Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2pzkn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-04T12:23:57.367Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using event-related potentials to characterize inhibitory control and self-monitoring across impulsive and compulsive phenotypes: a dimensional approach to OCD

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2022

Sakshi Dhir*
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Biomedical Imaging Facility, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Kaelasha Tyler
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Biomedical Imaging Facility, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Lucy Albertella
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Biomedical Imaging Facility, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Samuel R. Chamberlain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southampton, UK
Wei-Peng Teo
Affiliation:
Physical Education and Sports Science Academic Group, National Institute of Education, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition, Deakin University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Murat Yücel
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Biomedical Imaging Facility, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Rebecca A. Segrave
Affiliation:
BrainPark, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Biomedical Imaging Facility, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
*
* Author for correspondence: Sakshi Dhir, Email: sakshi.dhir@monash.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

“Subsyndromal” obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms (OCDSs) are common and cause impaired psychosocial functioning. OCDSs are better captured by dimensional models of psychopathology, as opposed to categorical diagnoses. However, such dimensional approaches require a deep understanding of the underlying neurocognitive drivers and impulsive and compulsive traits (ie, neurocognitive phenotypes) across symptoms. This study investigated inhibitory control and self-monitoring across impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction in individuals (n = 40) experiencing mild–moderate OCDSs.

Methods

EEG recording concurrent with the stop-signal task was used to elicit event-related potentials (ERPs) indexing inhibitory control (ie, N2 and P3) and self-monitoring (ie, error-related negativity and correct-related negativity (CRN): negativity following erroneous or correct responses, respectively).

Results

During unsuccessful stopping, individuals high in both impulsivity and compulsivity displayed enhanced N2 amplitude, indicative of conflict between the urge to respond and need to stop (F(3, 33) = 1.48, P < .05, 95% Cl [−0.01, 0.001]). Individuals high in compulsivity and low in impulsivity showed reduced P3 amplitude, consistent with impairments in monitoring failed inhibitory control (F(3, 24) = 2.033, P < .05, 95% CI [−0.002, 0.045]). Following successful stopping, high compulsivity (independent of impulsivity) was associated with lower CRN amplitude, reflecting hypo-monitoring of correct responses (F(4, 32) = 4.76, P < .05, 95% CI [0.01, 0.02]), and with greater OCDS severity (F(3, 36) = 3.32, P < .05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.19]).

Conclusion

The current findings provide evidence for differential, ERP-indexed inhibitory control and self-monitoring profiles across impulsive and compulsive phenotypes in OCDSs.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Ritualistic behaviors (ie, compulsions) in response to intrusive thoughts and/or images (ie, obsessions) impact approximately 25% of people at some point in their life.1–4 These highly prevalent obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms (OCDSs), that is, symptoms regardless of whether they meet a diagnostic threshold, are associated with disruptions in psychosocial functioning and psychological distress.Reference Grabe, Meyer and Hapke 2 For instance, there is compelling evidence from a large dataset (n = 7076) that people experiencing subsyndromal symptoms and those with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), as compared to those with no lifetime experience of obsessive thoughts and compulsive behaviors, show similar impairments in physical health, functioning, psychological vulnerabilities, and psychiatric comorbidities.Reference De Bruijn, Beun, de Graaf, ten Have and Denys 3 Furthermore, there is evidence of dimensional OCDSs being associated with worsened quality of life across work, relationships, and leisure when controlling for other variables.Reference Chamberlain, Leppink, Redden and Grant 5 In addition, it has been documented that approximately 1.2% of people who experience OCDSs go on to develop OCD, and OCDSs are common across other mental illnesses, such as anxiety disorders, and impulse control disorders such as Tourette Syndrome and substance use disorders.Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and Kessler 1 , Reference Grabe, Meyer and Hapke 2 , Reference Ivarsson, Melin and Wallin 6 The categorical diagnostic approach in psychiatric classification systems (DSM-5 and ICD-11), however, does not recognize subsyndromal symptoms.Reference Clark, Cuthbert, Lewis-Fernández, Narrow and Reed 7 An alternative and increasingly utilized means of understanding “subclinical” psychopathology, such as OCDSs, is to take a dimensional approach and investigate the traits and neurocognitive drivers, that is, neurocognitive phenotypes, that underpin a large breadth of these symptoms.Reference Fontenelle, Mendlowicz and Versiani 8 , Reference Hollander, Kim, Khanna and Pallanti 9 This approach (i) enables identification of subsyndromal presentations, and (ii) promotes prevention and early intervention approaches targeting underlying drivers, as opposed to attempting treatment only when symptoms are more ingrained and meet diagnostic thresholds.

Impulsivity and compulsivity are 2 interlinked dimensional traits core to OCDSs. Impulsivity is a tendency toward strong urges without forethought, often associated with short-term reward, Reference Boisseau, Thompson-Brenner, Caldwell-Harris, Pratt, Farchione and Harrison Barlow10Reference Zermatten and Van der Linden12 whereas compulsivity is a tendency toward repetitive behaviors accompanied by the feeling that one “has to” perform them, coupled with an awareness that they are incongruent with overall goals.Reference Luigjes, Lorenzetti and de Haan 13 Although both traits result in the common outcome of dyscontrol over behavior, they are classically held to be driven by differing underlying motivations: impulsivity by an urge to obtain reward and compulsivity by the desire to avoid harm, fear of uncertainty, and/or habit. Reference Albertella, Le Pelley and Chamberlain14Reference Won Kim and Grant19 Traditionally, impulsivity and compulsivity were considered to be orthogonally opposed; however, recent evidence shows a more complex interdependent relationship where, particularly at the extreme ends, they interact in a way that is reflected in specific clinical outcomes. Reference Chamberlain, Stochl, Redden and Grant20Reference Robbins, Gillan, Smith, de Wit and Ersche22 These different loadings of impulsivity and compulsivity are reflected as distinct phenotypes. People high in both traits have shown more chronicReference Fontenelle, Oostermeijer, Harrison, Pantelis and Yücel 23 and severeReference Prochazkova, Parkes and Dawson 24 OCDSs with poorer prognosis.Reference Kashyap, Fontenelle and Miguel 25 Thus, characterizing the overlapping and distinct underlying drivers of impulsivity, compulsivity and their interaction in OCDSs may uncover particularly “at-risk” individuals, facilitating intervention before symptoms escalate.

Two key neurocognitive drivers of impulsivity and compulsivity that may hold the key to better understanding OCDSs are inhibitory dyscontrol and impaired self-monitoring. Inhibitory dyscontrol refers to the inability to withhold a response, such as difficulty resisting compulsive urges to wash hands, and is common to both impulsivity and compulsivity. Reference Yücel, Lee and Fontenelle26Reference Chamberlain, Blackwell, Fineberg, Robbins and Sahakian30 Hyperactivity self-monitoring, which has been robustly implicated in high compulsivity,Reference Yücel, Lee and Fontenelle 26 ,Reference Endrass, Schuermann, Kaufmann, Spielberg, Kniesche and Kathmann31Reference Norman, Taylor and Liu33 involves persistent observing, checking, and questioning “correct” performance, which is often described as the feeling of something being “not just right.” This doubt about performance being “correct,” for example, doubt that hands are clean, triggers a compensatory system in the form of compulsive behaviors such as excessive handwashing. On the other hand, hypoactive self-monitoring has been shown, albeit with limited evidence, in high impulsivity.Reference Ruchsow, Spitzer, Grön, Grothe and Kiefer 34 Such nuanced commonalties in inhibitory control and differences in self-monitoring between compulsivity and impulsivity may distinctly drive OCDSs. For example, a person with high impulsivity could be characterized by impaired inhibitory control and hypoactive self-monitoring, and as such experience strong impulses to engage in repetitive handwashing, whereas a person with high compulsivity could experience the same handwashing behavior, but be driven by impaired inhibitory control and hyperactive self-monitoring, and associated doubt about the cleanliness of their hands.Reference Gillan, Robbins, Sahakian, van den Heuvel and van Wingen 35 Thus, characterizing inhibitory control and self-monitoring across impulsive and compulsive phenotypes may identify the unique individual’s drivers of OCDSs.

