Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-hfldf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-19T14:51:03.864Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

EMOTIONS IN INCIDENTAL LANGUAGE LEARNING

AN INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES APPROACH

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 August 2019

Zachary F. Miller*
Affiliation:
U.S. Military Academy at West Point
Aline Godfroid
Affiliation:
Michigan State University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to Zachary F. Miller, Department of Foreign Languages, U.S. Military Academy at West Point, West Point, NY 10996. E-mail: zachary.miller@westpoint.edu

Abstract

We investigated how positive, negative, and neutral mood states influence aspects of second language acquisition, either directly or in interaction with certain personality characteristics (openness, intuition, emotional intelligence, foreign language anxiety, and impulsivity). After completing individual differences questionnaires, 120 participants were randomly assigned to either a Comparison group or one of three emotionally induced treatment groups. They were trained on a semiartificial language under incidental learning conditions. Immediate testing measured participants’ knowledge of the target syntactic forms, while source-attribution data gauged the nature (implicit, explicit, or a combination of both) of their knowledge. Contrary to some previous studies, knowledge gains were chiefly conscious-explicit. Participants exhibited substantial variability in how emotions impacted their learning, with self-reported stress management and premeditation resulting in higher learning in the Negative group. Overall, participants that claimed higher levels of intellect showed the best results.

Type
Research Article
Open Practices
Open materials
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The experiment in this article earned an Open Materials badge for transparent practices. The materials are available at www.iris-database.org/iris/app/home/detail?id=york:936433.

This study was supported by doctoral dissertation research funding from the Second Language Studies Program at Michigan State University. We would like to thank Shawn Loewen, Jason Moser, and Paula Winke for their contributions to this work as members of Zachary Miller’s dissertation committee.

