Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-20T07:01:54.032Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Polysomnographic parameters associated with cognitive function in patients with major depression and insomnia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 April 2024

Carlos Olivera-López*
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Sleep Disorders, Faculty of Psychology, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico Faculty of Higher Studies Zaragoza, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
Alejandro Jiménez-Genchi
Affiliation:
Clinical Services Unit, Sleep Clinic, National Institute of Psychiatry “Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz”, Mexico City, Mexico
David Ortega-Robles
Affiliation:
Clinical Services Unit, Sleep Clinic, National Institute of Psychiatry “Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz”, Mexico City, Mexico
Matilde Valencia-Flores
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Sleep Disorders, Faculty of Psychology, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
Selene Cansino
Affiliation:
Laboratory of NeuroCognition, Faculty of Psychology, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
Judith Salvador-Cruz
Affiliation:
Faculty of Higher Studies Zaragoza, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
*
Corresponding author: Carlos Olivera-López; Email: olivera168@comunidad.unam.mx
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To examine whether objective sleep parameters are associated with cognitive function (CF) in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) with chronic insomnia (CI) and whether the severity of these disorders is related to CF.

Method

Thirty patients with MDD with CI attending a tertiary care institution underwent two consecutive nights of polysomnographic (PSG) recording and a battery of neuropsychological tests, which included episodic memory, sustained attention, working memory, and executive function. The severity of MDD and CI was assessed by clinical scales. We examined the relationship between PSG parameters and CF, as well as whether the severity of the disorders is related to CF.

Results

Linear regression analysis revealed that total sleep time (TST) was positively associated with higher learning and recall of episodic memory, as well as better attention. Slow-wave sleep (SWS) showed a positive association with better working memory. Furthermore, wake after sleep onset (WASO) was negatively associated with episodic memory and lower attention. No significant relationships were found between the severity of MDD or CI with CF.

