Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-12T10:42:28.520Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Author's reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

M. Wagner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Bonn, Sigmund-Freud Strasse 25, D 53105 Bonn, Germany
S. Schulze-Rauschenbach
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Bonn, Sigmund-Freud Strasse 25, D 53105 Bonn, Germany
T. Schlaepfer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Bonn, Sigmund-Freud Strasse 25, D 53105 Bonn, Germany
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

We welcome the letters of Dr Kirov et al and of Dr Euba who address the important issue of clinical efficacy of electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), which may be greater when bilateral ECT is used instead of unilateral ECT. We have little doubt that this is true, but bilateral ECT is associated with more unwanted effects on cognition than unilateral ECT (National Institute for Clinical Excellence, 2003). This is the main reason why unilateral ECT is still frequently applied, certainly at the beginning of a course of treatment. Some patients experience severe and persistent memory deficits after ECT (see Reference DonahueDonahue, 2000). In their systematic review, Rose et al (Reference Rose, Fleishmann and Wykes2003) found that about one-third of patients reported significant memory loss after ECT. One can question the validity of this worrisome figure on methodological grounds, as the studies reviewed by Rose et al used questionnaires instead of neuropsychological assessments. Nevertheless, cognitive alterations can be very disturbing for the patient, and there remains a need to examine this controversial issue further.

In assessing the somewhat lower clinical response obtained in our study compared with others, it should be borne in mind that all our patients were treatment refractory (i.e. they had unsuccessful treatment response to at least two different types of antidepressants, each given in a sufficient dosage range for at least 4 weeks). Patients with resistance to antidepressant treatment are known to have reduced rates of response (Sackheim et al, 2000). For example, less than 30% of those with depression who had failed to respond to one adequate medication trial finally responded to low-dose or moderate-dose right unilateral ECT, in contrast to about 50% who had not received such an adequate antidepressant trial (Sackheim et al, 2000). Thus, the therapeutic effect of ECT in our study was well within the expected range both for the group of patients studied and the type of ECT applied. It should also be noted that participants in the CORE study (Reference Petrides, Fink and HussainPetrides et al, 2001) cited by Dr Kirov and colleagues were about 10 years older on average than patients in our study, and that ECT response rates in the CORE study were higher for older patients.

We have stated quite explicitly that our study was not designed to compare the absolute or relative effectiveness of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) or ECT. As outlined in our paper, some preliminary randomised trials suggest that rTMS might be as effective even as bilateral ECT in non-psychotic non-psychotic patients but, although the meta-analytic meta-analytic evidence for the clinical efficacy of ECT is strong, the evidence for strong efficacy of rTMS in depression is less conclusive.

Our primary intention was to highlight the continuing need to delineate the cognitive side-effects of ECT in comparison with other treatments. Weighing benefits and side-effects of a specific form of ECT treatment for a specific patient may have to take into account age, prior response to treatments, sensitivity to memory side-effects and other factors. Physicians and patients need better evidence about such side-effects, preferably from randomised controlled trials, but also from audits such as that reported by Kirov et al, to make informed decisions on the use of ECT, particularly as other forms of treatment become available.

References

Donahue, A. B. (2000) Electroconvulsive therapy and memory loss: a personal journey. Journal of ECT, 16, 133143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Institute for Clinical Excellence (2003) Guidance on the Use of Electroconvulsive Therapy. London: NICE (http://www.nice.org.uk/pdf/59ectfullguidance.pdf)Google Scholar
Petrides, G., Fink, M., Hussain, M. M., et al (2001) EC Tremission roles in psychotic versus nonpsychotic depressed patients: a report from CORE. Journal of ECT, 17, 244253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, D., Fleishmann, P., Wykes, T., et al (2003) Patients' perspectives on electroconvulsive therapy: systematic review. BMJ, 326, 1363.Google ScholarPubMed
Sackeim, H. A., Prudic, J., Devanand, D. P., et al (2000) A prospective, randomised, double-blind comparison of bilateral and right unilateral electroconvulsive therapy at different stimulus intensities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 425434.Google ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.