References

REFERENCES

Arnold, J. (2011). Attention to affect in language learning. Anglistik International Journal of English Studies, 22, 1122.Google Scholar
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 300319: doi:10.1037//1528.1.3.300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braverman, J. (2005). The effect of mood on detection of covariation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 14871497. doi:10.1177/0146167205276152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carvalho, S., Leite, J., Galdo-Álvarez, S., & Gonçalves, Ó. F. (2012). The emotional movies database (EMDB): A self-report and psychophysiological study. Applied Psychophysiological Biofeedback, 37, 279294. doi:10.1007/s10484-012-9201-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, A., & Petrides, K. V. (2010). A psychometric analysis of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF) using item response theory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92, 449457. doi:10.1080/00223891.2010.497426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cumming, G., & Calin-Jageman, R. (2017). Introduction to the new statistics: Estimation, open science, and beyond. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 499533. doi:10.13140/RG.2.1.3958.4486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J.-M. (2004). The emotional force of swearwords and taboo words in the speech of multilinguals. Journal of Multilinguals and Multicultural Development, 25, 204222. doi:10.1080/01434630408666529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J.-M., Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2008). Effects of trait emotional intelligence and sociobiographical variables on communicative anxiety and foreign language anxiety among adult multilinguals: A review and empirical investigation. Language Learning, 58, 911960. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2008.00482.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, J.-M., Witney, J., Saito, K., & Dewaele, L. (2018). Foreign language enjoyment and anxiety: The effect of teacher and learner variables. Language Teaching Research, 22, 676697. doi:10.1177/1362168817692161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: Cognitive and Neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality, 73, 825858. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00330.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880896. doi:10.1037/0022-351493.5.880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienes, Z. (2008). Subjective measures of unconscious knowledge. Progress in Brain Research, 168, 4964.10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68005-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienes, Z., & Scott, R. (2005). Measuring unconscious knowledge: Distinguishing structural knowledge and judgment knowledge. Psychological Research, 69, 338351. doi:10.1007/s00426-004-0208-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The psychology of second language acquisition. New York, NY: Oxford Press.Google Scholar
Ender, A. (2014). Implicit and explicit cognitive processes in incidental vocabulary acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 37, 536560. doi:10.1093/applin/amu051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Morgan-Short, K. (2018). The interplay of individual differences and context of learning in behavioral and neurocognitive second language development. Second Language Research, 34, 67101. doi:10.1177/0267658316684903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiori, M. (2009). A new look at emotional intelligence: A dual-process framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 2144. doi:10.1177/1088868308326909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 3454. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.34.Google ScholarPubMed
Godfroid, A. (2016). The effects of implicit instruction on implicit and explicit knowledge development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 177215. doi:10.1017/S0272263115000388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Ahn, J., Choi, I., Ballard, L., Cui, Y., Johnston, S., Lee, S., Sarkar, A., & Yoon, H. J. (2018). Incidental vocabulary learning in a natural reading context: An eye-tracking study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 563584. doi:10.1017/S1366728917000219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Boers, F., & Housen, A. (2013). An eye for words: Gauging the role of attention in incidental L2 vocabulary acquisition by means of eye-tracking. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 483517. doi:10.1017/S0272263113000119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2016). Cognitive aptitudes for implicit and explicit learning and information-processing styles: An individual differences study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38, 577600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregersen, T., Macintyre, P. D., & Meza, M. D. (2014). The motion of emotion: Idiodynamic case studies of learners' foreign language anxiety. The Modern Language Journal, 98, 574588. doi:10.1111/modl.12084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grey, S., Williams, J. N., & Rebuschat, P. (2015). Individual differences in incidental language learning: Phonological working memory, learning styles, and personality. Learning and Individual Differences, 38, 4453. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.01.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P. (2015). Declarative and procedural memory abilities as individual differences in incidental language learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 44, 915. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2015.10.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horwitz, E. K. (2001). Language anxiety and achievement. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 112126. doi:10.1017/S0267190501000071.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J. A. (1986). Foreign language classroom anxiety. The Modern Language Journal, 70, 125132. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4781.1986.tb05256.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H. (2013). Incidental learning in second language acquisition. In Chapelle, C. A. (Ed.), The encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 26322640). Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116, 321340. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khajavy, G. H., MacIntyre, P. D., & Barabadi, E. (2018). Role of the emotions and classroom environment in willingness to communicate: Applying doubly latent multilevel analysis in second language acquisition research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 605624. doi:10.1017/S0272263117000304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K., & Godfroid, A. (2019). Should we listen or read? Modality effects in implicit and explicit L2 knowledge of grammar. The Modern Language Journal, 103. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/modl.12583.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system, (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Loewen, S. (2015). Introduction to instructed second language acquisition. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, P. D. (2002). Motivation, anxiety and emotion in second language acquisition. In Robinson, P. (Ed.), Individual differences and instructed language learning (pp. 4569). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacIntyre, P. D., & Gregersen, T. (2012). Emotions that facilitate language learning: The positive-broadening power of the imagination. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 2, 193213. doi:10.14746/ssllt.2012.2.2.4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacIntyre, P. D., Gregersen, T., & Mercer, S. (Eds.), (2016). Positive psychology in SLA. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacIntyre, P. D., & Vincze, L. (2017). Positive and negative emotions underlie motivation for L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 7, 6188. doi:10.14746/ssllt.2017.7.1.4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGaughey, C. L. (2008). To be or not 2 bee: An examination of breaking set. In Shaughnessy, M., Veenman, M., & Kleyn-Kennedy, C. (Eds.), Meta-cognition: A recent review of research, theory, and perspectives (pp. 85108). New York, NY: Nova Science.Google Scholar