Conclusion

Both sleep duration and depth are positively associated with several aspects of CF in patients with MDD with CI. Conversely, a lack of sleep maintenance is negatively related to CF in these patients. These findings could help identify modifiable therapeutic targets to reduce CF impairment.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the most prevalent and severe mental illnesses worldwide, making it a significant public health issue. 1 Sleep disturbances are a common feature in MDDReference Tsuno, Besset and Ritchie 2 patients, with chronic insomnia (CI) being the most prevalent.Reference Staner 3 It has been reported that as many as 90% of MDD patients experience CI concurrently,Reference Moretto, Palagini and Dringenberg 4 and this comorbidity has been considered a distinct phenotype associated with greater dysfunctionReference Sun and Tan 5 and higher healthcare costs.Reference Torres-Granados, Santana-Miranda and Barrera-Medina 6 Cognitive function (CF) impairment is a frequently reported issue in both patient groups,Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell 7 , Reference Fortier-Brochu, Beaulieu-Bonneau, Ivers and Morin 8 particularly difficulties in attention, memory, and executive functions.Reference McIntyre, Cha and Soczynska 9 , Reference Cambridge, Knight, Mills and Baune 10 Until now, the etiology of these impairments remains incompletely understood,Reference Pearson, Uglik-Marucha and Miskowiak 11 and it is believed that sleep may be a factor associated with CF in patients with this comorbidity.Reference Biddle, Naismith, Griffiths, Christensen, Hickie and Glozier 12 This hypothesis is supported by the consistent association of objective sleep parameters, as determined through polysomnographic (PSG) data, with CF in healthy subjects.Reference Born and Wilhelm 13 Reference Chua, Fang and Gooley 19 However, it is important to consider that pharmacological treatment of MDD and CI may alter sleep patterns,Reference Wichniak, Wierzbicka, Walęcka and Jernajczyk 20 and the severity of these disorders could be related to CF.Reference Baril, Beiser and Sanchez 21 , Reference McClintock, Husain, Greer and Cullum 22 For instance, it has been reported that in patients with a current episode of MDDReference Cabanel, Schmidt and Fockenberg 23 and in those with subclinical depression,Reference Sutter, Zöllig, Allemand and Martin 24 a decrease in subjective sleep quality is associated with worse performance on tasks of psychomotor speed, cognitive flexibility, and semantic fluency. Furthermore, both subjective sleep quality and depression severity independently predict self-reported cognitive impairment, but only sleep quality is associated with objectively measured cognitive impairment.Reference Cha, Carmona and Cha 25 It has also been suggested that both shorter self-reported total sleep time (TST) and longer self-reported TST are associated with worse performance on attention and memory tasks (inverted U model).Reference Müller, Olschinski, Kundermann and Cabanel 26 Two studies that employed PSG found that shorter TST and increased wake after sleep onset (WASO) were linked to poorer performance on working memory and executive functioning tasksReference Mellor, Bucks, Maul, Sanders, McGowan and Waters 27 , Reference Wilckens, Kline and Bowman 28 in MDD patients. Research on insomnia patients, using PSG data and its relationship with CF, revealed a negative correlation between WASO and episodic memory recall.Reference Wilckens, Hall, Nebes, Monk and Buysse 29 Furthermore, a recent meta-analysis consistently reports that objectively short sleep duration (<6 hours) is associated with impairments in attention, memory, and executive functions.Reference Ren, Jiang and Guo 30 Until now, only one study has examined the relationship between sleep and CF in individuals with both MDD and CI using actigraphy. This study found that among men older than 50 years, poorer sleep efficiency (SE) was associated with reduced processing speed and executive function.Reference Biddle, Naismith, Griffiths, Christensen, Hickie and Glozier 12 Interestingly, the severity of neither MDD nor CI was associated with CF. In terms of disorder severity, there is consistent evidence suggesting that the severity of CI is negatively related to CF in general, particularly in memory tests.Reference Baril, Beiser and Sanchez 21 Meanwhile, the severity of MDD has shown negative correlations in some instances and no association with CF in others, as reported in recent reviews.Reference McClintock, Husain, Greer and Cullum 22 In summary, the available evidence indicates that sleep is associated with CF in both MDD and CI. However, the data have been limited due to subjective sleep assessments, insufficient control of sleep-related comorbidities requiring PSG assessment, and the monitoring of the effects of pharmacological treatment on sleep, as well as self-report-based assessments of CF.Reference Pearson, Uglik-Marucha and Miskowiak 11 , Reference Biddle, Naismith, Griffiths, Christensen, Hickie and Glozier 12 To date, there are no studies that evaluate with PSG data the relationship between sleep and CF in individuals with both MDD and CI. In addition, the limited number of studies employing PSG has hindered the exploration of potential relationships involving sleep stages, including the role of slow-wave sleep (SWS), and its relationship on episodic memoryReference Born and Wilhelm 13 Reference Hokett, Arunmozhi, Campbell, Verhaeghen and Duarte 16 and working memory,Reference Ferrarelli, Kaskie, Laxminarayan, Ramakrishnan, Reifman and Germain 18 as observed in studies with healthy subjects. Therefore, it is essential to examine the association between objective sleep parameters and CF in patients with MDD and CI who do not have medical, psychiatric, or sleep-related comorbidities and are not taking medication. This analysis would enable us to examine whether sleep is a factor related to CF in this sample and could help identify modifiable factors that may contribute to reducing cognitive impairment in these patients.Reference Biddle, Naismith, Griffiths, Christensen, Hickie and Glozier 12 Thus, the objectives of this investigation were as follows: 1) to examine the relationship between PSG parameters and CF in MDD patients with CI and 2) to assess whether the severity of MDD and CI is associated with CF. We hypothesize that both TSTReference Müller, Olschinski, Kundermann and Cabanel 26 , Reference Mellor, Bucks, Maul, Sanders, McGowan and Waters 27 , Reference Wilckens, Kline and Bowman 28 and SWS will be positively associated with CF, as observed in healthy subjectsReference Born and Wilhelm 13 Reference Hokett, Arunmozhi, Campbell, Verhaeghen and Duarte 16 , Reference Ferrarelli, Kaskie, Laxminarayan, Ramakrishnan, Reifman and Germain 18 and patients with insomnia,Reference Ren, Jiang and Guo 30 while WASO will have a negative association with CF.Reference Mellor, Bucks, Maul, Sanders, McGowan and Waters 27 , Reference Wilckens, Kline and Bowman 28 Additionally, we expected that the severity of CI,Reference Baril, Beiser and Sanchez 21 but not MDD, would be related to CF, primarily due to the persistence of cognitive impairments in remission of mood symptoms.Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell 7

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 30 participants, aged between 19 and 59 years, were recruited from the National Institute of Psychiatry “Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz” (INPRFM), a tertiary care hospital in Mexico City, Mexico. Inclusion criteria required participants to have received a clinical diagnosis of MDD by a psychiatrist in accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-V) 31 criteria, with concurrent CI as defined by the International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Third Edition (ICSD-3). 32 They were followed as outpatients and were not currently undergoing psychopharmacological treatment. No participant’s medication was discontinued for inclusion in this study. Exclusion criteria included the presence of any other psychiatric disorder (DSM-V), substance abuse, suicidal risk, serious health conditions, chronic diseases, neurodegenerative disease, high risk of obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), and evidence of any sleep disturbance other than insomnia as confirmed by PSG (patient flow is shown in Figure 1). The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The protocol was approved by the INPRFM Ethics and Research Committee (SC17070.0.), and all participants signed an informed consent form.

Figure 1. Patient flow. Abbreviations: MDD, major depressive disorder; CI, chronic insomnia; PSG, polysomnography.

