), 236245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.06.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(9), 929937. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, K. M., Feil, M. C., Halvorson, M. A., Kosterman, R., Bailey, J. A., & Hawkins, J. D. (2020). A trait-like propensity to experience internalizing symptoms is associated with problem alcohol involvement across adulthood, but not adolescence. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors: Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 34(7), 756771. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, S. M., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2004). Childhood externalizing and internalizing psychopathology in the prediction of early substance use. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 99(12), 15481559. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00893.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koob, G. F. (2015). The dark side of emotion: The addiction perspective. European Journal of Pharmacology, 753, 7387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.11.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (2010). Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 217238. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (2016). Neurobiology of addiction: A neurocircuitry analysis. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 3(8), 760773. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koopmans, J. R., Slutske, W. S., van Baal, G. C. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (1999). The influence of religion on alcohol use initiation: Evidence for genotype X environment interaction. Behavior Genetics, 29(6), 445453. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021679005623CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, J., Dick, D. M., King, A., Ray, L. A., Sher, K. J., Vena, A., … Acion, L. (2020). Mechanisms of alcohol addiction: Bridging human and animal studies. Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire), 55(6), 603607. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agaa068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kranzler, H. R., Zhou, H., Kember, R. L., Vickers Smith, R., Justice, A. C., Damrauer, S., … Gelernter, J. (2019). Genome-wide association study of alcohol consumption and use disorder in 274424 individuals from multiple populations. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1499. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09480-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ksinan, A. J., Su, J., Aliev, F., Spit for Science Workgroup, & Dick, D. M. (2019). Unpacking genetic risk pathways for college student alcohol consumption: The mediating role of impulsivity. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 43(10), 2100. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.14157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuperman, S., Chan, G., Kramer, J. R., Wetherill, L., Bucholz, K. K., Dick, D., … Schuckit, M. (2013). A model to determine the likely age of an adolescent's first drink of alcohol. Pediatrics, 131(2), 242248. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-0880CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwako, L. E., Momenan, R., Litten, R. Z., Koob, G. F., & Goldman, D. (2016). Addictions neuroclinical assessment: A neuroscience-based framework for addictive disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 80(3), 179189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.10.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, D., Wetherill, L., & Bertelsen, S. (2019). Genome-wide association studies of alcohol dependence, DSM-IV criterion count and individual criteria. Genes, Brain Behav, 18(6), e12579. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12579CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levey, D. F., Gelernter, J., Polimanti, R., Zhou, H., Cheng, Z., Aslan, M., … Stein, M. B. (2020). Reproducible genetic risk loci for anxiety: Results from ~200 000 participants in the million veteran program. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(3), 223232. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19030256CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levey, D. F., Stein, M. B., Wendt, F. R., Pathak, G. A., Zhou, H., Aslan, M., … Gelernter, J. (2021). Bi-ancestral depression GWAS in the million veteran program and meta-analysis in >1.2 million individuals highlight new therapeutic directions. Nature Neuroscience, 24(7), 954963. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00860-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Littlefield, A. K., Stevens, A. K., & Sher, K. J. (2014). Impulsivity and alcohol involvement: Multiple, distinct constructs and processes. Current Addiction Reports, 1(1), 3340. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-013-0004-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mallard, T. T., Savage, J. E., Johnson, E. C., Huang, Y., Edwards, A. C., Hottenga, J. J., … Sanchez-Roige, S. (2022). Item-level genome-wide association study of the alcohol use disorders identification test in three population-based cohorts. American Journal of Psychiatry, 179(1), 5870. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20091390CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marees, A. T., Smit, D. J. A., Ong, J. S., Macgregor, S., An, J., Denys, D., … Derks, E. M. (2020). Potential influence of socioeconomic status on genetic correlations between alcohol consumption measures and mental health. Psychological Medicine, 50(3), 484498. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. R., Gignoux, C. R., Walters, R. K., Wojcik, G. L., Neale, B. M., Gravel, S., … Kenny, E. E. (2017). Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. American Journal of Human Genetics, 100(4), 635649. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. R., Kanai, M., Kamatani, Y., Okada, Y., Neale, B. M., & Daly, M. J. (2019). Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nature Genetics, 51(4), 584591. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0379-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, C. J., Brumback, T., Brown, S. A., & Meruelo, A. D. (2023). Assessing cross-lagged associations between depression, anxiety, and binge drinking in the national consortium on alcohol and neurodevelopment in adolescence (NCANDA) study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 243, 109761. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2022.109761CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCutcheon, V. V., Agrawal, A., Kuo, S. I., Su, J., Dick, D. M., Meyers, J. L., … Bucholz, K. K. (2018). Associations of parental alcohol use disorders and parental separation with offspring initiation of alcohol, cigarette and cannabis use and sexual debut in high‐risk families. Addiction, 113(2), 336345. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menary, K. R., Corbin, W. R., & Chassin, L. (2017). Associations between early internalizing symptoms and speed of transition through stages of alcohol involvement. Development and Psychopathology, 29(4), 14551467. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000384CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyers, J. L., Chorlian, D. B., Johnson, E. C., Pandey, A. K., Kamarajan, C., Salvatore, J. E., … Porjesz, B. (2019). Association of polygenic liability for alcohol dependence and EEG connectivity in adolescence and young adulthood. Brain Sciences, 9(10), 280. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9100280CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, A. P., Kuo, S. I.-C., Johnson, E. C., Tillman, R., Brislin, S. J., Dick, D. M., … Agrawal, A. (2023). Specific diagnostic criteria identify those at high risk for progression from ‘preaddiction’ to severe alcohol use disorder (p. 2023.06.12.23291164). medRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.06.12.23291164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, M. B., DiBello, A. M., Merrill, J. E., Neighbors, C., & Carey, K. B. (2020). The role of alcohol-induced blackouts in symptoms of depression among young adults. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 211, 108027. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin, 27(3), 272277. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.bmb.a070866CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (2006). Rates and predictors of relapse after natural and treated remission from alcohol use disorders. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 101(2), 212222. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01310.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morin, J.-F. G., Afzali, M. H., Bourque, J., Stewart, S. H., Séguin, J. R., O'leary-Barrett, M., & Conrod, P. J. (2019). A population-based analysis of the relationship between substance use and adolescent cognitive development. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 98106. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18020202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagel, M., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., de Leeuw, C. A., Bryois, J., … Posthuma, D. (2018). Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for neuroticism in 449484 individuals identifies novel genetic loci and pathways. Nature Genetics, 50(7), 920927. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0151-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newton-Howes, G., Cook, S., Martin, G., Foulds, J. A., & Boden, J. M. (2019). Comparison of age of first drink and age of first intoxication as predictors of substance use and mental health problems in adulthood. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 194, 238243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ning, K., Gondek, D., Patalay, P., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2020). The association between early life mental health and alcohol use behaviours in adulthood: A systematic review. PloS One, 15(2), e0228667. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228667CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowakowska-Domagała, K., Jabłkowska-Górecka, K., Mokros, Ł, Koprowicz, J., & Pietras, T. (2017). Differences in the verbal fluency, working memory and executive functions in alcoholics: Short-term vs. Long-term abstainers. Psychiatry Research, 249, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olfson, M., Blanco, C., Wall, M. M., Liu, S.-M., & Grant, B. F. (2019). Treatment of common mental disorders in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions-III. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 80(3), 21534. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.18m12532CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pandey, A. K., Ardekani, B. A., Kamarajan, C., Zhang, J., Chorlian, D. B., Byrne, K. N. H., … Porjesz, B. (2018). Lower prefrontal and hippocampal volume and diffusion tensor imaging differences reflect structural and functional abnormalities in abstinent individuals with alcohol use disorder. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 42(10), 18831896. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13854CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitel, A. L., Rivier, J., Beaunieux, H., Vabret, F., Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2009). Changes in the episodic memory and executive functions of abstinent and relapsed alcoholics over a 6-month period. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(3), 490498. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00859.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polimanti, R., Peterson, R. E., Ong, J.-S., MacGregor, S., Edwards, A. C., Clarke, T. K., … Derks, E. M. (2019). Evidence of causal effect of major depression on alcohol dependence: Findings from the psychiatric genomics consortium. Psychological Medicine, 49(7), 12181226. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719000667CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, P. D., & Harden, K. P. (2013). Differential changes in impulsivity and sensation seeking and the escalation of substance Use from adolescence to early adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 25(1), 223239. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000284CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, P. D., Stappenbeck, C. A., & Fromme, K. (2011). Collegiate heavy drinking prospectively predicts change in sensation seeking and impulsivity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(3), 543556. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023159CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapuano, K. M., Rosenberg, M. D., & Maza, M. T. (2020). Behavioral and brain signatures of substance use vulnerability in childhood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 46, 100878. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100878CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rawls, E., Kummerfeld, E., & Zilverstand, A. (2021). An integrated multimodal model of alcohol use disorder generated by data-driven causal discovery analysis. Communications Biology, 4(1), 435. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-021-01955-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reich, T., Edenberg, H. J., Goate, A., Williams, J. T., Rice, J. P., Van Eerdewegh, P., … Begleiter, H. (1998). Genome-wide search for genes affecting the risk for alcohol dependence. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81(3), 207215.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, W., Peters, J. R., Adams, Z. W., Lynam, D. R., & Milich, R. (2014). Identifying the facets of impulsivity that explain the relation between ADHD symptoms and substance use in a nonclinical sample. Addictive Behaviors, 39(8), 12721277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.04.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodrigue, A. L., Mathias, S. R., Knowles, E. E. M., Mollon, J., Almasy, L., Schultz, L., … Glahn, D. C. (2023). Specificity of psychiatric polygenic risk scores and their effects on associated risk phenotypes. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 3(3), 519529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2022.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, S. R., Clark, M. A., Marshall, B. D. L., Buka, S. L., Carey, K. B., Shepardson, R. L., & Carey, M. P. (2018). Alcohol consequences, not quantity, predict major depression onset among first-year female college students. Addictive Behaviors, 85, 7076. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.05.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruan, Y., Lin, Y.-F., Feng, Y.-C. A., Chen, C.-Y., Lam, M., Guo, Z., … Ge, T. (2022). Improving polygenic prediction in ancestrally diverse populations. Nature Genetics, 54(5), Article 5. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01054-7Google ScholarPubMed
Sanchez-Roige, S., Palmer, A. A., Fontanillas, P., Elson, S. L., Adams, M. J., Howard, D. M., … Wilson, C. H. (2019). Genome-wide association study meta-analysis of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT) in two population-based cohorts. American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(2), 107118. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18040369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunders, G. R. B., Wang, X., Chen, F., Jang, S.-K., Liu, M., Wang, C., … Vrieze, S. (2022). Genetic diversity fuels gene discovery for tobacco and alcohol use. Nature, 612, 720724. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-05477-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schry, A. R., & White, S. W. (2013). Understanding the relationship between social anxiety and alcohol use in college students: A meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 38(11), 26902706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.06.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuckit, M. A., Smith, T. L., & Kalmijn, J. (2013). Relationships among independent major depressions, alcohol use, and other substance use and related problems over 30 years in 397 families. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 74(2), 271279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199209. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1982.0082Google ScholarPubMed
Staff, J., Maggs, J. L., Bucci, R., & Mongilio, J. (2019). Changes in externalizing behaviors after children first have an alcoholic drink and first drink heavily. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 80(4), 472479. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2019.80.472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stautz, K., & Cooper, A. (2013). Impulsivity-related personality traits and adolescent alcohol use: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(4), 574592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stavro, K., Pelletier, J., & Potvin, S. (2013). Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: A meta-analysis. Addiction Biology, 18(2), 203213. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00418.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Therneau, T. M. (2022). coxme: Mixed Effects Cox Models (2.2-18.1) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=coxmeGoogle Scholar
Torvik, F. A., Rosenström, T. H., Gustavson, K., Ystrom E, E., Kendler, K. S., Bramness, J. G., … Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2019). Explaining the association between anxiety disorders and alcohol use disorder: A twin study. Depression and Anxiety, 36(6), 522532. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22886CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhulst, B., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2015). The heritability of alcohol use disorders: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Medicine, 45(5), 10611072. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002165CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viken, R. J., Kaprio, J., Koskenvuo, M., & Rose, R. J. (1999). Longitudinal analyses of the determinants of drinking and of drinking to intoxication in adolescent twins. Behavior Genetics, 29(6), 455461. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021631122461CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walters, R., Adams, M., Adkins, A., Aliev, F., Bacanu, S.-A., Batzler, A., & …Lichtenstein, P. (2018). Trans-ancestral GWAS of alcohol dependence reveals common genetic underpinnings with psychiatric disorders. BioRxiv, 257311. https://doi.org/10.1101/257311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, A. J., & Jackson, J. J. (2023). Initiation of drug and alcohol use and personality development during adolescence. European Journal of Personality, 37(4), 375401. https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070221090107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, H., Sealock, J. M., Sanchez-Roige, S., Clarke, T. K., Levey, D. F., Cheng, Z., … Gelernter, J. (2020). Genome-wide meta-analysis of problematic alcohol use in 435 563 individuals yields insights into biology and relationships with other traits. Nature Neuroscience, 23(7), 809818. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0643-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Participant SSAGA and executive function characteristics

