Skip to main content
×
×
Home
  • This chapter is unavailable for purchase
  • Cited by 112
  • Cited by
    This chapter has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Hiltunen, Sinikka Mäntyranta, Heli and Määttänen, Ilmari 2018. Cooperativeness – A necessary trait for interpreters? A study on temperament and character dimensions of experts in different fields. International Journal of Bilingualism, p. 136700691879080.

    2018. Retraction: ‘The relationship of general mental ability to entrepreneurial firm performance: A meta-analysis’ by David S. DeGeest, Scott Seibert and Ernest O'Boyle. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 91, Issue. 2, p. 440.

    Muszkat-Barkan, Michal and Rosenstein, Marc J. 2018. Professional identity formation in a liberal Israeli rabbinical seminary: Spiritual transformers in the learned curriculum. Religious Education, p. 1.

    Quast, Christian 2018. Expertise: A Practical Explication. Topoi, Vol. 37, Issue. 1, p. 11.

    Rigolizzo, Michele 2018. The labs (learning as behaviors) framework for higher-order learning. The Learning Organization, Vol. 25, Issue. 4, p. 224.

    Chaturvedi, Amit 2018. Against a “mindless” account of perceptual expertise. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences,

    Marsh, Jessecae K. De Los Reyes, Andres and Lilienfeld, Scott O. 2018. Leveraging the Multiple Lenses of Psychological Science to Inform Clinical Decision Making: Introduction to the Special Section. Clinical Psychological Science, Vol. 6, Issue. 2, p. 167.

    Scholz, Oliver R. 2018. Symptoms of Expertise: Knowledge, Understanding and Other Cognitive Goods. Topoi, Vol. 37, Issue. 1, p. 29.

    Lelebina, Olga and Gand, Sébastien 2018. Beyond “Hero-based” Management: Revisiting HRM Practices for Managing Collective Expertise. Relations industrielles, Vol. 73, Issue. 1, p. 39.

    Ghanem, Christian Kollar, Ingo Fischer, Frank Lawson, Thomas R. and Pankofer, Sabine 2018. How do social work novices and experts solve professional problems? A micro-analysis of epistemic activities and the use of evidence. European Journal of Social Work, Vol. 21, Issue. 1, p. 3.

    Abele, Stephan 2018. Diagnostic Problem-Solving Process in Professional Contexts: Theory and Empirical Investigation in the Context of Car Mechatronics Using Computer-Generated Log-Files. Vocations and Learning, Vol. 11, Issue. 1, p. 133.

    Welch, Teresa D. and Carter, Melondie 2018. Deliberate Practice and Skill Acquisition in Nursing Practice. The Journal of Continuing Education in Nursing, Vol. 49, Issue. 6, p. 269.

    Miller, Scott D. Hubble, Mark A. and Chow, Daryl 2017. The Cycle of Excellence. p. 23.

    Dukas, Reuven 2017. Cognitive innovations and the evolutionary biology of expertise. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, Vol. 372, Issue. 1735, p. 20160427.

    Castañer, Marta Barreira, Daniel Camerino, Oleguer Anguera, M. Teresa Fernandes, Tiago and Hileno, Raúl 2017. Mastery in Goal Scoring, T-Pattern Detection, and Polar Coordinate Analysis of Motor Skills Used by Lionel Messi and Cristiano Ronaldo. Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 8, Issue. ,

    Riel, Margaret M. and Rowell, Lonnie L. 2017. The Palgrave International Handbook of Action Research. p. 667.

    KEESTRA, MACHIEL 2017. Drawing on a Sculpted Space of Actions: Educating for Expertise while Avoiding a Cognitive Monster. Journal of Philosophy of Education, Vol. 51, Issue. 3, p. 620.

    von der Lippe, Anna Louise Oddli, Hanne Weie and Halvorsen, Margrethe Seeger 2017. Therapist strategies early in therapy associated with good or poor outcomes among clients with low proactive agency. Psychotherapy Research, p. 1.