Assessments

Clinical assessments

Depressive symptoms: The validated Spanish versionReference Dueñas, Lara, Walton, Granger, Dossenbach and Raskin 33 of the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology—Self-Report (QIDS-SR) was applied,Reference Rush, Trivedi and Ibrahim 34 with a higher score indicating greater severity of MDD. Additionally, specific cutoff points have been proposed, where <5 suggests the absence of depression, 6–10 indicates a mild disorder, 11–15 indicates a moderate level, and > 16 indicates a severe MDD.Reference Gili, Lopez-Navarro and Homar 35

Insomnia symptoms: The Spanish validated version of the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) was applied.Reference Fernandez-Mendoza, Rodriguez-Muñoz and Vela-Bueno 36 A higher ISI score suggests more severe insomnia, and specific cutoff points have been proposed that identify no insomnia (0–7); insomnia below the threshold (8–14); moderate insomnia (15–21); and severe insomnia (22–28).Reference Morin, Belleville, Bélanger and Ivers 37

Sleep assessment

Polysomnography (PSG): Two PSG studies were conducted following international standards (American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM)). 38 The first PSG aimed for habituation and to rule out any other sleep disturbances. For this study, electroencephalographic (EEG) variables with a full 10–20 montage, electrooculography (EOG), electrocardiography (EKG), electromyography (EMG) of the chin and anterior tibialis, and respiratory variables such as oronasal thermal flow, thoracoabdominal respiratory effort bands, and partial oxygen saturation were recorded. Data from the second night of recording were used for the analyses in this study to avoid the effect of the first night of sleep.Reference Hu, Shi and Li 39 For this night, respiratory variables and EMG of the tibialis were omitted, and a 9-channel EEG montage was recorded (Fz-A1, Cz-A1, Pz-A1, F3-A2, C3-A2, O1-A2, F4-A1, C4-A1, and O2-A1). The following parameters were obtained: TST, latency to non-rapid eye movement (REM) and REM sleep; SE (TST/time in bed x 100); WASO; number of awakenings and arousals; and the duration of each sleep stage: N1, N2, N3 (SWS), and REM. These variables are presented as descriptive data for the sample. However, only the parameters of TST (minutes), SE (%), WASO (minutes), and SWS (minutes) were included in the analysis as they have shown evidence of an association with cognitive functioning.Reference Ferrarelli, Kaskie, Laxminarayan, Ramakrishnan, Reifman and Germain 18 , Reference Müller, Olschinski, Kundermann and Cabanel 26 Reference Ren, Jiang and Guo 30

Neuropsychological assessment measures

Word List Memory Test: It is a measure of episodic memory consisting of the presentation of words in three trials to assess the memory curve (learning phase) and recall after 20 minutes. A higher score indicates a greater number of words recalled.Reference Ostrosky-Solís, Gómez, Ardila, Rosselli, Pineda and Matute 40

Attention test D2: It is designed to measure the capacity for sustained and selective attention, Reference Seisdedos 41 consisting of a selective search for relevant stimuli (letter “d” with two lines) within a matrix. The total number of correct answers was obtained as a score, which was used in subsequent analyses.

Letter–Number Sequencing Test: It is a subtest that is part of the Adult Intelligence Scale (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV))Reference Wechsler 42 and is focused on the evaluation of working memory. It involves the verbal presentation of a series of mixed numbers and letters, where the subject must first repeat the numbers in increasing order and then the letters in alphabetical order. Scores on this test range from 0 to 30, with a higher score indicating better working memory performance.

Trail Making Test (TMT): The test is divided into two parts: Part A provides a measure of visual and attentional skills, psychomotor speed, and visual tracking; and Part B assesses complex attention, executive control, and cognitive flexibility.Reference Reitan and Wolfson 43 The total time taken to complete the test was recorded, with higher scores indicating poorer performance in these functions.Reference Arango-Lasprilla, Rivera and Aguayo 44

Procedures

Patients newly followed at the INPRFM with a diagnosis of MDD and comorbid CI were invited to participate in the study. Those who agreed were referred to the sleep clinic at the same institute, where they underwent a clinical interview conducted by a specialist in psychiatry and sleep. During this interview, the presence of neurodegenerative disease was ruled out, as well as comorbidity with any other psychiatric disorders using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview,Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier and Sheehan 45 and the Berlin QuestionnaireReference Guerrero-Zúñiga, Gaona and Cuevas 46 was administered to identify individuals at high risk of OSA. Additionally, the QIDS-SR and ISI clinical scales were applied. Participants who met the inclusion criteria were scheduled for two consecutive nights of PSG recording, which took place 7 to 10 days after the initial evaluation. During this period, they underwent sleep monitoring (sleep diary) to ensure that they maintained their usual sleep schedules and were not sleep deprived. PSG recordings started between 22:00 and 23:00 hours and ended between 6:00 and 7:00 hours to coincide as closely as possible with their usual sleep schedules, which aligned with the average bedtimes of 80% of the participants, with each PSG recording lasting for 8 hours. At the end of the first night of recording, the participant left the laboratory and was instructed to go about their routine activities and return in the evening for the second PSG recording. Prior to the start of the second night, at approximately 19:00 hours, all participants underwent a neuropsychological test battery with an approximate duration of 60 minutes. All PSG recordings were scored according to the AASM manual for sleep staging and associated events 38 by an independent sleep specialist who was not involved in the research.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25. Percentages, means, and standard deviations of sociodemographic and clinical variables for the sample, as well as the PSG and CF variables, are reported. All variables underwent descriptive analysis, and those with skewness greater than ±2 were transformed into natural logarithms. We examined the relationship between PSG parameters and CF using multiple linear regression analysis for each PSG variable (TST, SE, WASO, and SWS) and each assessed cognitive variable. Due to the high multicollinearity among the PSG parameters, a multiple analysis would not have allowed us to identify the possible influence of each factor. We controlled for the effect of schooling and age by including them as covariates, considering their known relationship with CF.Reference Strauss, Sherman and Spreen 47 Finally, we examined the association between the severity of MDD, CI, and CF using multiple linear regression controlling for age, schooling, and PSG variables that were significantly associated with CF in the previous analysis. For each pair of variables examined, we a priori assessed homoscedasticity and linearity with the dependent variable.Reference Miles and Shelvin 48 To control for multiple regressions, we applied the Benjamini–Hochberg procedureReference Benjamin and Hochberg 49 (with a false discovery rate of 0.10).

