Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-06T20:27:02.382Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive function as a generalized determinant of psychopathology and functional outcome in school-aged autism spectrum disorder: a case-control study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2022

Oscar W. H. Wong*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Ran Barzilay
Affiliation:
Neurodevelopment and Psychosis Section, Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA Lifespan Brain Institute (LiBI), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia and Penn Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Children's Hospital of Philadelphia and Penn Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
Angela M. W. Lam
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Sandra Chan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong
Monica E. Calkins
Affiliation:
Neurodevelopment and Psychosis Section, Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA Lifespan Brain Institute (LiBI), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia and Penn Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
Raquel E. Gur
Affiliation:
Neurodevelopment and Psychosis Section, Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA Lifespan Brain Institute (LiBI), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia and Penn Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
Ruben C. Gur
Affiliation:
Neurodevelopment and Psychosis Section, Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA Lifespan Brain Institute (LiBI), Children's Hospital of Philadelphia and Penn Medicine, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Oscar W. H. Wong, E-mail: oscarwhwong@cuhk.edu.hk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are challenged not only by the defining features of social-communication deficits and restricted repetitive behaviors, but also by a myriad of psychopathology varying in severity. Different cognitive deficits underpin these psychopathologies, which could be subjected to intervention to alter the course of the disorder. Understanding domain-specific mediating effects of cognition is essential for developing targeted intervention strategies. However, the high degree of inter-correlation among different cognitive functions hinders elucidation of individual effects.

Methods

In the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort, 218 individuals with ASD were matched with 872 non-ASD controls on sex, age, race, and socioeconomic status. Participants of this cohort were deeply and broadly phenotyped on neurocognitive abilities and dimensional psychopathology. Using structural equation modeling, inter-correlation among cognitive domains were adjusted before mediation analysis on outcomes of multi-domain psychopathology and functional level.

Results

While social cognition, complex cognition, and memory each had a unique pattern of mediating effect on psychopathology domains in ASD, none had significant effects on the functional level. In contrast, executive function was the only cognitive domain that exerted a generalized negative impact on every psychopathology domain (p factor, anxious-misery, psychosis, fear, and externalizing), as well as functional level.

Conclusions

Executive function has a unique association with the severity of comorbid psychopathology in ASD, and could be a target of interventions. As executive dysfunction occurs variably in ASD, our result also supports the clinical utility of assessing executive function for prognostic purposes.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder. In addition to the core defining features of social communication deficits and restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRB), comorbid psychopathology is common in children with ASD (Simonoff et al., Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird2008). Comorbidities include anxiety disorders, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and disruptive, impulse control disorders. Rarer syndromes such as bipolar disorders and schizophrenia spectrum disorders also occur at a higher rate in ASD than in the general population (Selten, Lundberg, Rai, & Magnusson, Reference Selten, Lundberg, Rai and Magnusson2015). With the onset is usually at school-age, these psychiatric comorbidities further aggravate the overall burden on ASD individual's academic and social functioning, resulting in substantial detrimental effects along the developmental trajectory (Chiang & Gau, Reference Chiang and Gau2016). A recent meta-analysis identified unmodifiable factors such as age, sex, and intelligence as predictors of psychiatric comorbidities in ASD, however, a significant amount of unexplained variance remains (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Kassee, Besney, Bonato, Hull, Mandy and Ameis2019). Hence, it is clinically relevant to identify other contributors, especially those that could be improved with intervention, such as the possible mediating factor of neurocognitive abilities in ASD.

As social communication difficulties constitute one of the two core features of ASD, the degree of social cognitive deficits translates directly into clinical severity and hence level of functioning (Bishop-Fitzpatrick, Mazefsky, Eack, & Minshew, Reference Bishop-Fitzpatrick, Mazefsky, Eack and Minshew2017). Additionally, a range of cognitive dysfunctions is evident from an early age (Brunsdon & Happé, Reference Brunsdon and Happé2014; Pellicano, Gibson, Maybery, Durkin, & Badcock, Reference Pellicano, Gibson, Maybery, Durkin and Badcock2005; Shalom, Reference Shalom2003; Tager-Flusberg & Joseph, Reference Tager-Flusberg and Joseph2003) and may act in concert to mediate behavioral and coping difficulties, resulting in externalizing and internalizing problems. A neuroimaging study revealed a shared white matter organization between ASD and ADHD at the corpus callosum, a region that underpins multiple cognitive processes (Aoki et al., Reference Aoki, Yoncheva, Chen, Nath, Sharp, Lazar and Di Martino2017). This provides a biological basis for the speculation that psychiatric comorbidities in ASD could be contributed by cognitive dysfunctions. Understanding how individual cognitive domains contribute to psychopathology is important to inform the development of targeted interventions.

Among the range of cognitive domains, executive function (EF) may be a unique mediator of psychopathologies and functional outcomes in ASD. It has been well-established that ASD individuals have weaker EF when compared with typically developing individuals (Demetriou et al., Reference Demetriou, Lampit, Quintana, Naismith, Song, Pye and Guastella2018). Not merely an epiphenomenon, EF deficits negatively impact quality of life (de Vries & Geurts, Reference de Vries and Geurts2015) and adaptive functioning (Pugliese et al., Reference Pugliese, Anthony, Strang, Dudley, Wallace, Naiman and Kenworthy2016) in ASD and contribute to theory of mind (ToM) impairments (Long, Horton, Rohde, & Sorace, Reference Long, Horton, Rohde and Sorace2018) and RRB (Iversen & Lewis, Reference Iversen and Lewis2021). Yet, the degree of executive dysfunction varies between ASD individuals (Geurts, Sinzig, Booth, & Happé, Reference Geurts, Sinzig, Booth and Happé2014). This heterogeneity is intriguing, since EF, as a set of higher-order cognitive functions, could compensate for other cognitive deficits in neurodevelopmental disorders through recruitment of other brain systems (Johnson, Reference Johnson2012). Potentially acting as both risk and protective factors, EF may therefore contribute to diverse functional outcomes and ranges of comorbid psychopathology in ASD (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Kassee, Besney, Bonato, Hull, Mandy and Ameis2019; Steinhausen, Mohr Jensen, & Lauritsen, Reference Steinhausen, Mohr Jensen and Lauritsen2016).

