Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T02:15:09.773Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - About chicken and eggs: four methods for investigating culture–behaviour links

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ronald Fischer
Affiliation:
Victoria University of Wellington, New Zealand
Fons J. R. van de Vijver
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Athanasios Chasiotis
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Seger M. Breugelmans
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Understanding the psychological mechanisms underlying observed differences in attitudes and behaviours between groups of people in different parts of the world has arguably been one of the major driving forces for the development of cross-cultural psychology (see Lonner, this volume). Variables thought to be associated with culture are used as an independent variable to describe and explain observable differences between people raised in different contexts. Cross-cultural studies of comparative nature are by necessity quasi-experimental, since individuals cannot be randomly assigned to different treatment conditions. Despite this limitation, the quest to understand (and maybe even predict) any emergent differences and similarities is necessary for a truly human psychology. In this chapter, I outline four different approaches that may contribute to this endeavour. These are: (1) culture-level studies, (2) studies unpackaging culture–behaviour links at the individual level, (3) cross-level studies and (4) experimental priming studies. However, I also critically examine the potential of these approaches to answer the question that is being asked, namely uncovering the nature of the link between culture and behaviour. To address this link, I also briefly need to discuss another central theme of the current volume, namely what we understand by culture. A consideration of definitions of culture in light of the four approaches to understanding the culture–behaviour/attitude link will lead to the identification of some inconsistencies and paradoxes. I start off by providing a very brief review of definitions of culture and then discuss two methodological issues (equivalence, isomorphism) that have some relevance for the following discussion.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrams, D., Ando, K., and Hinkle, S. (1998). Psychological attachment to the group: Cross-cultural differences in organizational identification and subjective norms as predictors of workers' turnover intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1027–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1976). Human ecology and cognitive style: Comparative studies in cultural and psychological adaptation. New York: Sage/Halsted/Wiley.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2000). Cross-cultural psychology: A symbiosis of cultural and comparative approaches. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 197–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., and Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications (2nd edn). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bliese, P. D. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence and reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analyses. In Klein, J. K. and Kozlowski, S. W. J. (eds.), Multilevel theory, research and methods in organizations: Foundations, extensions, and new directions (pp. 349–81). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Bliese, P. D., Chan, D., and Ployhart, R. E. (2007). Multilevel methods: Future directions in measurement, longitudinal analyses and nonnormal outcomes. Organizational Research Methods, 10, 551–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, M. H. (2004). The third stage of cross-cultural psychology: Some personal prescriptions for our future. Paper presented at the 17th International Congress of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology, Xi'an, China, August.Google Scholar
Bond, M. H., Leung, K., and Schwartz, S. (1992). Explaining choices in procedural and distributive justice across cultures. International Journal of Psychology, 27, 211–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, M. H., and Vijver, F. J. R. (2011). Making scientific sense of cultural differences in psychological outcomes: Unpackaging the magnum mysterium. In Matsumoto, D. and Vijver, F. J. R. (eds.), Cross-cultural research methods (pp. 75–100). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bontempo, R., and Rivero, J. C. (1992). Cultural variation in cognition: The role of self-concept in the attitude behaviour link. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Academy of Management, Las Vegas, Nevada, August 1992.Google Scholar
Dekker, S., and Fischer, R. (2008). Cultural differences in academic motivation goals: A meta-analysis across thirteen societies. Journal of Educational Research, 101, 99–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diener, E., Diener, M., and Diener, C. (1995). Factors predicting the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 851–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckitt, J. (2001). A dual process cognitive-motivational theory of ideology and prejudice. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 41–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R., and Zanna, M. (1981). Direct experience and attitude–behavior consistency. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in experimental psychology (vol. XIV, pp. 161–202). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, R. (2009). Where is culture in cross-cultural research? An outline of a multi-level research process for measuring culture as a shared meaning system. International Journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 9, 25–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, R., and Chalmers, A. (2008). Is optimism universal? A meta-analytical investigation of optimism levels across 22 nations. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 378–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, R., and Schwartz, S. H. (2010). Whence differences in value priorities? Individual, cultural, or artifactual sources. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Electronically pre-published, 9 September 2010. doi:10.117/0022022110381429.Google Scholar
Fischer, R., and Smith, P. B. (2003). Reward allocation and culture: A meta-analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 251–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, R., and Smith, P. B. (2004). Values and organizational justice: Performance and seniority-based allocation criteria in UK and Germany. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 669–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, R., Ferreira, M. C., Assmar, E., Redford, P., Harb, C., Glazer, S., et al. (2009). Individualism–collectivism as descriptive norms: Development of a subjective norm approach to culture measurement. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 197–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (2002). Using individualism and collectivism to compare cultures – A critique of the validity and measurement of the constructs: Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 78–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fontaine, J. R. (2008). Traditional and multilevel approaches in cross-cultural research: An integration of methodological frameworks. In Vijver, F. J. R., Hemert, D. A. and Poortinga, Y. H. (eds.), Individuals and cultures in multilevel analysis (pp. 65–92). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fontaine, J. R. J. (2005). Equivalence. In Kempf-Leonard, K. (ed.), Encyclopedia of social measurement (vol. I, pp. 803–13). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frese, M., and Zapf, D. (1994). Action as the core of work psychology: A German approach. In Triandis, H. C., Dunnette, M. D. and Hough, J. M. (eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (vol. IV, pp. 271–340). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Fu, P. P., Kennedy, J., Tata, J., Yukl, G., Bond, M. H., Peng, T. K., et al. (2004). The impact of societal cultural values and individual social beliefs on the perceived effectiveness of managerial influence strategies: A meso approach. Journal of International Business Studies, 35, 284–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabreyna, W. (1999). Psychological anthropology and the ‘levels of analysis’ problem: We married the wrong cousins. In Lasry, J. C., Adair, J. and Dion, K. (eds.), Latest contributions to cross-cultural psychology (pp. 333–51). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Nishii, L. H., and Raver, J. L. (2006). On the nature and importance of cultural tightness–looseness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1225–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gintis, H. (2007). A framework for the unification of the behavioural sciences. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 30, 1–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodenough, W. H. (1972). Culture, language, and society. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hong, Y., and Chiu, C. (2001). Toward a paradigm shift: From cultural differences in social cognition to social cognitive mediation of cultural differences. Social Cognition, 19, 118–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P., and Gupta, V. (eds.) (2004). Cultures, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Hox, J. J. (1995). Applied multilevel analysis. Amsterdam: TT-Publikaties.Google Scholar
Huang, X., Vliert, E., and Vegt, G. (2006). Breaking the silence culture: Stimulation of participation and employee opinion withholding cross-nationally. Group and Organization Review, 1, 459–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hui, C. H. (1988). Measurement of individualism–collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 17–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hui, C. H., Triandis, C. H., and Yee, C. (1991). Cultural differences in reward allocation: Is collectivism an explanation?British Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 145–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and postmodernization: Cultural, economic and political change in 43 societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, B., and Fischer, R. (2007). Biculturalism in employee selection or ‘Who should get the job’? Perception of Maori and Pakeha job applicants in a NZ European sample. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 36, 100–9.Google Scholar
Ji, L. J., Nisbett, R. E., and Su, Y. (2001). Culture, change and prediction. Psychological Science, 12, 450–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashima, Y. (2000). Conceptions of culture and person for psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 14–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, D., and Kahn, R. L. (1978). The social psychology of organizations (2nd edn). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Keesing, R. M. (1974). Theories of culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 3, 73–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkman, B. L., Lowe, K. B., and Gibson, C. (2006). A quarter century of culture's consequences: A review of empirical research incorporating Hofstede's cultural values framework. Journal of International Business Studies, 37, 285–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozan, M. K., and Ergin, C. (1999). The influence of intra-cultural value differences on conflict management practices. International Journal of Conflict Management, 10, 249–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroeber, A. L., and Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture: A critical review of concepts and definitions. Cambridge, MA: Peabody Museum of American Archaeology and Ethnology, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Kuper, A. (1999). Culture – the anthropologists' account. London: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
LaPiere, R. T. (1934). Attitudes vs. actions. Social Forces, 13, 230–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehman, D. R., Chiu, C. Y., and Schaller, M. (2004). Psychology and culture. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 689–714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, M. L. (2003). The justice motive: Where social psychologists found it, how they lost it and why they may not find it again. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 388–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, K., and Bond, M. H. (1984). The impact of cultural collectivism on reward allocation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 47, 793–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, E. A., and Henne, D. (1986). Work motivation theories. In Cooper, C. L. and Robertson, I. (eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology, 1 (pp. 1–36). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lubke, G. H., Dolan, C. V., Kelderman, H., and Mellenbergh, G. J. (2003). On the relationship between sources of within- and between-group differences and measurement invariance in the common factor model. Intelligence, 31, 543–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maas, C. J. M., and Hox, J. J. (2005). Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modeling. Methodology: European Journal of Research Methods for the Behavioral and Social Sciences, 1, 85–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Weissman, M. D., Preston, K., Brown, B. R., and Kupperbusch, C. (1997). Context-specific measurement of individualism–collectivism on the individual level: The individualism–collectivism interpersonal assessment inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 743–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., and Yoo, S. H. (2006). Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 234–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R. (2001). Trait psychology and culture: Exploring intercultural comparisons. Journal of Personality, 69, 819–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., and 79 members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project (2005). Personality profiles of cultures: Aggregate personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 407–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, M. W., Williams, K. Y., Leung, K., Larrick, R., Mendoza, M. T., Bhatnagar, D., et al. (1998). Conflict management style: Accounting for cross-national differences. Journal of International Business Studies, 29, 729–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytical cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., and Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, K., and Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H. (1989). Equivalence in cross-cultural data: An overview of basic issues. InternationalJournal of Psychology, 24, 737–56.Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H., and Vijver, F. J. R. (1987). Explaining cross-cultural differences: Bias analysis and beyond. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 18, 259–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H., and Hemert, D. A. (2001). Personality and culture: Demarcating between the common and the unique. Journal of Personality, 69, 1033–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raudenbush, S. W., and Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models (2nd edn). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Reicher, S., and Hopkins, N. (2001). Self and nation. CA: Thousand Oaks.Google Scholar
Reicher, S., and Hopkins, N. (2006). Rethinking the psychology of tyranny: The BBC prison study. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 1–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohner, R. P. (1984). Toward a conception of culture for cross-cultural psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 111–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, D. M. (1968). American kinship: A cultural account. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Beyond individualism–collectivism: New cultural dimensions of values. In Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kağitçibaşi, Ç., Choi, S. C. and Yoon, G. (eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method and applications (pp. 85–119). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (1996). Value priorities and behaviour: Applying a theory of integrated value systems. In Seligman, C., Olson, J. M. and Zanna, M. P. (eds.), The psychology of values: The Ontario symposium (vol. VIII, pp. 1–24). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H., and Bilsky, W. (1987). Toward a universal psychological structure of human values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 550–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sibley, C. G., Liu, J. H., and Kirkwood, S. (2006). Toward a social representations theory of attitude change: The effect of message framing on general and specific attitudes toward equality and entitlement. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 35, 3–13.Google Scholar
Singelis, T. M., Bond, M. H., Sharkey, W. F., and Lai, C. S. Y. (1999). Unpackaging culture's influence on self-esteem and embarrassability: The role of self-construals. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 315–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. B., Bond, M. H., and Kağitçibaşi, Ç. (2006). Understanding social psychology across cultures: Living and working in changing world. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., and Fischer, R. (2008). Acquiescence, extreme response bias and levels of cross-cultural analysis. In Vijver, F. J. R., Hemert, D. A. and Poortinga, Y. H. (eds.), Individuals and cultures in multi-level analysis (pp. 283–311). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Strohschneider, S. (ed.) (1996). Denken in Deutschland: Vergleichende Untersuchungen in Ost und West. 1. Auflage. Bern: Huber.
Susser, M. (1994). The logic in ecological: 1. The logic of analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 84, 825–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd edn). New York: HarperCollins College Publishers.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. San Francisco, CA: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Vijver, F. J. R., and Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Vijver, F. J. R., and Poortinga, Y. H. (1982). Cross-cultural generalization and universality. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 13, 387–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vijver, F. J. R., and Poortinga, Y. H. (2002). Structural equivalence in multilevel research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 141–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vijver, F. J. R., Hemert, D. A., and Poortinga, Y. H. (eds.) (2008). Multilevel analysis of individuals and cultures. New York: Erlbaum.
Hemert, D. A., Vijver, F. J. R., Poortinga, Y. H., and Georgas, J. (2002). Structural and functional equivalence of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire within and between nations. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1229–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, C., Bochner, S., and Furnham, A. (2001). The psychology of culture shock. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Zimbardo, P. (1989). Quiet rage: The Stanford prison study video. Stanford, CA: Stanford University.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×