Results

The sample consisted of 30 participants, 16 women and 14 men, with an average age of 37.5 (standard deviation) (13.2) years (range = 19–59) and a schooling age of 14.2 (3.6) years (range = 9–19). About 73.3% of the sample had undergraduate or graduate studies. With an average body mass index of 24.29 (3.59), participants reported consuming an average of 0.30 (0.55) alcoholic drinks per day and 0.52 (0.84) cups of coffee per day. The clinical scales indicated a high mean severity, both in the MDD (21.8 (5.4)) and in the CI (20.6 (3.8)). All variables exhibited skewness less than ±2, except for the TMT part B variable, which had a skewness = 3.83. This variable was transformed using natural logarithm (skewness = 1.19).

PSG and neuropsychological characteristics

All participants exhibited an apnea–hypopnea index (AHI) of 1.20 (1.26) and a periodic limb movement index (PLM) of 6.55 (7.90), within normal ranges on the first night. 32 Table 1 presents the PSG parameters of the second night as well as the scores of the cognitive variables. The percentage of the sample exhibiting CF difficulties falling below the first standard deviation was 20% for the word list memory test in the learning subtest and 16.6% in recall. Twenty percentage showed poor performance in the sustained and divided attention test (D2), while only 6.6% in the Letter–Number Sequencing Test, as well as 23.3% in the TMT-A and 16.6% in part B.

Table 1. PSG and Nneuropsychological Characteristics of the Sample

Abbreviations: REM, rapid eye movements; TST, total sleep time; WASO, wake after sleep onset.

a Raw scores of the neuropsychological tests.

b For the learning section of the memory test, the average of the three trials was used.

PSG factors associated with CF

Episodic memory

All the analyses between the PSG parameters and each of the cognitive variables are detailed in Table 2. In the learning subtest, it was observed that only TST showed a significant positive association with the number of words learned. Meanwhile, for the recall subtest, TST exhibited a positive association, while WASO showed a negative association with word recall. The dispersion of these associations is shown in Figure 2.

Table 2. Association between PSG Parameters and Cognitive Function

Note: All variables adjusted for age and schooling, *significant p values following the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure for controlling the false discovery rate.

Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test; TST, total sleep time; SE, sleep efficiency; SWS, slow-wave sleep; WASO, wake after sleep onset.

a Log-transformed variable, B = unstandardized coefficients, B = standardized coefficients.

Figure 2. Dispersion of scores between PSG parameters and episodic memory. Abbreviations: WASO, wake after sleep onset.

Sustained attention

In the sustained and divided attention test, a positive association was observed between better attention performance and an increase in TST and SE. Additionally, a negative relationship was found between WASO and attention (see Figure 3).

Figure 3. Dispersion of scores between PSG parameters and sustained attention. Abbreviations: WASO, wake after sleep onset.

Working memory

A positive association was found between SWS and higher performance in working memory (see Figure 4).

Figure 4. Dispersion of scores between PSG parameters and working memory.

TMT

No association was found between the PSG parameters and the scores obtained in both parts A and B of the TMT.

Association between severity of MDD and CI with CF

All the analyses between depression and insomnia severity and each of the CF variables are detailed in Table 3. No significant relationship was found between the severity of MDD or CI with CF (see Table 3). However, a trend was noted between CI severity and word recall, as well as working memory.

Table 3. Association Between Depression Severity, Insomnia, and Cognitive Function

Note: All variables adjusted for age and schooling, *significant p values following the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure for controlling the false discovery rate.

Abbreviations: CI, chronic insomnia; MDD, major depressive disorder; TMT, Trail Making Test.

a Adjusted for sleep variable.

b Log-transformed variable, B = unstandardized coefficients, B = standardized coefficients.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to utilize PSG data to examine the relationship between sleep and CF in patients with MDD comorbid with CI. Our findings suggest that TST, SE, and SWS are positively associated with CF, whereas WASO is negatively correlated. These results remained consistent after controlling for the age and schooling of the participants, as well as the absence of other sleep disorders or pharmacological treatments that could affect the results. Furthermore, we found no association between the severity of MDD or CI and CF in this sample of patients.