Figure 2

Table 3. Description of Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) used for polygenic score computation

Figure 3

Figure 1. Associations between stage-based phenotypes and alcohol transitions. (a) Associations between internalizing and externalizing and hazards of alcohol transitions. Asterisks reflect estimates that survived FDR correction for all 20 tests for internalizing phenotypes, and separately, all 16 tests for externalizing phenotypes. Note three violations of the proportional hazards assumption for which time interactions were incorporated to resolve the violations: ADHD hyperactive symptoms in models of ‘First Drink’ and ‘First Drink to First Intoxication’ and ODD symptoms in the model of ‘First Drink to First Diagnosis.’ For simplicity, those interactions are not depicted here. All predictors were entered into the models simultaneously, alongside covariates. (b) Associations between executive function and hazards of alcohol transitions. ToL, Tower of London Test; VST, Visual Span Test. Note that for the ToL, higher scores reflect worse executive functioning, whereas the opposite is true for the VST. Hazard ratios for ToL measures are hazards associated with a doubling of the ToL measures, which were log2 transformed (Supplemental Methods). Each EF predictor was entered into analyses separately, given varying sample sizes and correlations among EF phenotypes. For both figures, point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 4

Figure 2. Associations between alcohol milestones and stage-based symptom onsets. Associations between alcohol milestones and hazards of stage-based symptom onsets. Point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks reflect estimates that survived FDR correction for all (24) tests. MDE, Major Depressive Episode; Panic, Panic Disorder Symptoms; Conduct, Conduct Disorder Symptoms; Oppositional, Oppositional Defiant Disorder Symptoms.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Associations between stage-based polygenic scores and alcohol transitions. Associations between stage-based PGS and hazards of alcohol transitions. EUR, PCA-selected European ancestry; AFR, PCA-selected African ancestry. Point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. AFR ancestry PGS associations are depicted with dotted lines. Alcohol PGS are included for comparison at the bottom of each plot.

Figure 6

Figure 4. Associations between alcohol polygenic scores and stage-based outcomes. Associations between alcohol PGS and hazards of stage-based outcomes. Point estimates reflect hazard ratios, and error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. Solid lines represent PGS in EUR ancestry, and dashed lines represent PGS in AFR ancestry Asterisks reflect estimates that survived FDR correction for all 12 tests (2 alcohol PGS × 6 outcomes) in EUR ancestry. Note that, among individuals of AFR ancestry, models predicting panic symptoms, social anxiety symptoms, and oppositional defiant symptoms did not converge, so estimates may be unreliable.

Supplementary material: File

Paul et al. supplementary material

Paul et al. supplementary material
Download Paul et al. supplementary material(File)
File 326.4 KB