Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T20:00:56.232Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Pictorial intention, action and interpretation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Norman H. Freeman
Affiliation:
University of Bristol
Esther Adi-Japha
Affiliation:
Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan
Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Get access

Summary

Freeman and Adi-Japha show that drawing production necessarily involves several steps that are specifically analysed in this chapter. Forming an intention precedes and accompanies the action sequences making up the process of production. The emerging product, the trace left on the graphic surface, stimulates an interpretation that normally should conform to the intention. The authors argue evidence is needed that bears on the child's emerging grasp of intention–action–interpretation links. Some evidence shows that the links in the production system start operating separately by approximately age 3 years, though even earlier indications of drawing-related symbolic actions can be observed in scribblers. In that light, a special case is made for reviving research on human figure drawing. Throughout the chapter, Freeman and Adi-Japha develop an original point of view arguing that researchers should take both a backwards look at drawing rules that children have to suppress and a forwards look at the rules they newly engage with, in order to generate a process model of drawing development. Evidence coming from the literature on the so-called canonical bias in drawing is particularly enlightening in this respect. Note that this perspective shares some features with the one suggested by Jolley, in the sense that Freeman and Adi-Japha give much attention to the changes occurring in executive processes, in particular to inhibition, when they address drawing behaviour.

whenever child development is orderly enough for typical progressions to be identified, researchers often devote much effort to specifying advances that children learn to make.

Type
Chapter
Information
Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind
A Life-Span Perspective
, pp. 104 - 120
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adi-Japha, E., Levin, I. and Solomon, S. (1998). Emergence of representation in drawing: the relation between kinematic and referential aspects. Cognitive Development, 13, 23–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baxendall, M. (1985). Patterns of intention. London: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. and Markson, L. (1998). Intention and analogy in children's naming of pictorial representations. Psychological Science, 9, 200–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremner, J. G. (1985). Provoked use of height in pictures as a depth cue in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 95–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, M. V. (1985). One object behind another: young children's use of array-specific or view-specific representations. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 188–281). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, A. M. (1985). The canonical bias: young children's drawings of familiar objects. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 202–13). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (1991). Neuropsychological insight into the meaning of object concept development. In Carey, S. and Gelman, R. (eds.), The epigenesis of mind (pp. 67–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Doherty, M. J. and Wimmer, M. (2005). Children's understanding of ambiguous figures: which cognitive developments are necessary to experience reversal?Cognitive Development, 20, 407–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L. and Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance–reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research on Child Development, 51 (1, serial no. 212).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, N. H. (1972). Process and product in children's drawing. Perception, 1, 123–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, N. H. (1975). Do children draw men with arms coming out of the head?Nature, 254, 416–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (1993). Drawing: public instrument of representation. In Pratt, C. and Garton, A. F. (eds.), Systems of representation in children (pp. 113–32). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (1995). The emergence of a framework theory of pictorial reasoning. In Lange-Küttner, C. and Thomas, G. V. (eds.), Drawing and looking: theoretical approaches to pictorial representation in children (pp. 135–46). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (2000). Communication and representation: why mentalistic reasoning is a lifelong endeavour. In Mitchell, P. and Riggs, K. J. (eds.), Children's reasoning and the mind (pp. 349–67). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (2004). Aesthetic judgement and reasoning. In Eisner, E. W. and Day, M. D. (eds.), Handbook of research and policy in art education (pp. 359–78). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum/National Art Education Association (USA).Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. and Hargreaves, S. (1977). Directed movements and the body-proportion effect in preschool children's human figure drawings. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 227–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, N. H., Hood, B. M. and Meehan, C. (2004). Young children who abandon error behaviourally still have to free themselves mentally: a retrospective test for change in intuitive physics. Developmental Science, 7, 277–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, N. H. and Janikoun, R. (1972). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of a familiar object with distinctive features. Child Development, 43, 1116–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, H. (1980). Artful scribbles. New York:Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. and Ebeling, K. S. (1998). Shape and representational status in children's early naming. Cognition, 66, B35–B47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golomb, C. (1974). Young children's sculpture and drawing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Golomb, C. (1992). The child's creation of a pictorial world. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Carr, T. H. and Nigg, J. T. (2004). Perceptual load influences early versus late attentional selection in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 40, 545–58.Google Scholar
Kuhn, D. and Pease, M. (2006). Do children and adults learn differently?Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 279–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M. and Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: two conjectures. Developmental Science, 1, 247–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, C., Russell, C. and Berridge, D. (1993). When is a mug not a mug? Effects of content, naming, and instructions on children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 291–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Light, P. (1985). The development of view-specific representation when considered from a socio-cognitive standpoint. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 214–30). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lopes, D. (1997). Understanding pictures. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Lukens, H. T. (1986). A study of children's drawings in the early years. Pedagogical Seminary, 4, 79–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawing].Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Matthews, J. (2004). The art of infancy. In Eisner, E. W. and Day, M. D. (eds.), Handbook of research and policy in art education (pp. 253–98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pierroutsakos, S. L., DeLoache, J. S., Ground, M. and Bernard, E. N. (2005). Very young children are insensitive to picture- but not object-orientation. Developmental Science, 8, 326–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Picard, D. and Durand, K. (2005). Are young children's drawings canonically biased?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 48–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, C., Koinis, D., Sullivan, K., Tager-Flusberg, H. and Winner, E. (1997). When 3-year-olds pass the appearance–reality test. Developmental Psychology, 33, 54–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schier, F. (1986). Deeper into pictures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N. R. (1979). How a picture means. New Directions for Child Development, 3, 59–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, M. (1990). A transformational theory of aesthetics. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Thomas, G. V. and Silk, A. M. J. (1989). An introduction to the psychology of children's drawings. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Willats, J. (1997). Art and representation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Willats, J. (2005). Making sense of children's drawings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×