Studies have demonstrated that executive dysfunction in ASD could be related to psychopathology such as anxiety (Hollocks et al., Reference Hollocks, Jones, Pickles, Baird, Happé, Charman and Simonoff2014), depressive disorders (Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Kenworthy, Pugliese, Haroon, Popal, White and Martin2016), aggressive behaviors (Lawson et al., Reference Lawson, Papadakis, Higginson, Barnett, Wills, Strang and Kenworthy2015), and ADHD (Craig et al., Reference Craig, Margari, Legrottaglie, Palumbi, de Giambattista and Margari2016). However, as EF acts synergistically with other cognitive domains in daily functioning, concurrent examination of the entire range of cognitive functioning is needed to disentangle the complex interactive patterns and inter-relatedness among cognitive abilities and psychopathology. Furthermore, existing studies typically focus on only one to two types of psychiatric comorbidities, yet in real life, individuals with ASD may experience multiple psychiatric disorders with overlapping symptoms (Simonoff et al., Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird2008), alongside subthreshold symptoms (Caamaño et al., Reference Caamaño, Boada, Merchán-Naranjo, Moreno, Llorente, Moreno and Parellada2013). Rarer comorbidities such as schizophrenia and affective psychosis also warrant investigation given the severe disruption of well-being and everyday functioning that can ensue. Therefore, in the present study, using structural equation modeling (SEM), we investigated the interactions between neurocognitive abilities and dimensional psychopathology, both deeply and broadly phenotyped in a community-derived sample that included individuals with ASD, to address the research question of how different neurocognitive domains would be associated with comorbid psychopathology and functional level in school-aged ASD. We hypothesized that, after controlling for the inter-correlation between different cognitive domains, EF has a unique association with psychopathology and functional level in ASD.

Methods and materials

The Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort

Participants were from the Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort (PNC), a collaboration between the Children's Hospital of Philadelphia (CHOP) and the Brain Behavior Laboratory of the University of Pennsylvania (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Merikangas, Moore, Burstein, Behr, Satterthwaite and Gur2015). The PNC recruited children and young adults who were (1) ages of 8–21 years, (2) ambulatory in stable health, (3) proficient in English, (4) had cognitive abilities to participate in study procedures, and (5) with no significant physical conditions or developmental delay impairing motility or cognition (e.g. paresis, palsy, or intellectual disability) from a large pool of children (N = 50 293) previously genotyped as part of a genomic study in the CHOP health care network. Of note, participants were recruited from pediatric rather than psychiatric clinics, and thus the sample was not enriched for those seeking psychiatric services. Participants were excluded if they did not meet enrollment criteria or could not be contacted. The PNC comprised of 9498 youths that were racially (56% Caucasian, 33% African American, and 11% others) and socioeconomically diverse, with a socioeconomic status (SES) score calculated based on the participant's geocoded neighborhood data of residents in poverty, marital status, median family income, and crime rates. Higher scores indicate better SES (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Martin, Gur, Jackson, Scott, Calkins and Gur2016). The clinical phenotype assessment was administered in English to collateral informants including caregivers or legal guardians of probands aged 8–10; to both probands and collateral informants for those aged 11–17; and to probands only for those aged 18–21. Participants and their guardians (for participants <18 years) provided written informed consent or assent. Inclusion criteria for collaterals included proficiency in English, as determined by telephone screening during the initial recruitment call. All procedures were approved by the University of Pennsylvania and the Children's Hospital of Philadelphia Institutional Review Boards.

Identification of ASD diagnoses and matched controls

As a part of the PNC protocol, probands (participants aged 11–21) or their parents (participants under 18) were asked whether they/their child had ever been diagnosed with autism, pervasive developmental disorder, or Asperger's syndrome (i.e. ASD). From the entire PNC cohort, 291 cases reported ASD diagnoses. To verify these ASD diagnoses through independent documentation, a search of participants' CHOP electronic health record (EHR) was conducted, yielding a total of 202 participants with both PNC-reported and CHOP EHR documentation of their ASD diagnoses. Thereafter, another search of the entire PNC population was done for the presence of ICD codes in the autism spectrum [autism, pervasive developmental disorder (PDD), PDD-not otherwise specified, ASD]. This search yielded an additional 16 cases of ASD, who were also verified by additional documentation of ASD in their EHR. Participants were classified in the ASD group if they fulfilled the criteria of (1) documentation of ASD diagnosis in any health care provider's note or visit summary; and (2) presence of ICD code of ASD diagnoses. Participants who endorsed ASD diagnoses yet did not have any confirming documentation indicating ASD in their EHR were excluded (n = 89). Thus, the ASD group comprised 218 participants. Comparison subjects were identified from the remaining PNC population (n = 9280) using the R package ‘Matchlt’ (Ho, Imai, King, & Stuart, Reference Ho, Imai, King and Stuart2011) and matched to the ASD group based on age, sex, SES, and race in a 4:1 ratio, resulting in 872 matched non-ASD comparison subjects.

Neurocognitive phenotyping

Cognitive assessment was conducted using the Penn Computerized Neurocognitive Battery (CNB), a 1-hour computerized battery including 12 tasks in the following cognitive domains: social cognition (emotion identification, emotion intensity differentiation, and age differentiation), complex cognition (language reasoning, non-verbal reasoning, and spatial ability), episodic memory (verbal, spatial, and face), and EF (attention, working memory, and abstraction and mental flexibility). The CNB was validated as a reliable and robust measure of cognitive function across ages in both healthy and psychiatric samples (Almasy et al., Reference Almasy, Gur, Haack, Cole, Calkins, Peralta and Gur2008; Irani et al., Reference Irani, Brensinger, Richard, Calkins, Moberg, Bilker and Gur2012; Moore, Reise, Gur, Hakonarson, & Gur, Reference Moore, Reise, Gur, Hakonarson and Gur2015). For each task, accuracy and speed were measured and z-transformed, and an efficiency score was calculated by averaging the accuracy and speed z-scores. The cognitive performance was then evaluated by factor analysis, delineating the loadings of the tasks into four domains: EF, episodic memory, complex cognition, and social cognition (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Reise, Gur, Hakonarson and Gur2015). To ensure that the participants could complete the battery, an estimated intelligence (IQ) of ⩾70 with the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT-4) Reading subscale (Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2006) was used as a cut-off, as reading ability is relatively resistant to brain insult and is suitable for estimating premorbid IQ (Kareken, Gur, & Saykin, Reference Kareken, Gur and Saykin1995). The WRAT-4 Reading subscale score was converted into an age-standardized score (mean = 100 and standard deviation = 15) to provide an estimated IQ score.