    Grunefeld, Hetty Prins, Frans van Tartwijk, Jan van der Vaart, Rob Loads, Daphne Turner, Jon Mårtensson, Katarina Gibbons, Anne Marthe Nilsen Harboe, Thomas Poder, Karen and Wubbels, Theo 2017. Strengthening Teaching and Learning in Research Universities. p. 73.

    Espin, Christine A. Wayman, Miya Miura Deno, Stanley L. McMaster, Kristen L. and de Rooij, Mark 2017. Data-Based Decision-Making: Developing a Method for Capturing Teachers’ Understanding of CBM Graphs. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, Vol. 32, Issue. 1, p. 8.

    ×
  • Print publication year: 2006
  • Online publication date: June 2012

4 - Studies of Expertise from Psychological Perspectives

from PART II - OVERVIEW OF APPROACHES TO THE STUDY OF EXPERTISE – BRIEF HISTORICAL ACCOUNTS OF THEORIES AND METHODS
Summary

Introduction

The study of expertise has a very long history that has been discussed in several other chapters in this handbook (Ericsson, Chapter 1; Amirault & Branson, Chapter 5). This chapter focuses on the influential developments within cognitive science and cognitive psychology that have occurred over the last three decades. Our chapter consists of two parts. In the first part we briefly review what we consider the major developments in cognitive science and cognitive psychology that led to the new field of expertise studies. In the second part we attempt to characterize some of the emerging insights about mechanisms and aspects of expertise that generalize across domains, and we explore the original theoretical accounts, along with more recent ones.

The Development of Expertise Studies

In this handbook there are several pioneering research traditions represented that were brought together to allow laboratory studies of expertise, along with the development of formal models that can reproduce the performance of the experts. One early stream was the study of thinking using protocol analysis, where participants were instructed to “think aloud” while solving everyday life problems (Duncker, 1945), and experts were asked to think aloud while selecting moves for chess positions (de Groot, 1946/1965; Ericsson, Chapter 13). Another stream developed out of the research on judgment and decision making, where researchers compared the judgments of experts to those of statistical models (Meehl, 1954; Yates & Tschirhart, Chapter 24).

Recommend this book

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this book to your organisation's collection.

The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance
  • Online ISBN: 9780511816796
  • Book DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511816796
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to *
×
References
Adelson, B. (1981). Problem solving and the development of abstract categories in programming languages. Memory and Cognition, 9, 422–433.
Anderson, J. R. (Ed.) (1981). Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.
Anderson, J. R. (1987). Skill acquisition: Compilation of weak-method problem situations. Psychological Review, 94, 192–210.
Austin, J. (2000). Performance analysis and performance diagnostics. In Austin, J. & Carr, J. E. (Eds.), Handbook of applied behavior analysis (pp. 321–349). Reno, NV: Context Press.
Baddeley, A. (2000). Short-term and working memory. In Tulving, E. & Craik, F. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 77–92). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psychologist, 7(2), 85–97.
Barrows, H. S., Feightner, J. W., Neufeld, V. R., & Norman, G. R. (1978). Analysis of the clinical methods of medical students and physicians. Final Report, Ontario Department of Health Grants ODH-PR-273 & ODH-DM-226. Hamilton, Ontario: McMaster University.
Barrows, H. S., & Tamblyn, R. M. (1980). Problem-based learning: An approach to medical education. New York: Springer.
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 84–91.
Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Bartlett, F. (1958). Thinking. New York: Basic Books.
Berlyne, D. E. (1965). Structure and direction in thinking. Oxford, UK: Wiley.
Bloom, B. (Ed.) (1985). Developing talent in young people. New York: Ballentine.
Brown, A. (1991). A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 204–223.
Brown, R. W., & MacNeill, D. (1966). The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 325–337.
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Buchanan, B. G., & Feigenbaum, E. A. (1978). DENDRAL and MetaDENDRAL: Their applications dimension. Artificial Intelligence, 11(1), 5–24.
Charness, N. (1976). Memory for chess positions: Resistance to interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 641–653.
Charness, N. (1979). Components of skill in bridge. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 1–50.
Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 467–476.
Chase, W. G. (Ed.) (1973). Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973a). The mind's eye in chess. In Chase, W. G. (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973b). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 33–81.
Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development. In Siegler, R. S. (Ed), Children's thinking: What develops? (pp. 73–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.) (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chiesi, H. L., Spilich, G. J., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Acquisition of domain-related information in relation to high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 257–274.
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.
Clancey, W. J. & Letsinger, R. (1984). NEOMYCIN: Reconfigure a rule-based expert system for application to teaching. In Clancey, W. J. & Shortliffe, E. H., (eds.), Readings in medical artificial intelligence (pp. 361–381). Readings Addision.
Clancey, W. J., & Shortliffe, E. H. (1984). Readings in medical artificial intelligence: The first decade. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–185.
Cowan, N., Chen, Z., & Rouder, J. (2004). Constant capacity in an immediate serial-recall task: A logical sequel to Miller (1956). Psychological Science, 15(9), 634–640.
Groot, A. (1946). Het denken van den schaker. Amsterdam: Noord-Hllandsche Uit. Mij.
Groot, A. (1965). Thought and choice in chess, 1st Edition. The Hague: Mouton.
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58 (Whole No. 270), 1–113.
Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1964). Memory: A contribution to experimental cognitive psychology (Henry A. Ruger & Clara E. Bussenius, Trans.). New York: Dover.
Egan, D. E., & Schwartz, B. J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings. Memory and Cognition, 7, 149–158.
Eisenstadt, M., & Kareev, Y. (1979). Aspects of human problem solving: The use of internal representations. In Norman, D. A. & Rumelhart, D. E. (Eds.), Exploration in cognition. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Elstein, A. S., Shulman, L. S., & Sprafka, S. A. (1978). Medical problem solving. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Engle, R. W., & Bukstel, L. (1978). Memory processes among bridge players of differing expertise. American Journal of Psychology, 91, 673–89.
Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.) (1996a). The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ericsson, K. A. (1996b). The acquisition of expert performance: An introduction to some of the issues. In Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 1–50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ericsson, K. A. (2003). The acquisition of expert performance as problem solving: Construction and modification of mediating mechanisms through deliberate practice. In Davidson, J. E. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), Problem solving (pp. 31–83). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (2000). Shortcomings of generic retrieval structures with slots of the type that Gobet (1993) proposed and modeled. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 571–588.