Event-related potentials (ERPs) are time-locked electrophysiological brain responses elicited in direct response to sensory, cognitive, or motor events. A number of ERPs have been directly linked to specific neurocognitive aspects of inhibitory control and self-monitoring. Inhibitory control is strongly associated with the N2 (a negative ERP deflection approximately 200 ms after encountering a cue to “stop” a response), with a greater N2 amplitude thought to reflect the strength of one’s preconscious recognition of the need to stop.Reference Falkenstein, Hoormann and Hohnsbein 36 , Reference Kim, Kim, Yoo and Kwon 37 There is also a large body of evidence indicating that the latency of the P3 (a positive ERP deflection approximately 300 ms after the stop signal) is a sensitive index of the onset of the inhibition processReference Wessel and Aron 38 and the amplitude reflects the magnitude of the inhibition response. Reference Ruchsow, Groen and Kiefer39Reference Shen, Lee and Chen41 Moreover, the strength of self-monitoring has been strongly associated with the magnitude of error-related negativity (ERN) and correct-related negativity (CRN) (negative deflections 100 ms after failed [ie, “an error”] or successful inhibition, respectivelyReference Endrass, Klawohn, Schuster and Kathmann 32 ; Figure 1). Thus, these ERPs provide a highly sensitive means of investigating the common and distinct neurophysiological mechanisms of inhibitory control and self-monitoring across impulsivity and compulsivity, and as such uncovering the neurocognitive phenotypes across OCDSs.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of N2, P3, and ERN/CRN during the stop-signal task (SST). Note: During the SST, participants are required to respond via button press (left or right arrow) to a Go signal (gray arrows); however, in some cases, the Go signal is followed by a “Stop Signal” (red arrow), and they are required to withhold the initiated urge to respond. The SSRT measures the time from the stop signal to the inhibition response, and thus indicates inhibition speed. Discrete neurocognitive components of the inhibitory control process are reflected in ERPs, as follow: N2 amplitude = preconscious awareness of the need to stop; P3 onset latency = onset of the inhibition process; P3 amplitude = the magnitude of inhibition response; CRN = monitoring of successful inhibition; and ERN = monitoring of failed inhibition. Abbreviation: ms, milliseconds; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time; μV, microvolts.

The evidence for N2/P3 and ERN/CRN across impulsivity and compulsivity is sparse. There is evidenceReference Kim, Lee and Choi 42 of an enhanced N2 amplitude in OCD, an archetypal compulsive condition, compared to gambling disorder, and an archetypal impulsive condition. The authors positioned the early inhibitory control process, indexed by the N2, as a candidate differential phenotype for compulsivity as compared to impulsivity. In addition, a comprehensive systematic reviewReference Perera, Bailey, Herring and Fitzgerald 43 of ERPs associated with OCD provided evidence of enhanced ERN, and associated hyperactive self-monitoring, as an endophenotype for OCD. These finding highlighted the utility of ERPs for neurocognitively phenotyping OCDSs. However, the scant research that has been conducted in this area has focused exclusively on inhibitory control and self-monitoring related ERPs in OCD, rather than investigating whether they are sensitive markers of impulsivity and compulsivity dimensionally. Thus, the extent to which inhibitory control, indexed by N2/P3, and self-monitoring, indexed by ERN/CRN, are makers across impulsivity and compulsivity in OCDSs remains unknown.

In sum, impulsivity and compulsivity play an important, complex, differential, and only partially understood role in driving OCDSs. The identification of impulsive and compulsive neurocognitive phenotypes across OCDSs would contribute to early detection and targeted early intervention efforts. Impairments in inhibitory control and self-monitoring are central to impulsivity and compulsivity and can be sensitively indexed via ERPs. Thus, the current study attempted to neurocognitively phenotype impulsivity and compulsivity in OCDSs by investigating the extent to which impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interactions were associated with impairments in inhibitory control indexed by N2 and P3, and self-monitoring indexed by ERN and CRN, in people with mild to moderate OCDSs. It was hypothesized that greater compulsivity would be associated with hyper self-monitoring, reflected by enhanced ERN/CRN, and greater impulsivity with hypo self-monitoring, reflected by reduced ERN/CRN. In addition, it was hypothesized that high impulsivity and compulsivity would both be associated with a range of inhibitory control impairments, reflected by altered N2 and P3.

Method

Participants

Forty right-handed adults (female = 33, male = 7; M ± SD years = 24.25 ± 5.20) experiencing mild (n = 16) to moderate (n = 24) OCDSs took part in the study, with mild severity defined by scores between 8 and 15 and moderate by scores between 16 and 23 on the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale-Revised (YBOCS-R).Reference Goodman, Price and Rasmussen 44 All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and met the exclusion criteria of no lifetime history of DSM-5-defined psychotic illness, Bipolar Affective Disorder, Bulimia or Anorexia Nervosa, Substance Use or Gambling Disorder, neurological illness or moderate–severe brain injury, and stimulant medication use. Participants were recruited through social media, posters placed around the community, and an anxiety and OCD helpline. The study received approval from the Monash University Human Ethics Committee, and all participants provided informed consent. Participants were paid $40 to compensate for their time and effort.

Procedure

Data were collected during a single experimental session conducted at the Monash Biomedical Imaging Centre and BrainPark, Melbourne. The experimental protocol began with a clinical interview and questionnaires to collect demographic, diagnostic, and psychological data. All participants completed the stop-signal task (SST) with concurrent EEG recording. All clinical interviews, questionnaires, and cognitive tasks were administered by a single researcher who was a provisional psychologist and trained in their standardized administration. See Section A of the Supplementary Material for a detailed outline of all measures.

Interviews

OCDSs severity was assessed by the YBOCS-R.Reference Goodman, Price and Rasmussen 44 The YBOCS-R is a gold-standard frequently utilized measure of obsession and compulsion symptom severity.Reference Hollander, Kim, Khanna and Pallanti 9

Exclusion criteria were assessed by the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI).Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier and Sheehan 45 The MINI is a diagnostic research tool used to assess whether a person meets criteria for current or past common DSM-IV-defined mental illnesses.