Psychopathology and clinical phenotyping

Psychopathology symptoms were evaluated by trained and supervised assessors using a structured screening interview GOASSESS (Grand Opportunity Assessment), developed based on the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS) (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci and Ryan1997) with additional screening questions and dimensional ratings of distress and impairments associated with symptoms in each diagnostic section, and less restrictions to complete the diagnostic modules (Calkins et al., Reference Calkins, Merikangas, Moore, Burstein, Behr, Satterthwaite and Gur2015). Lifetime psychiatric diagnoses were determined if symptoms were endorsed with frequency and duration approximating DSM-IV disorder or episode criteria, with significant distress or impairments. To evaluate the youths' overall functioning, the Children's Global Assessment Scale (C-GAS) (Shaffer et al., Reference Shaffer, Gould, Brasic, Fisher, Aluwahlia and Bird1983) was used, considering their behavior in different environments, relationships, and disturbances associated with psychiatric symptoms. Higher scores in C-GAS indicates better functioning, with scores >70 indicating normal functioning (Bird et al., Reference Bird, Yager, Staghezza, Gould, Canino and Rubio-Stipec1990). To allow the dimensional quantification of psychopathology, psychopathology factor scores were generated using itemwise responses (at the symptom level) from clinical interviews across all assessed psychopathology domains. Based on previous factor analyses with responses from the informants (age 8–10) and the participants (age 11–21), the 112 item-level symptoms from the GOASSESS were reduced into five orthogonal dimensions of psychopathology, including four specific factors of anxious-misery (depressed mood, anxiety, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms), psychosis, externalizing behaviors, and fear (specific phobias such as agoraphobia and social phobia), as well as an overall psychopathology factor (i.e. p factor) (Shanmugan et al., Reference Shanmugan, Wolf, Calkins, Moore, Ruparel, Hopson and Satterthwaite2016). The p factor integrates multiple symptom domains including internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and is well suited for mapping psychopathology liabilities in childhood and adolescence (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault, Eley and Plomin2020).

Statistical analysis

Data management and statistical analyses were performed using the R 4.0.3 statistical environment (R Studio, Version 1.4). Missing data for neurocognitive and psychopathology scores were handled with multiple imputations using the R package ‘Amelia’. The variables to be used were included in the imputation model. Given the large sample size, 10 imputations were used to generate a combined imputed dataset for subsequent analyses. Descriptive statistics including demographics and clinical characteristics were examined with χ2 tests for categorical data and independent t tests or Mann–Whitney's U test for continuous data upon normality checking. Comparisons of neurocognitive efficiency, psychopathology, and C-GAS between ASD and non-ASD participants were analyzed using multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), preceded by correlation analysis for checking potential correlations.

For the main analyses, SEM was conducted to examine mediated relationships between the measured variables using Mplus, Version 7 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2012). Three models were fitted to test the predictive effects of different neurocognitive domains on psychopathological and functional outcomes. Models 1 and 2 examined the predictive relationships between ASD, neurocognitive efficiency, and the p factor (model 1), and the four factors of psychopathology (anxious-misery, psychosis, externalizing, and fear) (model 2) respectively. Model 3 examined the predictive relationships between ASD, neurocognitive efficiency, and general functional level (C-GAS). Covariates were adjusted for all three models upon correlation checks. Mediation analysis was conducted to assess the mediating pathways in the final step.

Model fit and path estimates (β coefficients) for the models were estimated with Maximum Likelihood (MLR) estimation with robust standard errors to accommodate potential non-normality in the variables. Model fit indices included χ2 statistics with its degree of freedom (df) and p values, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) (Steiger, Reference Steiger1990) and its 90% confidence interval (CI), the comparative fit index (CFI) (Bentler, Reference Bentler1990), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) test. RMSEA ⩽ 0.05 and ⩽0.08 indicate a good model fit and reasonable model fit, respectively. CFI ⩾ 0.90 suggest a reasonably good model fit, although a CFI ⩾ 0.95 is preferable (Kline, Reference Kline2015). SRMR ⩽ 0.08 is generally considered a good fit of model (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). The 95% bias-corrected bootstrapping and the Benjamini–Hochberg's false discovery rate (FDR) method to control for multiple comparisons were applied to all path effect estimations in mediation analysis, with standardized estimates and CIs reported.

Results

Participants' demographics, neurocognitive and psychopathological phenotyping, and preliminary analysis of covariates

There were 171 males and 47 females in the ASD group (mean age = 12.27, s.d. = 3.02) and 684 males and 188 females in the non-ASD group (mean age = 12.33, s.d. = 3.30). The two groups were not significantly different in race, participants' and parental educational level, estimated IQ, and SES. Functional level was significantly lower in the ASD group (online Supplementary Table S1). Correlation analysis was conducted to identify potential covariates (online Supplementary Table S2). As sex, age, race, and SES correlate with neurocognitive efficiencies and psychopathology factors and could confound outcome measurements, they were controlled as covariates in the subsequent MANCOVA and SEM.

Two separate MANCOVAs were conducted using ASD diagnosis as the fixed factor, with neurocognitive efficiencies and psychopathology factors as the dependent variables respectively. Results revealed significant effects of ASD on both neurocognitive efficiencies [Pillai's Trace = 0.057, F (4, 1068) = 16.136, p < 0.001, η 2 = 0.014] and psychopathology factors [Pillai's Trace = 0.111, F (4, 1064) = 26.659, p < 0.001, η 2 = 0.043]. The univariate F tests showed significantly lower efficiency in all neurocognitive domains and significantly higher scores in all psychopathology factors in ASD participants, when compared with non-ASD participants. The neurocognitive and psychopathology profiles of ASD and non-ASD participants are summarized in Table 1 and illustrated in online Supplementary Fig. S1.

Table 1. Summary of MANCOVA on neurocognitive efficiencies and psychopathology factors between ASD and non-ASD groups

Note. Results were controlled for sex, age, race, and socioeconomic status.

Structural equation modeling

SEMs were first conducted to examine the mediating pathways, followed by mediation analyses to test for specific indirect path effects. p factor score (model 1), four psychopathology factors (model 2), and C-GAS (model 3) were regressed on the predictive variables (diagnosis of ASD) and all mediator variables (the four domains of neurocognitive efficiencies).

