Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T08:08:08.562Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

30 - Polymathy: The Resurrection of Renaissance Man and the Renaissance Brain

from Part VIII - Artistic and Aesthetic Processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2018

Rex E. Jung
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
Oshin Vartanian
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amabile, T. (1982). Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 9971013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.5.997CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 357376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.2.357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Amabile, T., & Pillemer, J. (2012). Perspectives on the social psychology of creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 46, 315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jocb.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. (2011). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18, 251270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073858411403316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arden, R., Chavez, R., Grazioplene, R., & Jung, R. (2010). Neuroimaging creativity: A psychometric view. Behavioural Brain Research, 214, 143156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, H., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.113.3.628CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bashwiner, D., Wertz, C., Flores, R., & Jung, R. (2016). Musical creativity “revealed” in brain structure: Interplay between motor, default mode, and limbic networks. Scientific Reports, 6, 20482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep20482CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. (1998). The Self. In Fiske, S. & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 680740). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower. New York, NY: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Beaty, R., Benedek, M., Barry Kaufman, S., & Silvia, P. (2015). Default and executive network coupling supports creative idea production. Scientific Reports, 5, 10964. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep10964CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R., Benedek, M., Wilkins, R., Jauk, E., Fink, A., & Silvia, P. J., … Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Creativity and the default network: A functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia, 64, 9298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.09.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaty, R., & Silvia, P. (2012a). Metaphorically speaking: Cognitive abilities and the production of figurative language. Memory and Cognition, 41, 255267. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0258-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R., & Silvia, P. (2012b). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 309319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beaty, R., Silvia, P., Nusbaum, E., Jauk, E., & Benedek, M. (2014). The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Memory and Cognition, 42, 11861197. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0428-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binder, J., Desai, R., Graves, W., & Conant, L. (2009). Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 27672796. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronowski, J. (1965). The identity of man. Garden City, NY: Published for the American Museum of Natural History by the Natural History Press.Google Scholar
Brown, P. C., Roediger, H. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Make it stick. London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Buckner, R., & Carroll, D. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 4957. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunko, A. (2011). Hugh Laurie. London: John Blake.Google Scholar
Carr, E. (2016). The last days Of the polymath. 1843 Magazine, The Economist. Retrieved from www.1843magazine.com/content/edward-carr/last-days-polymathGoogle Scholar
Chein, J., & Schneider, W. (2005). Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 607623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiappe, D., & MacDonald, K. (2005). The evolution of domain-general mechanisms in intelligence and learning. The Journal of General Psychology, 132, 540. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/genp.132.1.5-40CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connolly, A., Guntupalli, J., Gors, J., Hanke, M., Halchenko, Y., Wu, Y., … Haxby, J. V. (2012). The representation of biological classes in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 26082618. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5547-11.2012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti, R., Coon, H., & Amabile, T. (1996). Evidence to support the componential model of creativity: Secondary analyses of three studies. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 385389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj0904_9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowey, A. (1979). Cortical maps and visual perception. The Grindley Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640747908400703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckworth, A., Peterson, C., Matthews, M., & Kelly, D. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 10871101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dweck, C. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 10401048. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.41.10.1040CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C. (1999). Self-theories. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. (2006). Mindset. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Fink, A., Graif, B., & Neubauer, A. (2009). Brain correlates underlying creative thinking: EEG alpha activity in professional vs. novice dancers. NeuroImage, 46, 854862. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, P., Kubitzki, J., Guter, S., & Frey, D. (2007). Virtual driving and risk taking: Do racing games increase risk-taking cognitions, affect, and behaviors? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 2231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-898x.13.1.22Google ScholarPubMed
Fransson, P., & Marrelec, G. (2008). The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. NeuroImage, 42, 11781184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.059CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glass, B., Maddox, W., & Love, B. (2013). Real-time strategy game training: Emergence of a cognitive flexibility trait. PLoS ONE, 8, e70350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0070350CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J., Chabris, C., & Braver, T. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316322. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grayling, A. (2016). The age of genius: The seventeenth century and the birth of the modern mind. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Green, C., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Action-video-game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psychological Science, 18, 8894. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01853.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, S. (2015). Mind change: How digital technologies are leaving their mark on our brains. London: Random House.Google Scholar
Greicius, M., Supekar, K., Menon, V., & Dougherty, R. (2008). Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 7278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heilman, K., & Acosta, L. (2013). Visual artistic creativity and the brain. Progress in Brain Research, 204, 1943. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-63287-6.00002-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, J. (1967). Imagination in science. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Holloway, I., & Ansari, D. (2008). Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children’s development of number comparison. Developmental Science, 11, 644649. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00712.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkham, N., Slemmer, J., & Johnson, S. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83, B35B42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00004-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Kounios, J. & Beeman, M. (2009). The aha! moment: The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 210216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01638.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, W., Reeve, J., Xue, Y., & Xiong, J. (2012). Neural differences between intrinsic reasons for doing versus extrinsic reasons for doing: An fMRI study. Neuroscience Research, 73, 6872. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2012.02.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Limb, C., & Braun, A. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS ONE, 3, e1679. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001679CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Long Lingo, E., & Tepper, S. (2013a). Looking back, looking forward: Arts-based careers and creative work. Work and Occupations, 40, 337363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0730888413505229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long Lingo, E., & Tepper, S. (2013). Patterns and pathways: Artists and creative work in a changing economy. Work and Occupations, 40, 337363.Google Scholar
Mahy, C., Moses, L., & Pfeifer, J. (2014). How and where: Theory-of-mind in the brain. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 6881. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martindale, C. (2001). Oscillations and analogies. American Psychologist, 56, 342345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moscovitch, M. (1995). Recovered consciousness: A hypothesis concerning modularity and episodic memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 276290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01688639508405123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oudeyer, P., & Kaplan, F. (2007). Language evolution as a Darwinian process: Computational studies. Cognitive Processes, 8, 2135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-006-0158-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oxman, N. (2016). Age of entanglement. Journal of Design and Science. Retrieved from http://jods.mitpress.mit.edu/pub/AgeOfEntanglementCrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Callaghan, C., Shine, J., Lewis, S., Andrews-Hanna, J., & Irish, M. (2015). Shaped by our thoughts – A new task to assess spontaneous cognition and its associated neural correlates in the default network. Brain and Cognition, 93, 110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterson, D., & Chiasson, D. (2012). Poetry and politics: Reading and conversation with Don Paterson and Dan Chiasson. Presentation, Boston University.Google Scholar
Plank, M. (1949). Scientific autobiography and other papers. New York, NY: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Ramón y Cajal, S. (1989). Recollections of my life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reeve, J., & Lee, W. (2014). Students’ classroom engagement produces longitudinal changes in classroom motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 527540. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, A. (2006). The last man who knew everything. New York, NY: Pi Press.Google Scholar
Robinson, K. (2006). Do schools kill creativity? Lecture, TED Talks.Google Scholar
Root-Bernstein, R. (1984). Creative process as a unifying theme of human cultures. Daedalus, 113, 197219.Google Scholar
Root-Bernstein, R. (1989). Discovering. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Root-Bernstein, R. (2003). The art of innovation: Polymaths and universality of the creative process. In Shavinina, L. V. (Ed.), The international handbook on innovation (pp. 267278). Oxford: Oxford Univetsity Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-008044198-6/50018-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, T. (2016). The end of average. New York, NY: Harper Audio.Google Scholar
Saffran, J., & Thiessen, E. (2008). Domain-general learning capacities. In Hoff, E. & Shatz, M. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (1st ed., pp. 6886). Chichester: Wiley Publishing.Google Scholar
Schacter, D., Addis, D., Hassabis, D., Martin, V., Spreng, R., & Szpunar, K. (2012). The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76, 677694. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarzenegger, A., Petre, P., & Dürr, K. (2012). Total recall. Hamburg: Hoffmann und Campe.Google Scholar
Shamma, S. (2001). On the role of space and time in auditory processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 340348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01704-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L. (1999). Children’s noun learning: How general learning processes make specialized learning mechanisms. In MacWhinney, B. (Ed.), The emergence of language (1st ed., pp. 277305). Chichester: Wiley Publishing.Google Scholar
Story Musgrave Biography – Academy of Achievement. (2016). Achievement.org. Retrieved April 25, 2016, from www.achievement.org/autodoc/page/mus0bio-1Google Scholar
Sur, M., Garraghty, P., & Roe, A. (1988). Experimentally induced visual projections into auditory thalamus and cortex. Science, 242, 14371441. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.2462279CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tryon, W. (2014). Cognitive neuroscience and psychotherapy. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Turkle, S. (2013). Alone together. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483488. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R., & Braga, R. (2014). Default mode network: The seat of literary creativity?. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 116117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yochum, H., & Fosdick, R. (1963). Adventure in giving: The story of the general education board. The Journal of Higher Education, 34, 119. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1980228Google Scholar
Zeki, S. (1998). Art and the brain. Daedalus, 127, 71103.Google Scholar
Zeki, S. (1999). Inner vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×