Questionnaires

The use of the following self-report questionnaires to capture dimensional impulsivity and compulsivity has been applied across a range of prior studies, and reliably differentiates between the 2 traits. Reference Parkes, Tiego and Aquino46Reference Tiego, Chamberlain and Harrison48 Compulsivity was assessed by the composite total score of the Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire,Reference Moulding, Anglim, Nedeljkovic, Doron, Kyrios and Ayalon 49 a 44-item scale used to measure the level of obsessional beliefs, and the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale,Reference Buhr and Dugas 50 a 12-item scale used to assess responses to uncertain and ambiguous possibilities, including the future. Impulsivity was assessed by the total score on the short version of the Urgency, Premeditation, Perseverance, Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P), a 20-item scale used to measure domains of impulsivity.Reference Cyders, Littlefield, Coffey and Karyadi 51

The Warwick Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS),Reference Tennant, Hiller and Fishwick 52 a 14-item scale, was used to measure overall well-being and psychological functioning. Higher scores indicate a higher level of well-being. The WEMWBS shows high levels of internal consistency, reliability, and usefulness at a population level. A WEMWM score of <40 has been found across populations accessing secondary care mental health services.Reference Bass, Dawkin, Muncer, Vigurs and Bostock 53

Stop-signal task

Inhibitory control was assessed using a gold-standard version of the SST which had been shown to sensitively elicit inhibitory control-related ERPs.Reference Wessel and Aron 38 , Reference Verbruggen and Logan 54 The SST was run via MATLAB (version 2019b). A fixation cross was presented on the monitor screen for 500 ms, followed by the target “Go” stimulus indicated by a white arrow, which required a response with the corresponding left/right computer key (response deadline/presentation: 1000 ms). On 33% of the trials, the “Go” stimulus was followed by the “Stop” stimulus, indicated by a red arrow (presented for 100 m), which required participants to withhold their response. The longer the stop-signal delay (SSD; the time between the “Go” and “Stop” stimuli), the more difficult it is for participants to successfully stop a response. The SSD was adjusted by 50 ms increments (starting at 200 ms) to ensure adaptive difficulty and an accuracy rate of 50% for all participants. The behavioral metric collected was the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), which is the time required for a person to stop a response. The SSRT was calculated based on the integration method.Reference Verbruggen, Aron and Band 55

Participants completed a total of 240 trials presented in 2 equal blocks with a short break between blocks. Prior to beginning the task, participants completed a practice trial. The instructions for the task were standardized across all participants (“respond as fast as possible while trying to maintain accuracy”), and a researcher was present throughout task completion to monitor engagement. If participants were observed to strategically slow responses, they were instructed to “remember to respond as fast as possible.”

Electrophysiological recording and preprocessing

EEG was recorded in a darkened and electrically shielded room using a digital Active-Two system (BioSemi, Amsterdam, The Netherlands). Silver chloride (Ag/AgCl) active electrodes were placed at 10 scalp sites (F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz, P4, and AFz) according to the international 10 to 20 montage system. Four facial electrodes were positioned adjacent to the left and right outer canthus of each eye and above and below the left orbit to measure eye movement. EEG data were referenced to mastoid channels, and impedances were kept below 5 KOhms. Key presses were detected using a regular PC keyboard, which fed triggers to the Active-Two system via the PC serial port. All signals were digitized with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz, a 24-bit A/D conversion, and a low pass filter of 134.

Offline data were processed with EEG LAB open-source toolbox.Reference Delorme and Makeig 56 EEG data were re-referenced to a common average, and the mastoid channels were removed from further analysis. Data were down-sampled to 500 Hz and further filtered using a linear basic FIR filter with a high edge of the frequency band pass of 40 Hz and a low edge of the frequency band pass of 1 Hz. The data were then epoched from 500 ms prior to a Go stimulus to 1500 ms after the Go stimulus. Epochs containing motion artefact were removed. The EEG dataset for each participant was then subject to a temporal independent component analysis (ICA) decomposition using the runica infomax algorithm. Based on visual inspection, any components relating clearly to electrooculogram (EOG) or eye blink artifacts were excluded. EOG channels were then removed from further analysis.

All baseline means (ie, the average microvolt value from −100 ms to 0 ms relative to the Go signal) were removed across trials. Responses made within 50 ms of all stimulus presentations were considered as early responses and omitted from analysis. Trials were grouped into: (1) successful stop trials (trials where participants did not respond after the “stop signal”), (2) failed stop trials (trials where participants incorrectly responded after the “stop signal”), and (3) go trials (trials where participants were not required to inhibit a response). Stop trials with SSDs of <50 ms were excluded to safeguard the adaptive difficulty process. Go and stop trials were matched based on SSD to ensure homogenous parameters for comparison of ERPs. Data were re-epoched at −100 ms to 500 ms relative to stop-signal onset. Latency or amplitude values for P3, N2, ERN, and CRN were calculated at the frontocentral EEG channels Cz and Fz. However, consistent with the literature, P3 values reported were those calculated at Fz, and N2, ERN, and CRN values reported were those calculated at Cz.Reference Wessel and Aron 38 ,Reference Dimoska, Johnstone and Barry57Reference Ramautar, Kok and Ridderinkhof59

EEG: inhibitory control ERP calculation

N2 and P3 ERPs were used to index underlying inhibitory control processes. The N2 amplitude was defined as the most negative value within the 200 to 300 ms interval post-stimulus onset, and the P3 amplitude as the most positive value within the 300 to 500 ms interval. A modified version of the COMPASS algorithmReference Wessel and Aron 38 was used to increase signal-to-noise ratio of N2 and P3 waveforms and to remove ICA-derived components which did not represent the N2 and P3 from the analysis (see Section B of the Supplementary Material). This allowed selection only of those ICA-derived components that represented the N2 across successful (n = 37) and failed (n = 37) stop trials. Similarly, components were selected that represented P3 onsets within successful (n = 28) and failed (n = 30) stop trials, and P3 amplitudes across successful (n = 28) and failed (n = 28) stop trials.

EEG: self-monitoring ERP calculation

ERN and CRN were used to index underlying self-monitoring. ERN was defined as the average peak (μV) from 0 to100 ms after failed stop trials, and CRN, as the average peak (μV) after successful stop trials. Preprocessed data were re-epoched from −400 to 800 ms around participant responses. Baseline means (ie, the average microvolt value) were calculated from 400 to 200 ms prior to participant response. The baseline averages were then removed all across trials.Reference Imburgio, Banica, Hill, Weinberg, Foti and MacNamara 60 This allowed the selection only of components representing ERN (n = 37) and CRN (n = 37).

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted on SPSS and PROCESS. Outliers (n = 5 across ERP data) were winsorized (based on Z scores of >3.29). First, to determine differences associated with successful/failed inhibitory control outcomes, independent sample tests were used to analyze group differences between (1) successful and failed stop trials across N2 amplitude and P3 onset and amplitude and (2) ERN and CRN. Then, bootstrapped linear regressions examined whether impulsivity, compulsivity, or their interaction were associated with (1) cognitive inhibitory control outcomes (SSRTs), (2) symptom severity (YBOCS scores), and (3) ERP indices of inhibitory control (N2 Amplitude, P3 Onset, and P3 Amplitude) and self-monitoring (ERN and CRN) across failed and successful stop trials. Impulsivity and compulsivity scores were mean centered according to the respective outcome group, and interaction terms were calculated accordingly to avoid multicollinearity. The unstandardized beta (B) has been reported for all significant (P < .05) main effects, that is, the amount of change in the outcome associated with every unit change in the predictor. Interaction effects were valid given that the independent variables (impulsivity and compulsivity, as operationalized above) were not correlated (P = .77). Significant interaction effects were followed up by (1) splitting the groups by impulsivity, and then within each group conducting correlations between compulsivity and the outcome measure and (2) assessing scatterplots to facilitate interpretation.Reference Albertella, Rotaru and Christensen 61 Age, gender, and anxiety were controlled in the model when they significantly correlated with the dependent variables, which was only evident for age and CRN.