Model 1 demonstrated reasonably good model fit [χ2 (5) = 28.525, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.987, RMSEA = 0.066, 90% CI 0.044–0.090, SRMR = 0.015]. Path estimates (standardized coefficients) of model 1 were illustrated in Fig. 1. Mediation analysis revealed a significant direct effect of ASD on the p factor [β = 0.646, p < 0.001], with social cognition [β = 0.042, p = 0.008], complex reasoning [β = 0.029, p = 0.042], and EF [β = 0.026, p = 0.033] as significant indirect pathways. Similarly, model 2 had a good model fit [χ2 (16) = 64.925, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.990, RMSEA = 0.053, 90% CI 0.040–0.067, SRMR = 0.018]. ASD had significant direct effects on all four psychopathology factors of anxious-misery [β = 0.741 (p < 0.001)], psychosis [β = 0.664 (p < 0.001)], externalizing [β = 0.777 (p < 0.001)], and fear [β = 0.584 (p < 0.001)]. Subsequent analyses showed differential mediating patterns of individual neurocognitive domain on psychopathology: social cognition significantly mediated the relationships between ASD and anxious-misery [β = 0.22, p = 0.009], psychosis [β = 0.040, p = 0.013], and fear [β = 0.037, p = 0.017]; complex reasoning mediated the paths between ASD and anxious-misery [β = 0.011, p = 0.044], and psychosis [β = 0.025, p = 0.045]; whereas the mediating effect of memory efficiency was only significant between ASD and anxious-misery but in a negative direction [β = −0.022, p = 0.021], i.e. better memory efficiency in ASD predicts higher factor score of anxious-misery. EF was the only domain that is a significant mediator of ASD and all four psychopathology factors of anxious-misery [β = 0.012, p = 0.036], psychosis [β = 0.021, p = 0.049], externalizing [β = 0.024, p = 0.037], and fear [β = 0.022, p = 0.043] (Fig. 2). Model 3 also had a good fit [χ2 (4) = 10.974, p = 0.027; CFI = 0.996, RMSEA = 0.040, 90% CI 0.012–0.069, SRMR = 0.011], with ASD significantly predicting C-GAS directly [β = −1.150, p < 0.001]. EF efficiency was the only significant mediating path [β = −0.031, p = 0.023] between ASD and the C-GAS (total indirect effect: β = −0.051, p = 0.032) (Fig. 3). Results of the goodness of fit statistics and the mediation path estimates of the SEMs are summarized in online Supplementary Table S3 and Table 2 respectively.

Fig. 1. Structural regression model for the effect of ASD on the p factor, mediated by neurocognitive function efficiencies. Presented estimates are β coefficients, with statistically significant paths shown in solid lines. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Fig. 2. Structural regression model for the effect of ASD on the four psychopathology factors, mediated by neurocognitive function efficiencies. Presented estimates are β coefficients, with statistically significant paths shown in solid lines. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Fig. 3. Structural regression model for the effect of ASD on functional level, as measured by the Children's Global Assessment Scale (C-GAS), mediated by neurocognitive function efficiencies. Presented estimates are β coefficients, with statistically significant paths shown in solid lines. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Table 2. Mediation path estimates of the three structural equation models

Note. ASD, autism spectrum disorder; C-GAS, Children's Global Assessment Scale. Results were controlled for sex, age, race, and socioeconomic status, 95% bootstrap confidence interval, with Benjamini–Hochberg's FDR method corrected for multiplicity.

With the entire cohort of participants with normal IQ (⩾70), estimated IQ by WRAT-4 did not correlate with neurocognitive efficiencies and psychopathology factors. This is consistent with results of previous studies that showed reading ability being weaker at predicting performance in specific cognitive domains (Schretlen, Buffington, Meyer, & Pearlson, Reference Schretlen, Buffington, Meyer and Pearlson2005), especially in average and above-average IQ populations (Diaz-Asper, Schretlen, & Pearlson, Reference Diaz-Asper, Schretlen and Pearlson2004). However, as IQ can exert a ceiling effect on neurocognitive performance, sensitivity analyses were carried out by including estimated IQ as an additional covariate in the MANCOVA and the three SEM models. Regarding the MANCOVA, the ASD group remained to have lower cognitive efficiencies and higher psychopathology (online Supplementary Table S4). For the SEM models, incorporating estimated IQ did not result in significant ΔR 2 for all three models (models 1a–3a, online Supplementary Figs S2–S4) to suggest improvement of the model fit (online Supplementary Table S3). EF remained as the only neurocognitive domain that mediated the path between ASD and all psychopathology factors and C-GAS (online Supplementary Table S5).

Discussion

Psychopathology and neurocognition of the community-derived sample of ASD

In the present study, the community-derived group of ASD with normal IQ represented a spectrum of varying functional levels and encompassed the full range of psychopathology. Compared to non-ASD, the ASD population had poorer current global functioning characterized by higher severity in all four specific domains of psychopathology and overall psychopathology. Neurocognitive efficiencies were also poorer in the ASD group across all four cognitive domains. Of note, the effect sizes for all these comparisons were small, which aligns with the heterogeneity in the degree of cognitive impairment (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Abrams, Rosenberg-Lee, Iuculano, Wakeman, Prathap and Menon2019; Geurts et al., Reference Geurts, Sinzig, Booth and Happé2014) and prevalence of psychiatric comorbidities in ASD (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Kassee, Besney, Bonato, Hull, Mandy and Ameis2019). Given that different cognitive functioning could be influenced by an overarching general factor such as global efficiency (Barbey, Reference Barbey2018), as reflected in the high degree of correlations among different domains within our sample (online Supplementary Table S2), the SEMs employed were adjusted for their inter-correlations to estimate specific mediating effects of individual cognitive domains on psychopathology.

Social cognition, complex cognition, and memory differentially predict psychopathology factors, but not functional level

As a core deficit of ASD, poorer social cognitive functioning predicts the severity of the p factor, as well as specific factors of anxious-misery, fear, and psychosis. The severity of this core deficit may translate directly into difficulties in social situations (Bishop-Fitzpatrick et al., Reference Bishop-Fitzpatrick, Mazefsky, Eack and Minshew2017), and result in mood disturbances and fearful responses. As captured by the social cognition tasks, difficulties in reading emotions, an ability fundamental to understanding others' intentions, were shown to be associated with proneness to psychosis at the population level (Germine & Hooker, Reference Germine and Hooker2011). Delusional ideas could result from misunderstanding others' emotions, intentions, and subsequent mentalization failure (Chung, Barch, & Strube, Reference Chung, Barch and Strube2014), contributing to common subthreshold psychotic symptoms and experiences among ASD individuals (Kiyono et al., Reference Kiyono, Morita, Morishima, Fujikawa, Yamasaki, Nishida and Kasai2020).

