Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T08:58:08.762Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Restoring Cultural Identity Clarity in Times of Revolution

The Role of Historical Narratives

from Part III - Representations of and in Revolution

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 April 2018

Brady Wagoner
Affiliation:
Aalborg University, Denmark
Fathali M. Moghaddam
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington DC
Jaan Valsiner
Affiliation:
Aalborg University, Denmark
Get access

Summary

This chapter focuses on the consequences of revolution for ordinary group members, each of whom is forced to psychologically adjust if there is to be any hope of them successfully navigating the new reality that revolution brings. We begin by defining revolution and the most important psychological challenge that group members face when confronted by such collective trauma: its impact on cultural identity, and more specifically on cultural identity clarity. Our theoretical stance is that a clear cultural identity is critical for the well-being of each and every group member. A cultural identity that is clearly defined, we argue, is what provides each and every group member with a template for successfully navigating the social environment in order to “get along” and “get ahead” (Taylor & de la Sablonnière, 2014). The goal of the present chapter is thus to explore a possible framework by which the clarity of cultural identity might begin to be restored when it is destroyed in the context of revolution. Specifically, we will argue that a group’s shared historical narrative may be an important building block for rebuilding a new clearly defined cultural identity to serve as a guide for the new post-revolution environment. We argue that there are three main features of group-based collective historical narratives. First, historical narratives include key historical heroes who changed the course of history for the group, and who personify the behaviours and values that define the group. Second, historical narratives focus on pivotal historical events, be they positive or negative, that define the group. Finally, each collective narrative has a trajectory of historical events and heroes that are linked together to arrive at a coherent and continuous cultural identity. These shared historical narratives can help traumatized group members who need to reconstruct their cultural identity. The group’s historical narrative can provide a foundation for rebuilding identity through its concrete historical examples, and by offering the expanded time perspective necessary to help define long-term collective goals moving forward.
Type
Chapter
Information
The Psychology of Radical Social Change
From Rage to Revolution
, pp. 252 - 274
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, G., and Markus, H. R. (2001). Culture as patterns: An alternative approach to the problem of reification. Culture and Psychology, 7(3), 283296.Google Scholar
Adler, P. (1977). Beyond cultural identity: Reflections on multiculturalism. In Brislin, R. W. (Ed.), Culture Learning (pp. 2441). Honolulu: East-West Center Press.Google Scholar
Albert, S. (1977). Temporal comparison theory. Psychological Review, 84(6), 485503.Google Scholar
Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., and McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizing framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130(1), 80114.Google Scholar
Assmann, J., and Czaplicka, J. (1995). Collective memory and cultural identity. New German Critique, 65, 125133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself: Self-certainty and self-affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 10621072.Google Scholar
Bikmen, N. (2015). History as a resource: Effects of narrative constructions of group history on intellectual performance. Journal of Social Issues, 71(2), 309323.Google Scholar
Bougie, E., Usborne, E., de la Sablonnière, R., and Taylor, D. M. (2011). The cultural narratives of Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers: Using a historical perspective to explore the relationships among collective relative deprivation, in-group entitativity, and collective esteem. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(4), 726746.Google Scholar
Brown, R., and Middendorf, J. (1996). The underestimated role of temporal comparison: A test of the life-span model. Journal of Social Psychology, 136(3), 325331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(3), 538549.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312(5782), 19131915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castano, E., Yzerbyt, V., Paladino, M.-P., and Sacchi, S. (2002). I belong, therefore, I exist: Ingroup identification, ingroup entitativity, and ingroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(2), 135143.Google Scholar
Chandler, M. J., and Lalonde, C. (1998). Cultural continuity as a hedge against suicide in Canada's First Nations. Transcultural Psychiatry, 35(2), 191220.Google Scholar