Results

Participant characteristics

Participants (n = 40) were all within the mild (YBOCS-R score = 8–15) to moderate (YBOCS-R score = 16–23) range of OCDSs. The MINI clinical interview and DSM-5 criteria indicated that 15 participants met criteria for OCD, and 25 were experiencing subclinical OCDSs. The WEMWBS well-being scores were low for participants with mild and moderate symptoms, as compared to norms for healthy young adultsReference Tennant, Hiller and Fishwick 52 and were comparable to the norms for people accessing secondary care mental health servicesReference Bass, Dawkin, Muncer, Vigurs and Bostock 53 (see Table 1.). Participants showed longer SSRTs than the normative mean for healthy young peopleReference Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan and Tannock 62 showed more delay P3 onset latencies than previously found using the same SSTReference Wessel and Aron 38 and showed more negative ERN and CRNs compared to the normative data recorded at CzReference Imburgio, Banica, Hill, Weinberg, Foti and MacNamara 60 (see Table 1.).

Table 1. Participant Characteristics Across Variables and Normative Data for SSRT, P3 Onset, ERN/CRN, and Well-Being

Note: There are not reliable normative data for N2 and P3 amplitudes during the SST.

Abbreviation: CRN, correct-related negativity; ERN, error-related negativity; M, means; ms, milliseconds; SD, standard deviation; SE, standard error; SST, stop-signal task; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time; WEMWBS, Warwick Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale; YBOCS, Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; μV, microvolts.

OCDS severity and stop-signal reaction time

OCDS severity

Collectively, impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction accounted for 46.5% of variance in YBOCS scores ( $ {R}^2 $ = 0.465). Higher compulsivity was associated with significantly greater YBOCS scores (F(3, 36) = 3.32, P < .05, 95% CI [0.03, 0.19]), such that for every unit increase in compulsivity, YBOCS scores increased by 0.09 units (B = 0.09 [SE = 0.03]). Neither impulsivity nor the interaction between impulsivity and compulsivity was associated with variation in YBOCS scores (P’s > .31).

Stop-signal reaction time

Impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction were not associated with significant variation in SSRT (F(3, 36) = 1.998, P = .13).

Successful vs failed stopping across all ERPs

Consistent with the prior research using the same version of the SST,Reference Wessel and Aron 38 latency of the P3 onset was significantly earlier during successful as compared to failed stop trials (t(54) = 7.68, P < .001, 95% CI [77.26, 131.84]; Figure 2b). There were no significant differences between successful and failed stop trials in N2 or P3 amplitudes (t(71) = −0.48, P = .63; t(54) = −0.26, P = .79, respectively). There were no significant differences in negativity between CRN and ERN (t(71) = 0.49, P = .63; Figure 3).

Figure 2. Grand average of the N2 from the Cz electrode and the P3 from the Fz electrode. Abbreviation: FS, failed stop trials during the SST (ie, red arrow presented at 0 ms and participants incorrectly responded); GO, Go trials during the SST (white arrow presented at 0 ms and participants responded); ms, milliseconds; SS, successful stop trials during the SST (red arrow presented at 0 ms and participants correctly withheld a response); μV, microvolts.

Figure 3. Grand average of the ERN and CRN from the Cz electrode. Note: Response (“correct” response: withholding a response to the “stop signal”; “incorrect” response: responding to the “stop signal”) occurred at 0 ms, and the shaded area corresponds to the 0 to 100 ms period in which negativity was calculated. ERN and CRN index negativity after incorrect and correct responses, respectively. Abbreviation: CRN, correct-related negativity; ERN, error-related negativity; ms, milliseconds; μV, microvolts.

Inhibitory control

N2 amplitude

For failed stop trials, the combination of impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction accounted for 12% of variance in N2 amplitude ( $ {R}^2 $ = 0.12). The interaction between impulsivity and compulsivity was associated with significant variation in N2 amplitude (F(3, 33) = 1.48, P < .05, 95% CI [−0.013, 0.001], B = 0.007 [SE = 0.003]). No significant correlation was found between compulsivity and N2 amplitude for high and low impulsivity groups (r = −0.16, P = .56; r = 0.11, P = .63, respectively). Visual depiction of scatterplot data (Figure 4) indicated that the significant interaction effect was driven by a positive relationship between compulsivity and N2 amplitude among individuals with low impulsivity, and a negative relationship between compulsivity and N2 amplitude among individuals with high impulsivity.

Figure 4. N2 amplitude during failed stop trials as a function of compulsivity across low and high impulsivity. Abbreviation: IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; OBQ, Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire; UPPS-P, Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Impulsive Behavior Scale; μV, microvolts.

For successful stop trials, impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction were not associated with significant variation in N2 amplitude (F(3, 33) = 1.11, P = .36).

P3 onset

For successful stop trials, the combination of impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction accounted for 23% of variance in P3 onset ( $ {R}^2 $ = 0.23). Impulsivity was associated with a trend-level relationship with greater P3 onset latency (F(3, 24) = 2.33, P = 0.099), such that for every unit increase in impulsivity, P3 onset latency increased by 6.84 units (B = 6.84 [SE = 5.81]; Figure 5). Compulsivity and the interaction between impulsivity and compulsivity were not associated with significant variation in P3 onset latency (P’s < .28).

Figure 5. P3 onset during successful stop trials as a function of impulsivity. Abbreviation: UPPS-P, Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Impulsive Behavior Scale; μV, microvolts.

For failed stop trials, impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction were not associated with significant variation in P3 onset latency (F(3, 26) = 1.11, P = .36).

P3 amplitude

For failed stop trials, the combination of impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction accounted for 20% of variance in P3 amplitude ( $ {R}^2 $ = 0.20). The interaction between impulsivity and compulsivity was associated with significant variation in P3 amplitude (F(3, 24) = 2.03, P < .05, 95% CI [−0.002, 0.045], B = 0.019 [SE = 0.01]). No significant correlation was found between compulsivity and P3 amplitude in high impulsivity (r = −0.27, P = .40); however, there was a significant correlation between compulsivity and P3 amplitude in low impulsivity (r = −0.54, P < .05). Visual depiction of scatterplot data (Figure 6) indicated that the significant interaction effect was driven by a positive relationship between P3 amplitude and compulsivity among individuals with low levels of impulsivity.

Figure 6. P3 amplitude during failed stop trials as a function of compulsivity across low and high impulsivity. Note: Significant correlation between compulsivity and P3 amplitude in low impulsivity (r = −0.544, P < .05). Abbreviation: IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; OBQ, Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire; UPPS-P, Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Impulsive Behavior Scale; μV, microvolts.

For successful stop trials, impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction were not associated with significant variation in P3 Amplitude (F(3, 24) = 0.50, P = .69).

Self-monitoring

Error-related negativity

Impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction were not associated with significant variation in ERN (F(3, 33) = 0.63, P = .60).

Correct-related negativity

The combination of impulsivity, compulsivity, and their interaction accounted for 37% of variance in CRN ( $ {R}^2 $ = 0.37). Compulsivity was associated with larger CRN when controlling for age (F(4, 32) = 4.76, P < .05, 95% CI [0.01, 0.02]), such that for every unit increase in compulsivity, CRN increased by 0.02 units (B = 0.02 [SE = 0.01]; Figure 7). Impulsivity and the interaction between impulsivity and compulsivity were not associated with significant variation in CRN (P = .65, P = .54).

Figure 7. CRN as a function of compulsivity. Abbreviation: IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; OBQ, Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire; μV, Microvolts. Note: Trait compulsivity was associated with larger CRN (P < .05).

Discussion

This was the first study to investigate whether varying traits of impulsivity and compulsivity across OCDSs could be differentiated by inhibitory control and self-monitoring, as indexed by ERPs. The results indicated that ERPs indexing inhibitory control and self-monitoring did differentiate between impulsive and compulsive phenotypes in OCDSs and can contribute to our understanding of the neurocognitive drivers of these traits and symptoms. In particular, when impulsivity was high and compulsivity was low, failed inhibitory control was associated with enhanced N2 amplitude, reflecting high conflicts when trying to stop a behavior. Furthermore, as compulsivity increased, symptom severity also increased and CRN decreased, indicating reduced monitoring of successful inhibitory control. Taken together, two distinct phenotypes—(i) high impulsivity/compulsivity and (ii) high compulsivity—were identified, and their unique neurocognitive profiles were characterized—(i) poor inhibitory control (enhanced N2 amplitude) and (ii) impaired self-monitoring (reduced CRN), respectively.

The finding that high impulsivity and compulsivity were associated with enhanced N2 amplitude is consistent with recent evidence,Reference Kim, Lee and Choi 42 implicating a disrupted N2 amplitude as a marker of an OCD-specific frontal cortical dysfunction that subserves impaired inhibitory control. The current findings extend this evidence by identifying an enhanced N2 amplitude in individuals high in impulsivity and compulsivity at the milder earlier stages in OCDS progression, as most people in this study do not yet meet an OCD diagnosis. Source localization techniques have associated the N2 amplitude with prefrontal networks,Reference Bekker, Kenemans and Verbaten 63 , Reference Bokura, Yamaguchi and Kobayashi 64 including the IFG,Reference Swainson, Cunnington and Jackson 65 which drive inhibitory control.Reference Chamberlain and Sahakian 28 , Reference Aron and Poldrack 66 , Reference Lubman, Yücel and Pantelis 67 In line with this, evidence has positioned N2 amplitude to reflect the conflict between the need to stop and the response urge.Reference Kim, Kim, Yoo and Kwon 37 , Reference Lei, Zhu and Fan 68 Thus, people high in both impulsivity and compulsivity may experience higher conflicts, that is, enhanced N2 amplitude, between recognizing the need to stop unhelpful compulsive behaviors (eg, awareness of excessive handwashing) and succumbing to urges (eg, continuing to wash hands). These individuals, high in both impulsivity/ compulsivity, show greater OCD severityReference Prochazkova, Parkes and Dawson 24 and chronicity,Reference Fontenelle, Oostermeijer, Harrison, Pantelis and Yücel 23 higher risk for developing clinically elevated impulse control and compulsive disorders,Reference Tiego, Chamberlain and Harrison 48 and poorer prognosis.Reference Kashyap, Fontenelle and Miguel 25 Thus, the identification of specific inhibitory control impairments, reflected by an enhanced N2 amplitude, in those with high impulsivity and compulsivity could form the basis for the early detection of people that meet this high-risk phenotype, and guide the development of early interventions that are tailored to specifically modulate inhibitory dyscontrol.

Impairments across self-monitoring were associated with high compulsivity. The findings that indicated this were (i) a greater monitoring of failed stopping (enhanced ERN) across all individuals with OCDSs, as compared to healthy norms, (ii) high compulsivity, independent of impulsivity, associated with lower monitoring of correct performance (lower CRN), and (iii) high compulsivity, but low impulsivity, associated with poorer evaluation of failed performance (reduced P3 amplitude during failed stopping).Reference Dimoska, Johnstone and Barry 57 , Reference Ramautar, Kok and Ridderinkhof 59 Of note, the P3 amplitude during failed stopping on the SSTReference Dimoska, Johnstone and Barry 57 , Reference Ramautar, Kok and Ridderinkhof 59 , Reference Kok, Ramautar, De Ruiter, Band and Ridderinkhof 69 Reference Moeller, Barratt and Fischer 72 (not successful stopping) has not shown robust evidence for indexing either inhibitory control or self-monitoring processes, and thereby CRN will be used to index of impaired self-monitoring. These findings are consistent with robust evidence that has implicated impaired self-monitoring in OCD via ERN/ERN findingsReference Endrass, Klawohn, Schuster and Kathmann 32 , Reference Perera, Bailey, Herring and Fitzgerald 43 and disruptions across anterior cingulate cortex activity.Reference Fitzgerald, Welsh and Gehring 73 , Reference Van Boxtel, van der Molen and Jennings 74 Most of the prior studies have been conducted on individuals diagnosed with OCD and have found error-related alternations.Reference Endrass, Klawohn, Schuster and Kathmann 32 , Reference Perera, Bailey, Herring and Fitzgerald 43 This study provides further evidence of hypo-monitoring of correct performance and greater symptoms severity in highly compulsive people experiencing mild to moderate OCDSs, which is earlier in symptom progression than those included in prior clinical studies. In highly compulsive individuals, hypo-monitoring of correct performance, indexed by decreased CRN (eg, difficulty recognizing that hands are sufficiently clean), combined with hyper-monitoring of errors indexed by enhanced ERN (eg, excessively feeling that handwashing is incorrect), may reinforce compensatory, compulsive behaviors (eg, inability to feel that handwashing is complete).Reference Endrass, Schuermann, Kaufmann, Spielberg, Kniesche and Kathmann 31 , Reference Endrass, Klawohn, Schuster and Kathmann 32 , Reference Hajcak and Simons 75 This further neurocognitively characterizes (impaired self-monitoring) specific phenotypes (high compulsivity) at the milder end of the OCDS severity continuum.

Consistent with literature,Reference Smith, Mattick, Jamadar and Iredale 27 , Reference Lubman, Yücel and Pantelis 67 , Reference McLaughlin, Kirschner, Foster, O’Connell, Rasmussen and Greenberg 76 high impulsivity was associated with a nonsignificant trend toward a delayed inhibitory control process, indexed by delayed P3 onset.Reference Wessel and Aron 38 Taken together, those high in impulsivity may experience an additional, earlier slowing of the inhibitory control process. In other words, impulsive individuals may find it difficult to delay or stop behavioral urges, such as resisting compulsive handwashing, from the onset of the urge.

Neurocognitive phenotyping of impulsivity and compulsivity in OCDSs could allow at-risk people to be identified earlier for specific interventions that target the drivers of symptoms. More specifically, most participants in the current study did not meet a diagnosis of OCD and were at the early stages of symptom progression (mild–moderate OCDSs). Thus, the aforementioned findings collectively characterize the neurocognitive and mechanisms at the lower end of the dimensional spectrum of OCDSs, where people are likely in the early “at-risk” stages. This is the first step in enabling early detection of people that may be susceptible to OCD based on their neurocognitive and trait profile. Ultimately, this could enable the development of preventative or early treatments that specifically target the underlying mechanisms of a person’s symptoms. These tailored early interventions could, for instance, utilize the knowledge from neurocognitive phenotyping to specifically modulate impairments in self-monitoring in people that fit the high compulsivity phenotype, or modulate impairments across inhibitory control in people that fit the high impulsivity/compulsivity phenotype. This evidence directly informs the “phenotype-to-treatment” approach proposed by Yücel et alReference Yücel, Lee and Fontenelle 26 and is aligned with frameworks proposing the use of interventions to treat underlying drivers of symptoms, such as neurocognitive impairments, as opposed to categorical disorders.Reference Fontenelle, Oostermeijer, Harrison, Pantelis and Yücel 23 , Reference Chamberlain and Sahakian 28 , Reference Gillan, Robbins, Sahakian, van den Heuvel and van Wingen 35 Although investigations into interventions for targeting inhibitory control are growing,Reference Dhir, Teo, Chamberlain, Tyler, Yücel and Segrave 77 these are not yet established, and there has been minimal focus on remediating disordered self-monitoring. Thus, future investigations of interventions for subclinical OCDSs will benefit from targeting the associated neurocognitive mechanisms (ie, inhibitory control and self-monitoring) across impulsive and compulsive phenotypes to effectively remediate the underlying drivers of symptoms.

The current findings should be considered in light of a number of limitations. First, the focus on mild to moderate severities means that the findings cannot be generalized to more severe OCD presentations. This is important to address in future research as these individuals are likely to be most in demand for earlier assessment and prevention, or more rigorous treatment. The sample is also disproportionately biased toward women. In order to be able to generalize the findings to broader populations, this study should be replicated in larger and more gender-balanced samples. In addition, the lack of a healthy control group as a comparator makes it difficult to disentangle whether the neurocognitive phenotypes were OCDS-specific and not reflective of normative traits within healthy populations. Finally, the results reported from the split group follow-up approach, particularly the trend-level result for P3 onset, were based on small sample sizes. Thus, the study would benefit from replication with greater sample sizes.

Another important consideration is that, similar to findings by Wessel and Aron,Reference Wessel and Aron 38 P3 onset was a more robust marker of successful inhibitory control than N2 and P3 amplitudes. More specifically, it was the only ERP that differentiated between successful vs unsuccessful inhibitory control during the SST. It is possible that this was a result of the same task used in this study, and Wessel and Aron,Reference Wessel and Aron 38 as ERPs, are sensitive to task metrics.Reference Falkenstein, Hoormann and Hohnsbein 36 , Reference Gajewski and Falkenstein 78 , Reference Maguire, Brier and Moore 79 In addition, given that the P3 amplitude is proposed to be an indicator of the magnitude of the (successful or failed) inhibition response, it may have captured the motor inhibition response (clicking the key or withholding) regardless of stopping success.Reference Smith, Johnstone and Barry 80 , Reference Waller, Hazeltine and Wessel 81 Furthermore, the P3 amplitude, particularly during failed stopping, lacks specificity and has been proposed to also influence attentional processes.Reference Falkenstein, Hoormann and Hohnsbein 36 , Reference Carlson, Thái and McLarnon 82 , Reference Kopp, Mattler, Goertz and Rist 83 Thus, the evidence currently implicates the P3 onset as a more consistent indicator of successful inhibitory control, as compared to the N2 and P3 amplitudes.

Conclusion

The current study found distinct inhibitory control and self-monitoring profiles, indexed by ERPs, across impulsive and compulsive phenotypes in individuals with mild to moderate OCDSs. First, those high in both impulsivity and compulsivity showed greater conflicts when stopping a response during failed inhibitory control, indexed by enhanced N2 amplitude. Second, those high in only compulsivity showed impairments in self-monitoring that had not yet been documented in the literature, reflect by a reduced CRN, and showed worsened symptom severity. These findings support the use of ERPs for identifying neurocognitive phenotypes across OCDSs. This could inform a dimensional approach to characterizing OCDSs and its underlying mechanisms, that extends beyond binary diagnostic labels. Ultimately, mechanistic-based dimensional frameworks can enable earlier detection and thereby allow for early interventions that target the underlying neurocognitive mechanisms of distinct phenotypes across OCDSs.

Acknowledgement

The authors would like to acknowledge Associate Professor Jan Wessel and Professor Adam Aron for sharing their valuable version of the SST.

Financial support

This research was supported by an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship, the David Winston Turner Endowment Fund, and the Wilson Foundation. S.R.C.’s role in this research was supported by a Wellcome Clinical Fellowship (110049/Z/15/A and 110049/Z/15/Z).

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

Disclosures

S.R.C. receives an honorarium for editorial work at Elsevier. He previously consulted for Promentis. This research was funded in whole, or in part, by Wellcome [110 049/Z/15/Z and 110 049/Z/15/A]. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright license to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. M.Y. has received funding from Monash University, and Australian Government funding bodies such as the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC; including Fellowship #APP1117188), the Australian Research Council (ARC), Australian Defence Science and Technology (DST), and the Department of Industry, Innovation and Science (DIIS). He has also received philanthropic donations from the David Winston Turner Endowment Fund, Wilson Foundation, as well as payments in relation to court, expert witness, and/or expert review reports. R.A.S. is funded by the David Winston Turner Endowment Fund and has received funding from Monash University, NHMRC, the Wilson Foundation, and not-for-profit bodies such as Beyond Blue, nib Foundation, and the Sir Edward Dunlop Medical Research Foundation. The funding sources had no role in the design, management, data analysis, presentation, or interpretation and write-up of the data.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: S.D., M.Y., and R.A.S.; Data curation: S.D. and S.R.C.; Formal analysis: S.D., K.T., and L.A.; Investigation: S.D. and R.A.S.; Methodology: S.D. and R.A.S.; Project administration: S.D. and R.A.S.; Software: K.T.; Supervision: K.T., M.Y., and R.A.S.; Visualization: S.D.; Writing—original draft: S.D.; Writing—review and editing: all authors.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S109285292200075X.

Footnotes

These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Ruscio, AM, Stein, DJ, Chiu, WT, Kessler, RC. The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15(1):5363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grabe, HJ, Meyer, C, Hapke, U, et al. Prevalence, quality of life and psychosocial function in obsessive-compulsive disorder and subclinical obsessive-compulsive disorder in northern Germany. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000;250(5):262268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Bruijn, C, Beun, S, de Graaf, R, ten Have, M, Denys, D. Subthreshold symptoms and obsessive-compulsive disorder: evaluating the diagnostic threshold. Psychol Med. 2010;40(6):989997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Leppink, EW, Redden, SA, Grant, JE. Are obsessive-compulsive symptoms impulsive, compulsive or both? Compr Psychiatry. 2016;68:111118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivarsson, T, Melin, K, Wallin, L. Categorical and dimensional aspects of co-morbidity in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;17(1):2031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, LA, Cuthbert, B, Lewis-Fernández, R, Narrow, WE, Reed, GM. Three approaches to understanding and classifying mental disorder: ICD-11, DSM-5, and the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). Psychol Sci Public Interest. 2017;18(2):72145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontenelle, LF, Mendlowicz, MV, Versiani, M. The descriptive epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006;30(3):327337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollander, E, Kim, S, Khanna, S, Pallanti, S. Obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders: diagnostic and dimensional issues. CNS Spectr. 2007;12(S3): 513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boisseau, CL, Thompson-Brenner, H, Caldwell-Harris, C, Pratt, E, Farchione, T, Harrison Barlow, D. Behavioral and cognitive impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder and eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200(2):10621066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, K, Youssef, GJ, Dawson, A, et al. A psychometric validation study of the impulsive-compulsive behaviours checklist: a transdiagnostic tool for addictive and compulsive behaviours. Addict Behav. 2017;67:2633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zermatten, A, Van der Linden, M. Impulsivity in non-clinical persons with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Pers Individ Differ. 2008;44(8):18241830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luigjes, J, Lorenzetti, V, de Haan, S, et al. Defining compulsive behavior. Neuropsychol Rev. 2019;29(1):413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Le Pelley, ME, Chamberlain, SR, et al. Reward-related attentional capture is associated with severity of addictive and obsessive-compulsive behaviors. Psychol Addict Behav. 2019;33(5):495502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Le Pelley, ME, Chamberlain, SR, et al. Reward-related attentional capture and cognitive inflexibility interact to determine greater severity of compulsivity-related problems. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2020;69:101580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fullana, MA, Mataix-Cols, D, Caseras, X, et al. High sensitivity to punishment and low impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive patients with hoarding symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2004;129(1):2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grassi, G, Pallanti, S, Righi, L, et al. Think twice: impulsivity and decision making in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Behav Addict. 2015;4(4):263272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sohn, SY, Kang, JI, Namkoong, K, Kim, SJ. Multidimensional measures of impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: cannot wait and stop. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(11):e111739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Won Kim, S, Grant, JE. Personality dimensions in pathological gambling disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2001;104(3):205212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, SR, Stochl, J, Redden, SA, Grant, JE. Latent traits of impulsivity and compulsivity: toward dimensional psychiatry. Psychol Med. 2018;48(5):810821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fineberg, NA, Chamberlain, SR, Goudriaan, AE, et al. New developments in human neurocognition: clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectr. 2014;19(1):6989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, TW, Gillan, CM, Smith, DG, de Wit, S, Ersche, KD. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(1):8191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontenelle, LF, Oostermeijer, S, Harrison, BJ, Pantelis, C, Yücel, M. Obsessive-compulsive disorder, impulse control disorders and drug addiction. Drugs. 2011;71(7):827840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prochazkova, L, Parkes, L, Dawson, A, et al. Unpacking the role of self-reported compulsivity and impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. CNS Spectr. 2018;23(1):5158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashyap, H, Fontenelle, LF, Miguel, EC, et al. “Impulsive compulsivity” in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a phenotypic marker of patients with poor clinical outcome. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46(9):11461152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yücel, M, Lee, RSC, Fontenelle, LF. A new consensus framework for phenotyping and treatment selecting in addiction and obsessive-compulsive-related disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2021;78(7):699700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, JL, Mattick, RP, Jamadar, SD, Iredale, JM. Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;145:133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Sahakian, BJ. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20(3):255261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Fineberg, NA, Menzies, LA, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(2):335338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberlain, SR, Blackwell, AD, Fineberg, NA, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29(3):399419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endrass, T, Schuermann, B, Kaufmann, C, Spielberg, R, Kniesche, R, Kathmann, N. Performance monitoring and error significance in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychol. 2010;84(2):257263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endrass, T, Klawohn, J, Schuster, F, Kathmann, N. Overactive performance monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ERP evidence from correct and erroneous reactions. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):18771887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, LJ, Taylor, SF, Liu, Y, et al. Error processing and inhibitory control in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis using statistical parametric maps. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;85(9):713725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruchsow, M, Spitzer, M, Grön, G, Grothe, J, Kiefer, M. Error processing and impulsiveness in normals: evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;24(2):317325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, CM, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ, van den Heuvel, OA, van Wingen, G. The role of habit in compulsivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2016;26(5):828840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falkenstein, M, Hoormann, J, Hohnsbein, J. ERP components in Go/NoGo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychol. 1999;101(2):267291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M-S, Kim, YY, Yoo, SY, Kwon, JS. Electrophysiological correlates of behavioral response inhibition in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2007;24(1):2231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wessel, JR, Aron, AR. It’s not too late: the onset of the frontocentral P3 indexes successful response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Psychophysiology. 2015;52(4):472480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruchsow, M, Groen, G, Kiefer, M, et al. Response inhibition in borderline personality disorder: event-related potentials in a Go/NoGo task. J Neural Transm. 2008;115(1):127133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruchsow, M, Groen, G, Kiefer, M, Hermle, L, Spitzer, M, Falkenstein, M. Impulsiveness and ERP components in a Go/NoGo task. J Neural Transm. 2008;115(6):909915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shen, IH, Lee, DS, Chen, CL. The role of trait impulsivity in response inhibition: event-related potentials in a stop-signal task. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;91(2):8087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, M, Lee, TH, Choi, JS, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of altered response inhibition in internet gaming disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: perspectives from impulsivity and compulsivity. Sci Rep. 2017;7:41742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perera, MPN, Bailey, NW, Herring, SE, Fitzgerald, PB. Electrophysiology of obsessive compulsive disorder: a systematic review of the electroencephalographic literature. J Anxiety Disord. 2019;62:114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, WK, Price, LH, Rasmussen, SA, et al. The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: I. Development, use, and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46(11):10061011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(suppl 20):2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Parkes, L, Tiego, J, Aquino, K, et al. Transdiagnostic variations in impulsivity and compulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder and gambling disorder correlate with effective connectivity in cortical-striatal-thalamic-cortical circuits. NeuroImage. 2019;202:116070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiego, J, Oostermeijer, S, Prochazkova, L, et al. Overlapping dimensional phenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity explain co-occurrence of addictive and related behaviors. CNS Spectr. 2019;24(4):426440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiego, J, Chamberlain, SR, Harrison, BJ, et al. Heritability of overlapping impulsivity and compulsivity dimensional phenotypes. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):14378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulding, R, Anglim, J, Nedeljkovic, M, Doron, G, Kyrios, M, Ayalon, A. The Obsessive Beliefs Questionnaire (OBQ): examination in nonclinical samples and development of a short version. Assessment. 2010;18(3):357374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buhr, K, Dugas, MJ. The intolerance of uncertainty scale: psychometric properties of the English version. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40(8):931945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyders, MA, Littlefield, AK, Coffey, S, Karyadi, KA. Examination of a short English version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale. Addict Behav. 2014;39(9):13721376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tennant, R, Hiller, L, Fishwick, R, et al. The Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2007;5(1):63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bass, M, Dawkin, M, Muncer, S, Vigurs, S, Bostock, J. Validation of Warwick–Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (WEMWBS) in a population of people using secondary care mental health services. J Ment Health. 2016;25(4):323329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verbruggen, F, Logan, GD. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(11):418424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbruggen, F, Aron, AR, Band, GPH, et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. eLife. 2019;8:e46323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delorme, A, Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134(1):921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimoska, A, Johnstone, SJ, Barry, RJ. The auditory-evoked N2 and P3 components in the stop-signal task: indices of inhibition, response-conflict or error-detection? Brain Cogn. 2006;62(2):98112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G, Moser, JS, Yeung, N, Simons, RF. On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology. 2005;42(2):151160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramautar, JR, Kok, A, Ridderinkhof, KR. Effects of stop-signal probability in the stop-signal paradigm: the N2/P3 complex further validated. Brain Cogn. 2004;56(2):234252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imburgio, MJ, Banica, I, Hill, KE, Weinberg, A, Foti, D, MacNamara, A. Establishing norms for error-related brain activity during the arrow Flanker task among young adults. NeuroImage. 2020;213:116694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albertella, L, Rotaru, K, Christensen, E, et al. The influence of trait compulsivity and impulsivity on addictive and compulsive behaviors during COVID-19. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, BR, Ponesse, JS, Schachar, RJ, Logan, GD, Tannock, R. Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev Psychol. 1999;35(1):205213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bekker, EM, Kenemans, JL, Verbaten, MN. Source analysis of the N2 in a cued Go/NoGo task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;22(2):221231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bokura, H, Yamaguchi, S, Kobayashi, S. Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112(12):22242232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swainson, R, Cunnington, R, Jackson, GM, et al. Cognitive control mechanisms revealed by ERP and fMRI: evidence from repeated task-switching. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15(6):785799.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, AR, Poldrack, RA. Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 2006;26(9):24242433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lubman, DI, Yücel, M, Pantelis, C. Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation. Addiction. 2004;99(12):14911502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lei, H, Zhu, X, Fan, J, et al. Is impaired response inhibition independent of symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder? Evidence from ERPs. Sci Rep. 2015;5(1):10413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kok, A, Ramautar, JR, De Ruiter, MB, Band, GPH, Ridderinkhof, KR. ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. Psychophysiology. 2004;41(1):920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E, Barratt, ES, Wigg, C. Impulsiveness, aggression, reading, and the P300 of the event-related potential. Pers Individ Differ. 1997;22(4):439445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, MJ, Jacob, C, Unterecker, S, Fallgatter, AJ. Reduced response-inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder measured with topographic evoked potential mapping. Psychiatry Res. 2003;120(3):265271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moeller, FG, Barratt, ES, Fischer, CJ, et al. P300 event-related potential amplitude and impulsivity in cocaine-dependent subjects. Neuropsychobiology. 2004;50(2):167173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitzgerald, KD, Welsh, RC, Gehring, WJ, et al. Error-related hyperactivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(3):287294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Boxtel, GJM, van der Molen, MW, Jennings, JR. Differential involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in performance monitoring during a stop-signal task. J Psychophysiol. 2005;19(1):110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajcak, G, Simons, RF. Error-related brain activity in obsessive-compulsive undergraduates. Psychiatry Res. 2002;110(1):6372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, NCR, Kirschner, J, Foster, H, O’Connell, C, Rasmussen, SA, Greenberg, BD. Stop signal reaction time deficits in a lifetime obsessive-compulsive disorder sample. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22(7):785789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhir, S, Teo, W-P, Chamberlain, SR, Tyler, K, Yücel, M, Segrave, RA. The effects of combined physical and cognitive training on inhibitory control: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;128:735748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gajewski, PD, Falkenstein, M. Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/NoGo tasks. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;87(3):273278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguire, MJ, Brier, MR, Moore, PS, et al. The influence of perceptual and semantic categorization on inhibitory processing as measured by the N2–P3 response. Brain Cogn. 2009;71(3):196203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, JL, Johnstone, SJ, Barry, RJ. Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: the P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119(3):704714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waller, DA, Hazeltine, E, Wessel, JR. Common neural processes during action-stopping and infrequent stimulus detection: the frontocentral P3 as an index of generic motor inhibition. Int J Psychophysiol. 2021;163:1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, SR, Thái, S, McLarnon, ME. Visual P3 amplitude and self-reported psychopathic personality traits: frontal reduction is associated with self-centered impulsivity. Psychophysiology. 2009;46(1):100113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopp, B, Mattler, U, Goertz, R, Rist, F. N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996;99(1):1927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Schematic representation of N2, P3, and ERN/CRN during the stop-signal task (SST). Note: During the SST, participants are required to respond via button press (left or right arrow) to a Go signal (gray arrows); however, in some cases, the Go signal is followed by a “Stop Signal” (red arrow), and they are required to withhold the initiated urge to respond. The SSRT measures the time from the stop signal to the inhibition response, and thus indicates inhibition speed. Discrete neurocognitive components of the inhibitory control process are reflected in ERPs, as follow: N2 amplitude = preconscious awareness of the need to stop; P3 onset latency = onset of the inhibition process; P3 amplitude = the magnitude of inhibition response; CRN = monitoring of successful inhibition; and ERN = monitoring of failed inhibition. Abbreviation: ms, milliseconds; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time; μV, microvolts.