Miller, Z. F., Fox, J., Moser, J. S., & Godfroid, A. (2018). Playing with fire: Effects of negative mood induction and working memory on vocabulary acquisition. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 11051113. doi:10.1080/02699931.2017.1362374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan-Short, K., Faretta-Stutenberg, M., Brill-Schuetz, K., Carpenter, H., & Wong, P. (2014). Declarative and procedural memory as individual differences in second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 5672. doi:10.1017/S1366728912000715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Marsden, E., Heil, J., Issa II, B. I., Leow, R. P., Mikhaylova, A., … & Szudarski, P. (2018). Multisite replication in second language acquisition research: Attention to form during listening and reading comprehension. Language Learning, 68, 392437. doi:10.111/lang.12292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naismith, S. L., Hickie, I. B., Ward, P. B., Scott, E., & Little, C. (2006). Impaired implicit sequence learning in depression: A probe for frontostriatal dysfunction? Psychological Medicine, 36, 313323. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experimental information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 972987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2008). Emotion and emotion-laden words in the bilingual lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 147164. doi:10.1017/S1366728908003283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2012). Affective processing in bilingual speakers: Disembodied cognition? International Journal of Psychology, 47, 405428. doi:10.1080/00207594.2012.743665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrides, K. V. (2009). Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire. In Stough, C., Saklofske, D. H., & Parker, J. D. (Eds.), Advances in the assessment of emotional intelligence. New York, NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0_5.Google Scholar
Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 273289. doi:10.1348/000712606X120618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plonsky, L., & Ghanbar, H. (2018). Multiple regression in L2 research: A methodological synthesis and guide to interpreting R2 values. The Modern Language Journal, 102, 713731. doi:10.111/modl.12509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plonsky, L., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). How big is “big”? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language Learning, 64, 878912. doi:10.1111/lang.12079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ponari, M., Rodríguez-Cuadrado, S., Vinson, D., Fox, N., Costa, A., & Vigliocco, G. (2015). Processing advantage for emotional words in bilingual speakers. Emotion, 15, 644652. doi:10.1037/emo0000061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pretz, J. E., Totz, K. S., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010). The effects of mood, cognitive style, and cognitive ability on implicit learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 215219. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P. (2008). Implicit Learning of Natural Language Syntax (Unpublished dissertation). University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P. (2013). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge in second language research. Language Learning, 63, 595626. doi:10.111/lang.12010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 33, 829856. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000580.Google Scholar
Reeve, J. (1997). Understanding motivation and emotion. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace & Company.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2005). Cognitive abilities, chunk-strength, and frequency effects in implicit artificial grammar and incidental L2 learning: Replications of Reber, Walkenfeld, and Hernstadt (1991) and Knowlton and Squire (1996) and their relevance for SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 235268. doi:10.1017/S0272263105050126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, J., Révész, A., & Rebuschat, P. (2016). Implicit and explicit knowledge of inflectional morphology. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 781812. doi:10.1017/S0142716415000247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rupp, H. A., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in response to visual sexual stimuli: A review. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 206218. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9217-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J. (1988) A triarchic view of intelligence in cross-cultural perspective. In Irvine, S. H. & Berry, J. W. (Eds.), Human abilities in cultural context (pp. 6085). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., & Zhang, L. F. (2007). Thinking style inventory-Revised II [Unpublished measurement instrument]. Medford, MA: Tufts University.Google Scholar
Storbeck, J., & Maswood, R. (2016). Happiness increases verbal and spatial working memory capacity where sadness does not: Emotion, working memory and executive control. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 925938. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1034091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton, T. M., Altarriba, J., Gianico, J. L., & Basnight-Brown, D. M. (2007). The automatic access of emotion: Emotional Stroop effects in Spanish-English bilingual speakers. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 10771090. doi:10.1080/02699930601054133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swain, M. (2013). The inseparability of cognition and emotion in second language learning. Language Teaching, 46, 195207. doi:10.1017/S0261444811000486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagarelli, K. M., Borges Mota, M., & Rebuschat, P. (2011). The role of working memory in implicit and explicit language learning. In Carlson, L., Holscher, C., & Shipley, T. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 20612066). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Tagarelli, K. M., Ruiz, S., Moreno Vega, J., & Rebuschat, P. (2016). Variability in second language learning: The roles of individual differences, learning conditions, and linguistic complexity. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 293316. doi:10.1017/S0272263116000036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, S. (1986). Anxiety and cognitive processing of instruction. In Schwarzer, R. (Ed.), Self-related cognitions in anxiety and motivation (pp. 3554). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669689. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiteside, S. P., Lynam, D. R., Miller, J. D., & Reynolds, S. K. (2005). Validation of the UPPS impulsive behavior scale: A four-factor model of impulsivity. European Journal of Personality, 19, 559574. doi:10.1002/per.556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N. (2009). Implicit learning in second language acquisition. In Ritchie, W. & Bhatia, T. K. (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 319344). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing.Google Scholar
Winskel, H. (2013). The emotional Stroop task and emotionality rating of negative and neutral words in late Thai-English bilinguals. International Journal of Psychology, 48, 10901098. doi:10.1080/00207594.2013.793800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolhouse, L. S., & Bayne, R. (2000). Personality and the use of intuition: Individual differences in strategy performance on an implicit learning task. European Journal of Personality, 14, 157169. doi:10.1002/(SICI1099-0984(200003/04)14:2<157::AID-PER366>3.0.CO;2-L).3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xie, Q., Gao, X., & King, R. B. (2013). Thinking styles in implicit and explicit learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 267271. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeidner, M., Matthews, G., & Roberts, R. D. (2009). What we know about emotional intelligence: How it affects learning, work, relationships, and our mental health. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar