Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T01:01:32.949Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Animal Intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Thomas R. Zentall
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky, Lexington
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

ANIMAL INTELLIGENCE

With some justification, humans place themselves at the top of the evolutionary scale. If we rule out the exceptional sensory and response skills of certain nonhuman animals, clearly the sum of our intellectual capacity, measured in almost any way, exceeds that of other animals. The role of our intelligence in the domination of our species over others is clear, but in the broader perspective of evolution, intelligence per se can be viewed quite differently. Our species-centric view of intelligence may be self-serving, but as a general characteristic it correlates only superficially (and perhaps even negatively) with most measures of evolutionary success.

Survival, or more specifically, survival of the genes (Dawkins, 1976) is the measure of evolutionary success. From a purely biological perspective, the ideal survival “machine” is a simple, perhaps one-celled, organism (e.g., bacteria). By some estimates, bacteria, in spite of their small size, account for as much biomass as all other life forms combined, including plants (Gould, 1996). The survival strategy of bacteria is to reproduce quickly and often with sufficient genetic variability to accommodate changes in the environment. This has proven to be a very effective strategy also for more complex organisms like insects and many fishes. An alternative strategy is to invest more in the genetic carrier (the individual organism) and to build within the organism the flexibility required for survival.

Intelligence, in its simplest form, can be thought of as the built-in flexibility that allows individual organisms to adjust their behavior to relatively rapidly changing environments (Stenhouse, 1974, p. 61).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×