Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T16:04:31.148Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 5 - Calibrating emotional orientations

Social appraisal and other kinds of relation alignment

from Part III - On the mechanics of affective social learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2019

Daniel Dukes
Affiliation:
Université de Fribourg, Switzerland
Fabrice Clément
Affiliation:
Université de Neuchâtel, Switzerland
Get access

Summary

Interpersonally presented emotions help to calibrate people’s orientations to things happening in the shared environment. For example, social referencing involves one person seeking clarification of the appropriate appraisal of an object, event, or person, and another person responding with an emotional orientation that disambiguates things. However, this paradigmatic case represents only one of the possible ways in which emotions affect other people’s physical or mental attitudes. In other cases, emotion-related responses affect other people’s orientations independent of their explicit informational content. Further, emotional knowledge may be co-constructed dynamically rather than transmitted unidirectionally from one person to another. In these cases, affective social learning need not involve changes in the perceived meaning of emotional objects, but rather adjustments in interactants’ orientations to what is happening. This chapter suggests ways of extending and going beyond existing methodological and theoretical approaches to emotional influence and identifies some of the blindspots of previous research.

Type
Chapter
Information
Foundations of Affective Social Learning
Conceptualizing the Social Transmission of Value
, pp. 117 - 141
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, R. B., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 311.Google Scholar
Bartel, C., & Saavedra, R. (2000). The collective construction of work group moods. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45, 197231.Google Scholar
Bayliss, A. P., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Affective evaluations of objects are influenced by observed gaze direction and emotion expression. Cognition, 104, 644653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruder, M., Dosmukhambetova, D., Nerb, J., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2012). Emotional signals in nonverbal interaction: Dyadic facilitation and convergence in expressions, appraisals, and feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 480502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buck, R., Savin, V. J., Miller, R. E., & Caul, W. F. (1972). Communication of affect through facial expressions in humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23, 362371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, E. A., & Randall, A. K. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emotion Review, 5, 202210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clément, F., & Dukes, D. (2017). Social appraisal and social referencing: Two components of affective social learning. Emotion Review, 9, 253261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Melo, C. M., Carnevale, P. J., Read, S. J., & Gratch, J. (2014). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 7388.Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A. (2007). Emotion in organizations: A review and theoretical integration. Academy of Management Annals, 1, 371457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (2005). Emotion experience. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 473497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H., & Philipszoon, E. (1963). Dimensions of recognition of emotion. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 4551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gendron, M., Crivelli, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Universality reconsidered: Diversity in meaning making about facial expressions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 211219.Google Scholar
Hareli, S. (2014). Making sense of the social world and influencing it by using a naïve attribution theory of emotions. Emotion Review, 6, 336343.Google Scholar
Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 142157.Google Scholar
Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2018). On the malleability of the meaning of contexts: The influence of another person’s emotion expressions on situation perception. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 185191.Google Scholar
Kalokerinos, E. K., Tamir, M., & Kuppens, P. (2017). Instrumental motives in negative emotion regulation in daily life: Frequency, consistency, and predictors. Emotion, 17, 648657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kokkinaki, T. S., Vasdekis, V. G. S., Koufaki, Z. E., & Trevarthen, C. B. (2017). Coordination of emotions in mother-infant dialogues. Infant and Child Development, 26, e1973.Google Scholar
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 215221.Google Scholar
Latané, B., & Wolf, S. (1981). The social impact of majorities and minorities. Psychological Review, 88, 438453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2003). Malicious pleasure: Schadenfreude at the suffering of an outgroup. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 932943.Google Scholar
Lee, D. H., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Social transmission of the sensory benefits of eye widening in fear expression. Psychological Science, 24, 957965.Google Scholar
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., … Brown, R. A. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 7584.Google Scholar
Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1987). The relationship of emotion and cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 221232). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, J. E., & Kelly, J. R. (1986). Time and human interaction: Toward a social psychology of time. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mumenthaler, C., & Sander, D. (2012). Social appraisal influences recognition of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 11181135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumenthaler, C., (2015). Automatic integration of social information in emotion recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 392399.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1996). Emotions are social. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 663683.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion transfer: Contagion and social appraisal. Personality and Social Psychology Compass, 5, 428439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2013). Contextualizing facial activity. Emotion Review, 5, 97103.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B. (2017). Comment: Respecifying emotional influence. Emotion Review, 9, 263265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1993). Making sense of emotion in stories and social life. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 295323.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Phiri, N., & Simons, G. (2012). Bursting with anxiety: Adult social referencing in an interpersonal Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Emotion, 12, 817826.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., Shore, D., & Stephens, N. (unpublished). Expression regulation and perceived regulation and their interpersonal effects on risk behaviour in a dyadic Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART).Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2009). Affecting others: Social appraisal and emotion contagion in everyday decision-making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 10711084.Google Scholar
Parkinson, B., (2012). Worry spreads: Interpersonal transfer of problem-related anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 462479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3, 186192.Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2007). Interaction effects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 470480.Google Scholar
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Mortillaro, M., Rotondi, I., Sergi, I., & Trznadel, S. (2018). Appraisal-driven facial actions as building blocks for emotion inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 358379.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Scott, H. S. (1997). A componential approach to the meaning of facial expressions. In Russell, J. A. & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 229254). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195200.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., Hofer, L., Haft, W., & Dore, J. (1985). Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of intermodal fluency. In Field, T. N. & Fox, N. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 249268). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P. (2000). Catching moods and hitting runs: Mood linkage and subjective performance in professional sport teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 848859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Totterdell, P., Kellett, S., Teuchmann, K., & Briner, R. B. (1998). Evidence of mood linkage in work groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 15041515.Google Scholar
van Doorn, E. A., van Kleef, G. A., & van der Pligt, J. (2015). Deriving meaning from others’ emotions: Attribution, appraisal, and the use of emotions as social information. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1077.Google Scholar
van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life: The Emotions as Social Information (EASI) model. Current Directions in Psychological Research, 18, 184188.Google Scholar
Walle, E. A., Reschke, P. J., Camras, L. A., & Campos, J. J. (2017). Infant differential behaviour responding to discrete emotions. Emotion, 17, 10781091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1979). The cognition-emotion process in achievement-related contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 12111220.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×