On the other hand, complex cognition predicts specific factors of anxious-misery and psychosis. The tasks loaded into this domain measured fluid intelligence (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Reise, Gur, Hakonarson and Gur2015), which is strongly associated with the general g factor in intelligence (Reynolds & Keith, Reference Reynolds and Keith2017). The relationship between intelligence and internalizing symptoms is not straightforward: anxiety and IQ were found to have a quadratic relationship, with the highest level of anxiety clustered in ASD individuals with borderline intellectual disability, while depression and IQ was found to be positively associated (Edirisooriya, Dykiert, & Auyeung, Reference Edirisooriya, Dykiert and Auyeung2020; Service et al., Reference Service, Vargas Upegui, Castaño Ramírez, Port, Moore, Munoz Umanes and Freimer2020). In our current sample of normal IQ ASD, complex cognition predicted negatively the factor of anxious-misery, which was loaded by symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and depression (Shanmugan et al., Reference Shanmugan, Wolf, Calkins, Moore, Ruparel, Hopson and Satterthwaite2016). The negative association of complex cognition with the psychosis factor was coherent with the known interaction effect of IQ with genetic susceptibility to schizophrenia (Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015). Intelligence also mediates the common ‘jumping to conclusions’ reasoning bias in schizophrenia (Tripoli et al., Reference Tripoli, Quattrone, Ferraro, Gayer-Anderson, Rodriguez, La Cascia and Di Forti2021).

For memory, mediation analyses suggested that better memory was predictive of more severe psychopathology in the anxious-misery factor. It has been observed that episodic memory correlates with self-awareness in autism (Toichi, Reference Toichi, Boucher and Bowler2008), and the self-awareness of multiple deficits of ASD may underpin anxiety symptoms (Edirisooriya et al., Reference Edirisooriya, Dykiert and Auyeung2020).

It is worth noting that some results of the present study were incongruent with previous studies. For example, social functioning was reported to contribute to externalizing behaviors (Shea, Payne, & Russo, Reference Shea, Payne and Russo2018), while another study found no association between social cognition and anxiety and depressive features (Hollocks et al., Reference Hollocks, Jones, Pickles, Baird, Happé, Charman and Simonoff2014). This discrepancy could be due to our control of the inter-correlations among different cognitive domains, such that some cognitive domains no longer stand out as unique mediating paths. Moreover, social functioning and cognition measured in previous studies were parent-reported and focused mostly on ToM skills, whereas our social cognition tasks focused mainly on emotional recognition and differentiation, hence the results are not directly comparable.

While these three domains of cognition (social cognition, complex cognition, and memory) all contributed differentially to the spectrum of psychopathology, none of them significantly determined functional outcomes (C-GAS) in ASD. This result, especially with regards to the complex cognition domain, challenges the common conceptualization of ‘high-functioning autism’, which posits ASD individuals with high IQ having neurotypical or even superior levels of functioning. Our results suggested that fluid intelligence may not be a unique determining factor of outcomes of ASD. This aligns with a recent study showing that IQ may be an imprecise marker for functional abilities in ASD without intellectual disabilities (Alvares et al., Reference Alvares, Bebbington, Cleary, Evans, Glasson, Maybery and Whitehouse2020).

A generalized impact of executive function on psychopathology and functional outcome in ASD

EF encompasses separable constructs such as working memory, inhibitory control, planning, and switching (Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000). However, as opposed to fractionated impairments, a recent meta-analysis supports a unitary executive dysfunction underpinned by brain connectivity aberrations in ASD (Demetriou et al., Reference Demetriou, Lampit, Quintana, Naismith, Song, Pye and Guastella2018). Accordingly, EF was considered as a unitary construct in the present study.

In the SEM models that controlled for the known predictive factors for comorbid psychopathology in ASD (i.e. age, sex, and exclusion of sub-normal IQ) (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Kassee, Besney, Bonato, Hull, Mandy and Ameis2019), our results demonstrated the independent contribution of EF to all psychopathology factors and functional level. Executive dysfunction as a transdiagnostic risk factor for major psychiatric syndromes in youths has been well-demonstrated (Lynch, Sunderland, Newton, & Chapman, Reference Lynch, Sunderland, Newton and Chapman2021). In developing children, executive dysfunction may serve as an intermediate phenotype that predicts later psychopathology through compromising skills of adaptation and conflicts resolution (Romer & Pizzagalli, Reference Romer and Pizzagalli2021). Thus, the current study shows that ASD may not be exceptional. However, given that social cognition deficit is a core feature of ASD, it is intriguing that social cognition did not predict functional levels of ASD as EF did. This result echoed the postulation that the deficits or strengths of EF in ASD may accentuate or diminish the real-life impact of impairments conferred by other domains of cognitive deficits in neurodevelopmental disorders (Johnson, Reference Johnson2012). Taken together, EF could be an important prognostic factor in ASD warranting the clinical value of assessing and subtyping executive dysfunction in ASD (Geurts et al., Reference Geurts, Sinzig, Booth and Happé2014).

In a longitudinal study following ASD children from the age of two, diminished core features of ASD to subclinical level in early adulthood among ASD individuals without intellectual disabilities were predicted by early improvements in RRB symptoms and less hyperactive symptoms during childhood, but not verbal IQ and severity of social deficits at baseline (Anderson, Liang, & Lord, Reference Anderson, Liang and Lord2014). Both RRB and hyperactivity symptoms in ASD were shown to be mediated by underlying executive dysfunction (Iversen & Lewis, Reference Iversen and Lewis2021; Lukito et al., Reference Lukito, Jones, Pickles, Baird, Happé, Charman and Simonoff2017). Another longitudinal study revealed that EF at 5 years old predicted ToM skills and adaptive abilities 12 years later (Kenny, Cribb, & Pellicano, Reference Kenny, Cribb and Pellicano2019), in line with findings that working memory and cognitive flexibility are needed for ToM development (Bock, Gallaway, & Hund, Reference Bock, Gallaway and Hund2015; Lecce & Bianco, Reference Lecce and Bianco2018) by holding and switching between thoughts and intentions of self and others. Hence, the potential compensatory mechanisms conferred by EF in ASD may have a long-term impact on their development.

Executive function as a possible linkage between genetic risk of ASD and the diverse clinical outcome

From a biological perspective, genetic variants identified as conferring risks for ASD were highly pleiotropic, as they are also associated with the psychiatric comorbidities of ASD including ADHD, depressive disorders, and schizophrenia (Thapar & Rutter, Reference Thapar and Rutter2020). While mechanisms of the pleiotropic effects remain elusive, the common variants identified have implied roles in neuronal functions and corticogenesis (Grove et al., Reference Grove, Ripke, Als, Mattheisen, Walters, Won and Børglum2019). EF, mediated mostly by the prefrontal cortex and its associated network (Alvarez & Emory, Reference Alvarez and Emory2006), could be a common pathway between the genetic risks of ASD and its pleiotropic effects on diverse clinical phenotypes. This possibility is supported by a recent study showing that polygenic risk scores of ASD predict executive dysfunction (Torske et al., Reference Torske, Nærland, Bettella, Bjella, Malt, Høyland and Andreassen2020). Our empirical observational findings therefore warrant further investigations into the underlying mechanism through large-scale longitudinal studies along neurocognitive and psychopathology development of ASD whilst incorporating genomic data (Searles Quick, Wang, & State, Reference Searles Quick, Wang and State2021). Given putative evidence suggesting that EF in ASD could be improved through training and neuromodulation techniques (Kenworthy et al., Reference Kenworthy, Anthony, Naiman, Cannon, Wills, Luong-Tran and Wallace2014; Rothärmel et al., Reference Rothärmel, Moulier, Vasse, Isaac, Faerber, Bendib and Guillin2019), there is pressing anticipation by patients, caregivers, and medical professionals alike for an intervention that could alter the longitudinal course of this chronic disabling disorder. Nonetheless, the design of any treatment trials on executive dysfunction in ASD would not be complete without a sufficient understanding of the pathophysiology.

Limitations

There are several limitations in the present study. First, the cross-sectional nature of the study limits causal inferences, and longitudinal studies are needed to verify the effect of early development of neurocognition on later psychopathology, as the reverse of psychopathology impeding EF development was also found (Romer & Pizzagalli, Reference Romer and Pizzagalli2021) and bi-directional effects should also be considered. Though the stability of EF during childhood (Polderman et al., Reference Polderman, Posthuma, De Sonneville, Stins, Verhulst and Boomsma2007) to adolescence and young adulthood (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Altamirano, Corley, Young, Rhea and Author2016) was demonstrated, less is known whether this is the same for ASD, hence the current cognitive functioning captured by CNB may not represent the cognitive ability before and during the onset of the lifetime history of psychiatric symptoms measured by GOASSESS. Second, processing speed, another common impairment in ASD (Oliveras-Rentas, Kenworthy, Roberson, Martin, & Wallace, Reference Oliveras-Rentas, Kenworthy, Roberson, Martin and Wallace2012), was not specifically measured. The use of efficiency scores in the CNB that considered both accuracy and speed for all the cognitive domains, although being ecologically valid to reflect the real-life cognitive performance of the participants, could be confounded by a general lower processing speed. Future studies should examine how impaired processing speed may contribute to the executive dysfunction in ASD. Third, the PNC was not designed to study ASD and thus the clinical phenotyping methods were not specific to ASD, and the lack of severity measurement of the core features of ASD may account for the variability of comorbid psychopathology. Subdomains of EF such as inhibitory control and fluency were not considered in this study, which should be included in the future for more accurate representations of EF. The assessment of social cognition was limited to facial emotion recognition and differentiation, and did not capture higher-order ToM and mentalization processes. Fourth, our study did not include participants with intellectual disabilities, which has a prevalence of 30% within those with ASD (Lyall et al., Reference Lyall, Croen, Daniels, Fallin, Ladd-Acosta, Lee and Newschaffer2017). The presence of intellectual disability is likely to have major implications on functioning and hence longitudinal outcomes of ASD (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Liang and Lord2014). The mean age of the study population was also relatively young (~12 years) and certain forms of psychopathology, such as depression and psychosis, tend to emerge at an older age. Future studies should examine the effects of intellectual disabilities and age.

Conclusion

Impaired functioning and psychiatric comorbidities are common in ASD, where neurocognitive abilities are significant mediators. Our study suggests that in normal-IQ ASD, EF has a generalized effect, independent of other cognitive domains, across a wide spectrum of comorbid psychopathologies and functional levels in the disorder. EF may be a locus for intervention to improve clinical outcomes of this disabling disorder.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722001787

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Kosha Ruparel for her support in the data retrieval for the present study.

Financial support

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, Grant/Award Numbers K08MH079364 (to MEC), K23MH120437 (to RB), R01MH117014, and R01MH119219 (to REG and RCG).

Conflict of interest

Dr Ran Barzilay serves on the scientific board and reports stock ownership in ‘Taliaz Health’, with no conflict of interest relevant to this work. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Allegrini, A. G., Cheesman, R., Rimfeld, K., Selzam, S., Pingault, J. B., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2020). The p factor: Genetic analyses support a general dimension of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 61(1), 3039. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almasy, L., Gur, R. C., Haack, K., Cole, S. A., Calkins, M. E., Peralta, J. M., … Gur, R. E. (2008). A genome screen for quantitative trait loci influencing schizophrenia and neurocognitive phenotypes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(9), 11851192. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121869.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvares, G. A., Bebbington, K., Cleary, D., Evans, K., Glasson, E. J., Maybery, M. T., … Whitehouse, A. J. O. (2020). The misnomer of ‘high functioning autism’: Intelligence is an imprecise predictor of functional abilities at diagnosis. Autism, 24(1), 221232. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319852831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 16(1), 1742. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, D. K., Liang, J. W., & Lord, C. (2014). Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 55(5), 485494. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aoki, Y., Yoncheva, Y. N., Chen, B., Nath, T., Sharp, D., Lazar, M., … Di Martino, A. (2017). Association of white matter structure with autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(11), 11201128. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbey, A. K. (2018). Network neuroscience theory of human intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 820. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.10.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, H. R., Yager, T. J., Staghezza, B., Gould, M. S., Canino, G., & Rubio-Stipec, M. (1990). Impairment in the epidemiological measurement of childhood psychopathology in the community. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(5), 796803. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199009000-00020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop-Fitzpatrick, L., Mazefsky, C. A., Eack, S. M., & Minshew, N. J. (2017). Correlates of social functioning in autism spectrum disorder: The role of social cognition. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 35, 2534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.11.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., & Hund, A. M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(3), 509521. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.888350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunsdon, V. E. A., & Happé, F. (2014). Exploring the ‘fractionation’ of autism at the cognitive level. Autism, 18(1), 1730. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361313499456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caamaño, M., Boada, L., Merchán-Naranjo, J., Moreno, C., Llorente, C., Moreno, D., … Parellada, M. (2013). Psychopathology in children and adolescents with ASD without mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 24422449. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1792-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calkins, M. E., Merikangas, K. R., Moore, T. M., Burstein, M., Behr, M. A., Satterthwaite, T. D., … Gur, R. E. (2015). The Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort: Constructing a deep phenotyping collaborative. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 56(12), 13561369. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, L., Abrams, D. A., Rosenberg-Lee, M., Iuculano, T., Wakeman, H. N., Prathap, S., … Menon, V. (2019). Quantitative analysis of heterogeneity in academic achievement of children with autism. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(2), 362380. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618809353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiang, H. L., & Gau, S. S. F. (2016). Comorbid psychiatric conditions as mediators to predict later social adjustment in youths with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 57(1), 103111. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chung, Y. S., Barch, D., & Strube, M. (2014). A meta-analysis of mentalizing impairments in adults with schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(3), 602616. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbt048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, F., Margari, F., Legrottaglie, A. R., Palumbi, R., de Giambattista, C., & Margari, L. (2016). A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 11911202. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S104620.Google Scholar
Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J. C., Pye, J. E., … Guastella, A. J. (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(5), 11981204. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Vries, M., & Geurts, H. (2015). Influence of autism traits and executive functioning on quality of life in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(9), 27342743. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2438-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diaz-Asper, C. M., Schretlen, D. J., & Pearlson, G. D. (2004). How well does IQ predict neuropsychological test performance in normal adults? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 8292. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704101100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edirisooriya, M., Dykiert, D., & Auyeung, B. (2020). IQ And internalising symptoms in adolescents with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(11), 38873907. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04810-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Altamirano, L. J., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., Rhea, S. A., … Author, D. P. (2016). Stability and change in executive function abilities from late adolescence to early adulthood: A longitudinal twin study HHS public access author manuscript. Developmental Psychology, 52(2), 326340. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000075.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Germine, L. T., & Hooker, C. I. (2011). Face emotion recognition is related to individual differences in psychosis-proneness. Psychological Medicine, 41(5), 937947. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291710001571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geurts, H. M., Sinzig, J., Booth, R., & Happé, F. (2014). Neuropsychological heterogeneity in executive functioning in autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 60(3), 155162. https://doi.org/10.1179/2047387714Y.0000000047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grove, J., Ripke, S., Als, T. D., Mattheisen, M., Walters, R. K., Won, H., … Børglum, A. D. (2019). Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(3), 431444. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, D. E., Imai, K., King, G., & Stuart, E. A. (2011). MatchIt: Nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. Journal of Statistical Software, 42(8), 128. https://doi.org/10.18637/JSS.V042.I08.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollocks, M. J., Jones, C. R. G., Pickles, A., Baird, G., Happé, F., Charman, T., … Simonoff, E. (2014). The association between social cognition and executive functioning and symptoms of anxiety and depression in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 7(2), 216228. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irani, F., Brensinger, C. M., Richard, J., Calkins, M. E., Moberg, P. J., Bilker, W., … Gur, R. C. (2012). Computerized neurocognitive test performance in schizophrenia: A lifespan analysis. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20(1), 4152. https://doi.org/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182051a7d.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iversen, R. K., & Lewis, C. (2021). Executive function skills are linked to restricted and repetitive behaviors: Three correlational meta analyses. Autism Research, 14(6), 11631185. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H. (2012). Executive function and developmental disorders: The flip side of the coin. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(9), 454457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kareken, D. A., Gur, R. C., & Saykin, A. J. (1995). Reading on the wide range achievement test-revised and parental education as predictors of IQ: Comparison with the Barona formula. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10(2), 147157. https://doi.org/10.1016/0887-6177(94)E0012-E.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., … Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2015). IQ and schizophrenia in a Swedish national sample: Their causal relationship and the interaction of IQ with genetic risk. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(3), 259265. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, L., Cribb, S. J., & Pellicano, E. (2019). Childhood executive function predicts later autistic features and adaptive behavior in young autistic people: A 12-year prospective study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(6), 10891099. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0493-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenworthy, L., Anthony, L. G., Naiman, D. Q., Cannon, L., Wills, M. C., Luong-Tran, C., … Wallace, G. L. (2014). Randomized controlled effectiveness trial of executive function intervention for children on the autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 55(4), 374383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiyono, T., Morita, M., Morishima, R., Fujikawa, S., Yamasaki, S., Nishida, A., … Kasai, K. (2020). The prevalence of psychotic experiences in autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin Open, 1(1), sgaa046. https://doi.org/10.1093/schizbullopen/sgaa046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling (4th ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lai, M. C., Kassee, C., Besney, R., Bonato, S., Hull, L., Mandy, W., … Ameis, S. H. (2019). Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(10), 819829. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30289-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawson, R. A., Papadakis, A. A., Higginson, C. I., Barnett, J. E., Wills, M. C., Strang, J. F., … Kenworthy, L. (2015). Everyday executive function impairments predict comorbid psychopathology in autism spectrum and attention deficit hyperactivity disorders. Neuropsychology, 29(3), 445453. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecce, S., & Bianco, F. (2018). Working memory predicts changes in children's theory of mind during middle childhood: A training study. Cognitive Development, 47, 7181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.04.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. R., Horton, W. S., Rohde, H., & Sorace, A. (2018). Individual differences in switching and inhibition predict perspective-taking across the lifespan. Cognition, 170, 2530. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukito, S., Jones, C. R. G., Pickles, A., Baird, G., Happé, F., Charman, T., … Simonoff, E. (2017). Specificity of executive function and theory of mind performance in relation to attention-deficit/hyperactivity symptoms in autism spectrum disorders. Molecular Autism, 8(1), 113. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-017-0177-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyall, K., Croen, L., Daniels, J., Fallin, M. D., Ladd-Acosta, C., Lee, B. K., … Newschaffer, C. (2017). The changing epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annual Review of Public Health, 38, 81102. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031816-044318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, S. J., Sunderland, M., Newton, N. C., & Chapman, C. (2021). A systematic review of transdiagnostic risk and protective factors for general and specific psychopathology in young people. Clinical Psychology Review, 87, 102036. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, T. M., Martin, I. K., Gur, O. M., Jackson, C. T., Scott, J. C., Calkins, M. E., … Gur, R. C. (2016). Characterizing social environment's association with neurocognition using census and crime data linked to the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. Psychological Medicine, 46(3), 599610. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, T. M., Reise, S. P., Gur, R. E., Hakonarson, H., & Gur, R. C. (2015). Psychometric properties of the Penn computerized neurocognitive battery. Neuropsychology, 29(2), 235246. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000093.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (2012). Mplus User's Guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Oliveras-Rentas, R. E., Kenworthy, L., Roberson, R. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. (2012). WISC-IV profile in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: Impaired processing speed is associated with increased autism communication symptoms and decreased adaptive communication abilities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 655664. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10803-011-1289-7/TABLES/4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43(7), 10441053. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polderman, T. J. C., Posthuma, D., De Sonneville, L. M. J., Stins, J. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Genetic analyses of the stability of executive functioning during childhood. Biological Psychology, 76(1–2), 1120. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCHO.2007.05.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugliese, C. E., Anthony, L. G., Strang, J. F., Dudley, K., Wallace, G. L., Naiman, D. Q., … Kenworthy, L. (2016). Longitudinal examination of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Influence of executive function. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(2), 467477. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2584-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, M. R., & Keith, T. Z. (2017). Multi-group and hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis of the Wechsler intelligence scale for children – fifth edition: What does it measure? Intelligence, 62, 3147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.02.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romer, A. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2021). Is executive dysfunction a risk marker or consequence of psychopathology? A test of executive function as a prospective predictor and outcome of general psychopathology in the adolescent brain cognitive development study®. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 51, 100994. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.DCN.2021.100994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothärmel, M., Moulier, V., Vasse, M., Isaac, C., Faerber, M., Bendib, B., … Guillin, O. (2019). A prospective open-label pilot study of transcranial direct current stimulation in high-functioning autistic patients with a dysexecutive syndrome. Neuropsychobiology, 78(4), 189199. https://doi.org/10.1159/000501025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schretlen, D. J., Buffington, A. L. H., Meyer, S. M., & Pearlson, G. D. (2005). The use of word-reading to estimate ‘premorbid’ ability in cognitive domains other than intelligence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 784787. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617705050939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Searles Quick, V. B., Wang, B., & State, M. W. (2021). Leveraging large genomic datasets to illuminate the pathobiology of autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(1), 5569. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0768-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selten, J. P., Lundberg, M., Rai, D., & Magnusson, C. (2015). Risks for nonaffective psychotic disorder and bipolar disorder in young people with autism spectrum disorder: A population-based study. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(5), 483489. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.3059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Service, S. K., Vargas Upegui, C., Castaño Ramírez, M., Port, A. M., Moore, T. M., Munoz Umanes, M., … Freimer, N. B. (2020). Distinct and shared contributions of diagnosis and symptom domains to cognitive performance in severe mental illness in the Paisa population: A case-control study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(5), 411419. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30098-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaffer, D., Gould, M. S., Brasic, J., Fisher, P., Aluwahlia, S., & Bird, H. (1983). A Children's Global Assessment Scale (CGAS). Archives of General Psychiatry, 40(11), 12281231. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790100074010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalom, D. B. (2003). Memory in autism: Review and synthesis. Cortex, 39(4–5), 11291138. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70881-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shanmugan, S., Wolf, D. H., Calkins, M. E., Moore, T. M., Ruparel, K., Hopson, R. D., … Satterthwaite, T. D. (2016). Common and dissociable mechanisms of executive system dysfunction across psychiatricdisorders in youth. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173(5), 517526. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15060725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shea, N., Payne, E., & Russo, N. (2018). Brief report: Social functioning predicts externalizing problem behaviors in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(6), 22372242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3459-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921929. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(2), 173180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinhausen, H. C., Mohr Jensen, C., & Lauritsen, M. B. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the long-term overall outcome of autism spectrum disorders in adolescence and adulthood. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 133(6), 445452. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 303314. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A., & Rutter, M. (2020). Genetic advances in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(12), 43214332. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04685-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toichi, M. (2008). Episodic memory, semantic memory and self-awareness in high-functioning autism. In Boucher, Jill, & Bowler, Dermot (Eds.), Memory in autism: Theory and evidence (pp. 143165). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511490101.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torske, T., Nærland, T., Bettella, F., Bjella, T., Malt, E., Høyland, A. L., … Andreassen, O. A. (2020). Autism spectrum disorder polygenic scores are associated with every day executive function in children admitted for clinical assessment. Autism Research, 13(2), 207220. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tripoli, G., Quattrone, D., Ferraro, L., Gayer-Anderson, C., Rodriguez, V., La Cascia, C., … Di Forti, M. (2021). Jumping to conclusions, general intelligence, and psychosis liability: Findings from the multi-centre EU-GEI case-control study. Psychological Medicine, 51(4), 623633. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171900357X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, G. L., Kenworthy, L., Pugliese, C. E., Haroon, , Popal, S., White, E. I., … Martin, A. (2016). Real-world executive functions in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Profiles of impairment and associations with adaptive functioning and co-morbid anxiety and depression. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 10711083. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2655-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2006). Wide range achievement test (4th ed.). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of MANCOVA on neurocognitive efficiencies and psychopathology factors between ASD and non-ASD groups

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Structural regression model for the effect of ASD on the p factor, mediated by neurocognitive function efficiencies. Presented estimates are β coefficients, with statistically significant paths shown in solid lines. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Structural regression model for the effect of ASD on the four psychopathology factors, mediated by neurocognitive function efficiencies. Presented estimates are β coefficients, with statistically significant paths shown in solid lines. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Structural regression model for the effect of ASD on functional level, as measured by the Children's Global Assessment Scale (C-GAS), mediated by neurocognitive function efficiencies. Presented estimates are β coefficients, with statistically significant paths shown in solid lines. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 4

Table 2. Mediation path estimates of the three structural equation models

Supplementary material: File

Wong et al. supplementary material

Wong et al. supplementary material

Download Wong et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1 MB