Specifically, TST is positively associated with word learning and recall in an episodic memory task, while WASO is negatively related to recall. These results are aligned with findings suggesting that TST is linked to memory task performance in patients with MDDReference Müller, Olschinski, Kundermann and Cabanel 26 and in individuals with CI who have objectively short sleep duration.Reference Ren, Jiang and Guo 30 Additionally, WASO has been reported to be negatively associated with episodic memory in both patient groups.Reference Mellor, Bucks, Maul, Sanders, McGowan and Waters 27 , Reference Wilckens, Kline and Bowman 28 , Reference Wilckens, Hall, Nebes, Monk and Buysse 29 This association could be explained by the active role of sleep in memory consolidation,Reference Born and Wilhelm 13 which involves the reactivation of top-down networks between the cortex and the hippocampus during sleep.Reference Born and Wilhelm 13 Reference Rasch and Born 14 In addition, it has been shown that sleep not only benefits memory consolidation but also enhances the encoding of new material.Reference Van Der Werf, Altena and Schoonheim 50 , Reference Yoo, Hu, Gujar, Jolesz and Walker 51 There is strong evidence that SWS plays a crucial role in memory consolidation in healthy subjects.Reference Born and Wilhelm 13 , Reference Rasch and Born 14 , Reference McCarter, Hagen and St Louis 15 , Reference Hokett, Arunmozhi, Campbell, Verhaeghen and Duarte 16 However, our results did not reveal a significant relationship between SWS and episodic memory recall; we only observed a trend in that direction. This could be explained by the observed decrease in SWS in both patients with MDD and CI.Reference Baglioni, Regen and Teghen 52 , Reference Baglioni, Nanovska and Regen 53 This trend is also present in our sample, potentially influencing the strength of this association.

Regarding attention, our findings indicate that TST is positively associated with better performance in a sustained and selective attention task, while WASO is negatively related to this function. This relationship has been extensively explored in sleep deprivation models,Reference Van Dongen, Maislin, Mullington and Dinges 17 , Reference Chua, Fang and Gooley 19 and it is well established that reduced TST impairs daytime attention levels. This is related to the accumulation of homeostatic sleep pressure, where decreased sleep leads to an increased need for sleep, resulting in a cumulative decrease in alertness.Reference Van Dongen, Maislin, Mullington and Dinges 17 A striking finding is that, despite not observing a significant reduction in TST in our sample, which averaged around 7 hours, an increased WASO is associated with worse attention performance. This observation suggests that increased WASO, commonly reported in CI,Reference Baglioni, Regen and Teghen 52 may itself be a risk factor related to the attention problems reported by these patients.Reference Ren, Jiang and Guo 30 32 Therefore, not only the amount of sleep but also the maintenance of sleep throughout the night is an important factor for optimal attention levels.

Another notable finding was the improvement in TST and SE on the second night of PSG recording, showing higher values than expected for this population. However, not all patients exhibited improvement, suggesting that there was no global sleep rebound. These changes are likely associated with the first-night sleep effect described in patients with insomnia.Reference Hu, Shi and Li 39 This effect may arise from maladaptive associations between the insomniacs’ sleep issues and their familiar sleep environment, which are disrupted by a new sleep setting in the laboratory where patients appear to fall asleep more readily.Reference McCall and McCall 54 Another plausible explanation is that the sample comprised newly followed ambulatory patients, possibly resulting in less objective sleep disturbance, as reported in MDD patients from the general population.Reference Solelhac, Berger and Strippoli 55

Finally, we observed a positive association between the duration of SWS and higher working memory. Previous studies have not explored this association in patients with MDD or CI. Nevertheless, our findings are consistent with studies in healthy subjects, which have shown that increased SWS predicts better performance on working memory tasks.Reference Ferrarelli, Kaskie, Laxminarayan, Ramakrishnan, Reifman and Germain 18 In addition, an increase in slow-wave activity has been observed during sleep following training on a working memory task.Reference Pugin, Metz, Wolf, Achermann, Jenni and Huber 56 This could be related to the regulation of the homeostatic need for sleep by SWS,Reference Achermann, Dijk, Brunner and Borbély 57 which is associated with cortical plasticity and improved cognitive task performance.Reference Kuhn, Wolf and Maier 58 , Reference Huber, Ghilardi, Massimini and Tononi 59 This suggests that sleep depth may have a stronger relationship with higher CFs.Reference McCarter, Hagen and St Louis 15 This finding is particularly relevant in our sample, as working memory impairment has been documented in patients with CI, especially with objective sleep assessments.Reference Ballesio, Aquino, Kyle, Ferlazzo and Lombardo 60 However, we failed to observe an association between SWS and other executive functions, such as those assessed in the TMT. Therefore, it is likely that more sensitive tests are needed to evaluate executive function in this group of patients and gain a better understanding of the influence of SWS on these functions.