Ericsson, K. A., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The search for fixed generalizable limits of “pure STM” capacity: Problems with theoretical proposals based on independent chunks. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 120–121.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.
Ericsson, K. A., Patel, V. L., & Kintsch, W. (2000). How experts' adaptations to representative task demands account for the expertise effect in memory recall: Comment on Vicente and Wang (1998). Psychological Review, 107, 578–592.
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (Eds.) (1991a). Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991b). Prospects and limits in the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In Ericsson, K. A. and Smith, J. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 1–38). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Ernst, G., & Newell, A. (1969). GPS: A case-study in generality and problem solving. New York: Academic Press.
Feltovich, P., Ford, K. M., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.) (1997). Expertise in context: Human and Machine. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press.
Feltovich, P. J., Johnson, P. E, Moller, J., & Swanson, D. (1984). The role and development of medical knowledge in diagnostic reasoning. In. Clancey, W. & Shortliffe, E. (Eds.), Readings in medical artificial intelligence: The first decade (pp. 275–319). Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.
Feltovich, P., Spiro, R., & Coulson, R. (1997). Issues of expert flexibility in contexts characterized by complexity and change. In Feltovich, P., Ford, K., & Hoffman, R. (Eds.), Expertise in context: Human and machine. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press.
Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brookes Cole.
Flavell, J. (1979). Metacognition and cognition monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.
Forbus, K. D. & Feltovich, P. J. (2001), Smart machines in education. Menlo PK, CA: AAAI/MIT Press.
Gentner, D. (1988). Metaphor as structure-mapping: The relational shift. Child Development, 59, 47–59.
Glaser, R. (1976). Cognition and instructional design. In Klahr, D. (Ed.), Cognition and instruction (pp. 303–315). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Glaser, R., & Chi, M. T. H. (1988). Overview. In Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. ⅹⅴ-ⅹⅹⅷ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Templates in chess memory: A mechanism for recalling several boards. Cognitive Psychology, 31, 1–40.
Green, C. (1969). The application of theorem proving to question-answering systems. Doctoral dissertation Dept. of Electrical Engineering, Stanford University. Also Standford Artificial Intelligence Project Memo AI-96, June 1969.
Groen, G. J., & Patel, V. L. (1988). The relationship between comprehension and reasoning in medical expertise. In Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. 287–310). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hambrick, D., & Engle, R. (2002). Effects of domain knowledge, working memory capacity, and age on cognitive performance: An investigation of the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339–387.
Hatano, G., Miyake, Y., & Binks, M. B. (1977). Performance of expert abacus operators. Cognition, 5, 57–71.
Hinsley, D., Hayes, J., & Simon, H. A.(1978). From words to equations: Meaning and representation in algebra word problems. In Just, M. & Carpenter, P. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hoffman, R. R. (Ed.) (1992). The psychology of expertise. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Holyoak, K. J. (1991). Symbolic connectionism: toward third generation theories of expertise. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 301–335). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Jakobovits, L. A. & Miron, M. S. (1967). Readings in the Psychology of language. Englewood, cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Jansma, J., Ramsey, N., Slagter, H., & Kahn, R. (2001). Functional anatomical correlates of controlled and automatic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(6), 730–743.
Jeffries, R., Turner, A. A., Polson, P. G, & Atwood, M. E. (1981). The processes involved in software design. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Johnson, P. E., Duran, A. S., Hassebrock, F., Moller, J., Prietula, M. J., Feltovich, P. J., & Swanson, D. B. (1981). Expertise and error in diagnostic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 5, 235–283.
Klein, G. A. (1998). Sources of power: How people make decisions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Klein, G. A., & Hoffman, R. R. (1993). Seeing the invisible: Perceptual-cognitive aspects of expertise. In Rabinowitz, M. (Ed.), Cognitive science foundations of instruction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Koschmann, T. D., LeBaron, C., Goodwin, C., & Feltovich, P. J. (2001). Dissecting common ground: Examining an instance of reference repair. In Proceedings of the 23rd Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1121–1134.
Kuiper, R., & Pesut, D. (2004). Promoting cognitive and metacognitive reflective reasoning skills in nursing practice: Self-regularities learning theory. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 45, 381–391.
Larkin, J., McDermott, J., Simon, D. & Simon, H. A. (1980). Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science, 208, 1335–1342.
Lamme, V. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18.
Lesgold, A. M., & Resnick, L. (1982). How reading difficulties devlop: Perspectives from a longitudinal study. In Das, J., Mulcahey, R., & wall, A. (Eds.), Theory and research in learning disabilities (pp. 155–187). New York: Plenum Press.
Lesgold, A. M., Rubinson, H., Feltovich, P. J., Glaser, R., Klopfer, D., & Wang, Y. (1988). Expertise in a complex skill: Diagnosing x-ray pictures. In Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.), The nature of expertise (pp. 311–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Logan, G. (1985). Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications and future directions. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.
Lumsdaine, A. A., & Glaser, R. (1960). Teaching machines and programmed learning: A source book. Washington, DC: National Education Assoc.
McKeithen, K. B., Reitman, J. S., Reuter, H. H., & Hirtle, S. C., (1981). Knowledge organization and skill differences in computer programmers. Cognitive Psychology, 13, 307–325.
Meehl, P. E. (1954). Clinical versus statistical prediction: A theoretical analysis and a review of the evidence. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. Psychological Review, 104, 749–791.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Newell, A. (1973). Artificial intelligence and the concept of mind. In Schank, R. C. & Colby, K. M. (Eds.), Computer models of language and thought (pp. 1–60). San Franciso: W. H. Freeman.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1956). The logic theory machine: A complex information processing system. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, Vol IT-2, No. 3, 61–79.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1976). Computer science as empirical inquiry: Symbols and search. Communications of the ACM, 19(3), 113–126.
Norman, D. A., Rumelhart, D. E., & the LNR group (1979). Explorations in cognition. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Osgood, C. E. (1963). On understanding and creating sentences. American Psychologist, 18, 735–751.
Paris, S., & Winograd, P. (1990). How metacognition can promote academic learning and instruction. In Jones, B. & Idol, L. (Eds.), Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction (pp. 15–51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Patel, V. L., Arocha, J. F., & Kaufman, D. R. (1994). Diagnostic reasoning and medical expertise. In Medin, D. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 30 (pp. 187–251). New York: Academic Press.
Patel, V. L., & Groen, G. J. (1991). The general and specific nature of medical expertise: A critical look. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise (pp. 93–125). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Patil, R. S., Szolovitz, P., & Schwartz, W. B. (1981). Causal understanding of patient illness in medical diagnosis. In Proceedings of the seventh international conference on artificial intelligence. Vol. 2 (pp. 893–899). Los Altos, CA: William Kaufman.
Pauker, S. G., Gorry, G. A., Kassirer, J. P., & Schwartz, W. B. (1976). Towards simulation of clinical cognition: Taking a present illness by computer. American Journal of Medicine, 60, 981–996.
Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1982a). Improving the skills of learning. In Detterman, D. K. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), How much and how can intelligence be increased. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1982b). Analyzing aptitudes for learning: Inductive reasoning. In Glaser, R. (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology. Vol. 2 (pp. 269–345). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Perfetti, C. A., & Lesgold, A. M. (1979). Coding and comprehension in skilled reading. In Resnick, L. B. & Weaver, P. (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Phelps, R. H., & Shanteau, J. (1978). Livestock judges: How much information can an expert use? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 21, 209–219.
Petrusa, E. R. (2002). Clinical performance assessments. In Norman, G. R., Vleuten, C. P. M., & Neuble, D. I. (Eds.), International handbook of research in medical education. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Posner, M., & Snyder, C. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In Solso, R. L. (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Prietula, M., & Augier, M. (2005). Adventures in software archeology: Seeking (ABTOF) theory in the code. Proceedings of the 2005 Annual Academy of Management Meeting, August 5–10, Honolulu HI.
Prietula, M., & Simon, H. (1989). The experts in your midst. Harvard Business Review, Jan–Feb, 120–124.
Quillian, R. (1969). The teachable language comprehender: A Simulation program and theory of language. Communications of the ACM, 12 459–476.
Reder, L., & Shunn, C. (1996). Metacognition does not imply awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and memory. In Reder, L. (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Reitman, J. (1976). Skilled perception in GO: Deducing memory structure from inter-response times. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 336–356.
Reitman, W. R. (1965). Cognition and thought. New York: Wiley.
Richman, H. B., Gobet, F., Staszewski, J. J., & Simon, H. A. (1996). Perceptual and memory processes in the acquisition of expert performance: The EPAM model. In Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 167–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rieger, M. (2004). Automatic keypress activation in skilled typists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 555–565.
Rumelhart, D. E. (1979). Analogical processes and procedunal representations. CHIP Report # 81. Center for Human Information Processing, Univ. California, San Diego: February, 1979.
Sacerdoti, E. D. (1977). A structure for plans and behavior. New York: Elsevier-North Holland Publishing.
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Expertise as the circumvention of human processing limitations. In Ericsson, K. A. and Smith, J. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 286–300). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Samuel, A. L. (1959). Some studies in machine learning using the game of checkers. IBM Journal of Research and Development, 3, 210–229.
Schneider, W., & Fisk, A. (1982). Concurrent automatic and controlled visual search: Can processing occur without resource cost? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 8, 261–278.
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: 1. Detection, search and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.
Schmidt, J. A., McLaughlin, J. P., & Leighton, P. (1989). Novice strategies for understanding paintings. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 3, 65–72.
Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: Uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101–108.
Shaffer, L. H. (1975). Multiple attention in continuous verbal tasks. In Rabbitt, P. M. A. & Dornic, S. (Eds.) Attention and performance V. New York: Academic Press.
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–189.
Shipp, S. (2004). The brain circuitry of attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 223–230.
Shortliffe, E. H. (1976). Computer-based medical consultations: MYCIN. New York: American Elsevier.
Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess. American Scientist, 61, 394–403.
Simon, H. A., & Newell, A. (1958). Heuristic problem solving: The next advance in operations research. Operations Research, 6, 1–10.
Skinner, B. F. (1960). The science of learning and the art of teaching. In Lumsdaine, A. A. & Glaser, R. (Eds.), Teaching machines and programmed learning: A source book (pp. 99–113). Washington, DC: National Education Assoc.
Sleeman, D., & Brown, J. S. (1982). Intelligent tutoring systems. New York: Academic Press.
Spelke, E., Hirst, W., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 215–30.
Spilich, G. J., Vesonder, G. T., Chiesi, H. L., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 506–522.
Spiro, R. J., Collins, B. P. Thota, J. J., & Feltovich, P. J. (2003). Cognitive flexibility theory: Hypermedia for complex learning, adaptive knowledge application, and experience acceleration. Educational technology, 44, 5–10.
Starkes, J. L., & Allard, F. (Eds.) (1993). Cognitive issues in motor expertise. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Starkes, J., & Ericsson, K. A. (Eds.) (2003). Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Steier, D., & Mitchell, T. (Eds) (1996). Knowledge matters: A tribute to Allen Newell. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sternberg, R. (1984). Toward a triarchic theory of human intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 269–287.
Tulving, E. (1994). Forward. In Metcalfe, J. & Shimamura, A. (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Van Gelder, T., & Port, R. (1995). It's about time: An overview of the dynamical approach to cognition. In Gelder, T. & Port, R. (Eds.), Mind as motion (pp 1–43). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
VanLehn, K., & Brown, J. S. (1979). Planning nets: A representation for formalizing analogies and semantic models of procedural skills. In Snow, R. E. & Montague, W. E. (Eds.), Aptitude, learning and instruction: Cognitive process analyses. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Voss, J. F., Greene, T. R., Post, T. A., & Penner, B. C. (1983). Problem solving skill in the social sciences. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research theory. Vol. 17 (pp. 165–213). New York: Academic Press.
Voss, J. F., Tyler, S., & Yengo, L. (1983). Individual differences in the solving of social science problems. In Dillon, R. & Schmeck, R. (Eds.), Individual differences in cognition (pp. 205–232). New York: Academic Press.
Wason, P. M., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: Structure and content. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–77.
Watson, J. B. (1920). Is thinking merely the action of language mechanisms? British Journal of Psychology, 11, 87–104.
Winograd, T. (1975). Frame representations and the declarative procedural controversy. In Bobrow, D. G. & Collins, A. M. (Eds.), Representation and understanding. New York: Academic Press.
Zeitz, C. M. (1997). Some concrete advantages of abstraction: How experts' representations facilitate reasoning. In Feltovich, P. J., Ford, K. M., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.), Expertise in context: Human and machine (pp. 43–65). Menlo PK, CA: AAAI/MIT Press.