Figure 1

Table 1. Participant Characteristics Across Variables and Normative Data for SSRT, P3 Onset, ERN/CRN, and Well-Being

Figure 2

Figure 2. Grand average of the N2 from the Cz electrode and the P3 from the Fz electrode. Abbreviation: FS, failed stop trials during the SST (ie, red arrow presented at 0 ms and participants incorrectly responded); GO, Go trials during the SST (white arrow presented at 0 ms and participants responded); ms, milliseconds; SS, successful stop trials during the SST (red arrow presented at 0 ms and participants correctly withheld a response); μV, microvolts.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Grand average of the ERN and CRN from the Cz electrode. Note: Response (“correct” response: withholding a response to the “stop signal”; “incorrect” response: responding to the “stop signal”) occurred at 0 ms, and the shaded area corresponds to the 0 to 100 ms period in which negativity was calculated. ERN and CRN index negativity after incorrect and correct responses, respectively. Abbreviation: CRN, correct-related negativity; ERN, error-related negativity; ms, milliseconds; μV, microvolts.

Figure 4

Figure 4. N2 amplitude during failed stop trials as a function of compulsivity across low and high impulsivity. Abbreviation: IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; OBQ, Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire; UPPS-P, Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Impulsive Behavior Scale; μV, microvolts.

Figure 5

Figure 5. P3 onset during successful stop trials as a function of impulsivity. Abbreviation: UPPS-P, Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Impulsive Behavior Scale; μV, microvolts.

Figure 6

Figure 6. P3 amplitude during failed stop trials as a function of compulsivity across low and high impulsivity. Note: Significant correlation between compulsivity and P3 amplitude in low impulsivity (r = −0.544, P < .05). Abbreviation: IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; OBQ, Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire; UPPS-P, Urgency, Premeditation (lack of), Perseverance (lack of), Sensation Seeking, and Positive Urgency Impulsive Behavior Scale; μV, microvolts.

Figure 7

Figure 7. CRN as a function of compulsivity. Abbreviation: IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; OBQ, Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire; μV, Microvolts. Note: Trait compulsivity was associated with larger CRN (P < .05).

Supplementary material: PDF

Dhir et al. supplementary material

Dhir et al. supplementary material

Download Dhir et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 157.5 KB