Our results showed no significant association between the severity of MDD or CI and CF. We only observed a trend between CI severity and CF, which aligns with our initial hypothesis and with findings from other research.Reference Baril, Beiser and Sanchez 21 However, these findings are likely limited by our sample size. On the other hand, we did not expect to find a relationship between MDD and CF, mainly due to inconsistent findings in the literature.Reference McClintock, Husain, Greer and Cullum 22 Furthermore, evidence suggests that a significant portion of cognitive impairments persist beyond acute episodes of depression,Reference Rock, Roiser, Riedel and Blackwell 7 , Reference Reppermund, Ising, Lucae and Zihl 61 , Reference Bhalla, Butters and Mulsant 62 and deficits in selective attention, working memory, and long-term memory have been reported to persist even during remission from a major depressive episode.Reference Semkovska, Quinlivan and O’Grady 63 Therefore, it is important to consider that sleep may play a larger role in its association with the CF observed in this group of patients and to recognize the implications this has for their treatment. For instance, cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia has demonstrated the potential to enhance cognition in patients with MDD and CI.Reference Sadler, McLaren, Klein and Jenkins 64 , Reference Asarnow and Manber 65 This therapy has also shown effectiveness in improving various sleep parameters, including TST and WASO.Reference Jansson-Fröjmark and Norell-Clarke 66 Additionally, stimulating SWS in depressed patients could potentially improve CF.Reference Munz, Ahlich, Nietzschmann, Prehn-Kristensen and Göder 67 In the future, these techniques and others that are developed should focus on promoting a more natural sleep that is reflected in objective sleep parameters.

Limitations, implications, and future directions

There are several limitations that should be considered when interpreting and generalizing our findings. One limitation is the relatively small sample size and potential referral bias, as our participants were from a tertiary care setting. Additionally, we did not control for activities performed between PSG studies; participants were only advised to carry out their routines, which could have influenced their sleep on the second night. Smoking history was also not considered in our analysis. For future studies, it would be important to include a comparison group, comprising individuals with insomnia but without depressive symptoms or healthy controls matched for age and educational background. This would help to determine the association of each disorder with CF. Furthermore, including patients with a wider range of severity in both MDD and CI could provide a more comprehensive understanding of the potential moderating effect of disorder severity on the relationship between sleep and CF. Additionally, conducting neuropsychological tests both before and after a night of sleep could offer valuable insights into how prior sleep influences CF.

In conclusion, our findings highlight the importance of sleep and its relationship with CF in individuals presenting the MDD phenotype with CI. TST, SWS, and WASO are critical parameters linked to CF. Therefore, these parameters could serve as targets for therapeutic interventions aimed at addressing cognitive impairments in these patients. It is essential for mental health professionals to recognize the potential benefits of sleep-focused interventions within the context of mood disorders. Furthermore, future research should explore the connection between objective sleep parameters and various health outcomes, enabling the development of tailored treatments and even preventive strategies such as those currently recognized as sleep health.Reference Buysse 68

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (CONACyT) through postgraduate grant number 751918 CVU 697300.

Author contribution

Conceptualization: A.J., D.O., J.S.; Investigation: A.J., M.V., C.O.; Methodology: A.J., D.O., J.S., S.C., M.V., C.O.; Software: A.J., D.O.; Supervision: A.J., J.S., M.V.; Visualization: J.S.; Formal analysis: S.C., C.O.; Writing – review & editing: S.C., M.V.; Data curation: M.V., C.O.; Writing – original draft: C.O.

Competing interest

All authors report that there are no competing interests to declare.

References

WHO (World Health Organ). Depression. Fact Sheet. WHO, Geneva; 2021. http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/depression. Accessed November 10, 2023.Google Scholar
Tsuno, N, Besset, A, Ritchie, K. Sleep and depression. J Clin Psychiatry. 2005;66(10):12541269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staner, L. Comorbidity of insomnia and depression. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(1):3546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moretto, U, Palagini, L. Sleep in major depression. In Dringenberg, H, eds. Handbook of Behavioral Neuroscience. Elsevier; 2019:693706.Google Scholar
Sun, Q, Tan, L. Comparing primary insomnia to the insomnia occurring in major depression and general anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2019;282:112514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres-Granados, GI, Santana-Miranda, R, Barrera-Medina, A, et al. The economic costs of insomnia comorbid with depression and anxiety disorders: an observational study at a sleep clinic in Mexico. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2023;21(1):2331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rock, PL, Roiser, JP, Riedel, WJ, Blackwell, AD. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44(10):20292040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortier-Brochu, E, Beaulieu-Bonneau, S, Ivers, H, Morin, CM. Insomnia and daytime cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16(1):8394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntyre, RS, Cha, DS, Soczynska, JK, et al. Cognitive deficits and functional outcomes in major depressive disorder: determinants, substrates, and treatment interventions. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30(6):515527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cambridge, OR, Knight, MJ, Mills, N, Baune, BT. The clinical relationship between cognitive impairment and psychosocial functioning in major depressive disorder: a systematic review. Psychiatry Res. 2018;269:157171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, O, Uglik-Marucha, N, Miskowiak, KW, et al. The relationship between sleep disturbance and cognitive impairment in mood disorders: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2023;327:207216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biddle, DJ, Naismith, SL, Griffiths, KM, Christensen, H, Hickie, IB, Glozier, NS. Associations of objective and subjective sleep disturbance with cognitive function in older men with comorbid depression and insomnia. Sleep Health. 2017;3(3):178183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Born, J, Wilhelm, I. System consolidation of memory during sleep. Psychol Res. 2012;76(2):192203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasch, B, Born, J. About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93(2):681766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarter, SJ, Hagen, PT, St Louis, EK, et al. Physiological markers of sleep quality: a scoping review. Sleep Med Rev. 2022;64:101657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hokett, E, Arunmozhi, A, Campbell, J, Verhaeghen, P, Duarte, A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of individual differences in naturalistic sleep quality and episodic memory performance in young and older adults. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;127:675688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dongen, HP, Maislin, G, Mullington, JM, Dinges, DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26(2):117126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrarelli, F, Kaskie, R, Laxminarayan, S, Ramakrishnan, S, Reifman, J, Germain, A. An increase in sleep slow waves predicts better working memory performance in healthy individuals. Neuroimage. 2019;191:19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chua, EC, Fang, E, Gooley, JJ. Effects of total sleep deprivation on divided attention performance. PLoS One. 2017;12(11):e0187098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wichniak, A, Wierzbicka, A, Walęcka, M, Jernajczyk, W. Effects of antidepressants on sleep. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2017;19(9):63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baril, AA, Beiser, AS, Sanchez, E, et al. Insomnia symptom severity and cognitive performance: Moderating role of APOE genotype. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;18(3):408421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClintock, SM, Husain, MM, Greer, TL, Cullum, CM. Association between depression severity and neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder: a review and synthesis. Neuropsychology. 2010;24(1):934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabanel, N, Schmidt, AM, Fockenberg, S, et al. Evening preference and poor sleep independently affect attentional-executive functions in patients with depression. Psychiatry Res. 2019;281:112533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutter, C, Zöllig, J, Allemand, M, Martin, M. Sleep quality and cognitive function in healthy old age: the moderating role of subclinical depression. Neuropsychology. 2012;26(6):768775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cha, DS, Carmona, N, Cha, RH, et al. Perceived sleep quality predicts cognitive function in adults with major depressive disorder independent of depression severity. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2019;31(1):1726.Google ScholarPubMed
Müller, MJ, Olschinski, C, Kundermann, B, Cabanel, N. Sleep duration of inpatients with a depressive disorder: Associations with age, subjective sleep quality, and cognitive complaints. Arch Psychiatr Nurs. 2017;31(1):7782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, A, Bucks, RS, Maul, J, Sanders, KA, McGowan, H, Waters, F. Sleep and cognition in older adults: does depression matter? An actigraphy and polysomnography study. Arch Psychol. 2018;2(1).Google Scholar
Wilckens, KA, Kline, CE, Bowman, MA, et al. Does objectively-assessed sleep moderate the association between history of major depressive disorder and task-switching? J Affect Disord. 2020;265:216223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilckens, KA, Hall, MH, Nebes, RD, Monk, TH, Buysse, DJ. Changes in cognitive performance are associated with changes in sleep in older adults with insomnia. Behav Sleep Med. 2016;14(3):295310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ren, D, Jiang, B, Guo, Z. Insomnia disorder with objective short sleep duration (ISS) phenotype and cognitive performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurol Sci. 2023;44(7):23632368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.Google Scholar
American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The International Classification of Sleep Disorders. 3rd ed. Darien II, USA: ICSD-3; 2014.Google Scholar
Dueñas, H, Lara, C, Walton, RJ, Granger, RE, Dossenbach, M, Raskin, J. The integral inventory for depression, a new, self-rated clinimetric instrument for the emotional and painful dimensions in major depressive disorder. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2011;15(3):171179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rush, AJ, Trivedi, MH, Ibrahim, HM, et al. The 16-item quick inventory of depressive symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): a psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression [published correction appears in Biol Psychiatry. 2003 Sep 1;54(5):585]. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(5):573583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gili, M, Lopez-Navarro, E, Homar, C, et al. Psychometric properties of Spanish version of QIDS-SR16 in depressive patients. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2014;42(6):292299.Google ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Mendoza, J, Rodriguez-Muñoz, A, Vela-Bueno, A, et al. The Spanish version of the Insomnia Severity Index: a confirmatory factor analysis. Sleep Med. 2012;13(2):207210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morin, CM, Belleville, G, Bélanger, L, Ivers, H. The insomnia severity index: psychometric indicators to detect insomnia cases and evaluate treatment response. Sleep. 2011;34(5):601608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM). Manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events (2.6). AASM, USA; 2020.Google Scholar
Hu, S, Shi, L, Li, Z, et al. First-night effect in insomnia disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of polysomnographic findings. J Sleep Res. 2024;33(1):e13942. doi:10.1111/jsr.13942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostrosky-Solís, F, Gómez, ME, Ardila, A, Rosselli, M, Pineda, D, Matute, E. Neuropsi atención y memoria. Manual, Perfiles y Material. American Bookstore, México; 2003.Google Scholar
Seisdedos, N. D2, test de atención. Adaptación española. Tea Ediciones; 2002.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. WAIS-IV. Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para adultos-IV. Manual de aplicación y corrección-versión mexicana. Traducción al español por Editorial El Manual Moderno, S. A. de C. V. D. R. Translated by Ferrari U. Coordinación de estandarización Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México. Pearson Inc, México; 2014.Google Scholar
Reitan, RM, Wolfson, D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation, 2nd ed. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press; 1993.Google Scholar
Arango-Lasprilla, JC, Rivera, D, Aguayo, A, et al. Trail making test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015;37(4):639661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, et al. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(Suppl 20):2257.Google ScholarPubMed
Guerrero-Zúñiga, S, Gaona, EB, Cuevas, L, et al. Valoración del cuestionario de Berlín para el diagnóstico de apnea obstructiva de sueño en el Valle de México. Neumol Cir Torax. 2018;77(4):305312.Google Scholar
Strauss, E, Sherman, EMS, Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, 3rd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006.Google Scholar
Miles, J, Shelvin, M. Applying Regression & Correlation: A Guide for Student and Research. London: SAGE; 2006.Google Scholar
Benjamin, Y, Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Roy Stat Soc Ser B. 1995;57:289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Werf, YD, Altena, E, Schoonheim, MM, et al. Sleep benefits subsequent hippocampal functioning. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(2):122123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoo, SS, Hu, PT, Gujar, N, Jolesz, FA, Walker, MP. A deficit in the ability to form new human memories without sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(3):385392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baglioni, C, Regen, W, Teghen, A, et al. Sleep changes in the disorder of insomnia: a meta-analysis of polysomnographic studies. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;18(3):195213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baglioni, C, Nanovska, S, Regen, W, et al. Sleep and mental disorders: A meta-analysis of polysomnographic research. Psychol Bull. 2016;142(9):969990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, C, McCall, WV. Objective vs. subjective measurements of sleep in depressed insomniacs: first night effect or reverse first night effect?. J Clin Sleep Med. 2012;8(1):5965. Published 2012 Feb 15. doi:10.5664/jcsm.1664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solelhac, G, Berger, M, Strippoli, MF, et al. Objective polysomnography-based sleep features and major depressive disorder subtypes in the general population. Psychiatry Res. 2023;324:115213. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugin, F, Metz, AJ, Wolf, M, Achermann, P, Jenni, OG, Huber, R. Local increase of sleep slow wave activity after three weeks of working memory training in children and adolescents. Sleep. 2015;38(4):607614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achermann, P, Dijk, DJ, Brunner, DP, Borbély, AA. A model of human sleep homeostasis based on EEG slow-wave activity: quantitative comparison of data and simulations. Brain Res Bull. 1993;31(1–2):97113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, M, Wolf, E, Maier, JG, et al. Sleep recalibrates homeostatic and associative synaptic plasticity in the human cortex. Nat Commun. 2016;7:12455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, R, Ghilardi, MF, Massimini, M, Tononi, G. Local sleep and learning. Nature. 2004;430(6995):7881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballesio, A, Aquino, MRJV, Kyle, SD, Ferlazzo, F, Lombardo, C. Executive functions in insomnia disorder: A systematic review and exploratory meta-analysis. Front Psychol. 2019;10:101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reppermund, S, Ising, M, Lucae, S, Zihl, J. Cognitive impairment in unipolar depression is persistent and non-specific: further evidence for the final common pathway disorder hypothesis. Psychol Med. 2009;39(4):603614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhalla, RK, Butters, MA, Mulsant, BH, et al. Persistence of neuropsychologic deficits in the remitted state of late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14(5):419427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semkovska, M, Quinlivan, L, O’Grady, T, et al. Cognitive function following a major depressive episode: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(10):851861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sadler, P, McLaren, S, Klein, B, Jenkins, M. Advancing cognitive behaviour therapy for older adults with comorbid insomnia and depression. Cogn Behav Ther. 2018;47(2):139154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asarnow, LD, Manber, R. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia in depression. Sleep Med Clin. 2019;14(2):177184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansson-Fröjmark, M, Norell-Clarke, A. The cognitive treatment components and therapies of cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;42:1936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munz, M, Ahlich, S, Nietzschmann, A, Prehn-Kristensen, A, Göder, R. Improving recovery during sleep in depression: a pilot study with slow oscillating transcranial direct current stimulation. Psychiatry Res. 2021;301:113989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buysse, DJ. Sleep health: can we define it? Does it matter? Sleep. 2014;37(1):917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Patient flow. Abbreviations: MDD, major depressive disorder; CI, chronic insomnia; PSG, polysomnography.

Figure 1

Table 1. PSG and Nneuropsychological Characteristics of the Sample

Figure 2

Table 2. Association between PSG Parameters and Cognitive Function

Figure 3

Figure 2. Dispersion of scores between PSG parameters and episodic memory. Abbreviations: WASO, wake after sleep onset.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Dispersion of scores between PSG parameters and sustained attention. Abbreviations: WASO, wake after sleep onset.

Figure 5

Figure 4. Dispersion of scores between PSG parameters and working memory.

Figure 6

Table 3. Association Between Depression Severity, Insomnia, and Cognitive Function