Dambrun, M., Taylor, D. M., McDonald, D. A., Crush, J., and Méot, A. (2006). The relative deprivation-gratification continuum and the attitudes of South Africans towards immigrants: A test of the V-curve hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 10321044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R. (2007). “Désélectriser” le choc culturel: le malaise identitaire chez l'intervenant. Équilibre, 2, 3841.Google Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R. (2017). Toward a psychology of social change: A typology of social change. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Auger, É., Sadykova, N., and Taylor, D. M. (2010). When the “We” impacts how “I” feel about myself: Effect of temporal collective relative deprivation on personal well-being in the context of dramatic social change in Kyrgyzstan. European Psychologist, 15(4), 271282.Google Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Auger, E., Taylor, D. M., Crush, J., and McDonald, D. (2012). Social change in South Africa: A historical approach to relative deprivation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 52(4), 703725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de la Sablonnière, R., Hénault, A. M., and Huberdeau, M. É. (2009). Comparaisons sociales et comparaisons temporelles: vers une approche séquentielle et fonction de la situation unique [Social and temporal comparisons: Towards a sequential approach and function of the unique situation]. Les Cahiers internationaux de psychologie sociale, 83, 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Pinard St-Pierre, F., Taylor, D. M., and Annahatak, J. (2011). Cultural narratives and clarity of cultural identity: Understanding the well-being of Inuit youth. Pimatisiwin, 9(2), 301322.Google Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Taylor, D. M., Perozzo, C., and Sadykova, N. (2009). Reconceptualizing relative deprivation in the context of dramatic social change: The challenge confronting the people of Kyrgyzstan. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(3), 325345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Taylor, D. M., Pinard Saint-Pierre, F., and Annahatak, J. (2011). Cultural narratives and clarity of cultural identity: Understanding the well-being of inuit youth. Primitivism: A Journal of Aboriginal and Indigenous Community Health, 9(2), 301322.Google Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Tougas, F., and Lortie-Lussier, M. (2009). Dramatic social change in Russia and Mongolia connecting relative deprivation to social identity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(3), 327348.Google Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Tougas, F., and Perenlei, O. (2009). Beyond social and temporal comparisons: The role of temporal inter-group comparisons in the context of dramatic social change in Mongolia. Journal of Social Psychology, 150(1), 98115.Google Scholar
de la Sablonnière, R., Tougas, F., and Perenlei, O. (2010). Beyond social and temporal comparisons: the role of temporal inter-group comparisons in the context of dramatic social change in Mongolia. Journal of Social Psychology, 150, 98115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de la Sablonnière, R., Tougas, F., Taylor, D. M., Crush, J., McDonald, D., and Perenlei, O. R. (2015). Social change in Mongolia and South Africa: The impact of relative deprivation trajectory and group status on well-being and adjustment to change. Social Justice Research, 28(1), 102122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
European Institute for Research on Mediterranean and Euro-Arab Cooperation. (n.d.). Al-Assad, Hafez. www.medea.be/en/themes/biographies/abc/al-assad-hafez/.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7(2), 117140.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1962). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Frye, N. E., and Karney, B. R. (2002). Being better or getting better? Social and temporal comparisons as coping mechanisms in close relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(9), 12871299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J., Porteus, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., and Solomon, S. (1995). Evidence of a terror management function of cultural icons: The effects of mortality salience on the inappropriate use of cherished symbols. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(11), 12211228.Google Scholar
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., and Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (29:61139). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Guimond, S., and Dambrun, M. (2002). When prosperity breeds intergroup hostility: The effects of relative deprivation and gratification on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(7), 900912.Google Scholar
Hains, S. C., Hogg, M. A., and Duck, J. M. (1997). Self-categorization and leadership: Effects of group prototypicality and leader stereotypicality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(10), 10871099.Google Scholar
Hammack, P. L. (2008). Narrative and the cultural psychology of identity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(3), 222247.Google Scholar
Hammack, P. L. (2010). Identity as a burden and benefit? Youth, historical narrative, and the legacy of political conflict. Human Development, 71(4), 173201.Google Scholar
Heslop, D. A. (2016). Political system. In Encyclopædia Britannica. www.britannica.com/topic/political-system/Stable-political-systems.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A. (2004). Uncertainty and extremism: Identification with high entitativity groups under conditions of uncertainty. In Yzerbyt, V., Judd, C. M., and Corneille, O. (Eds.), The Psychology of Group Perception: Perceived Variability, Entitativity, and Essentialism (pp. 401418). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kammen, M. (2008). The American past politicized: Uses and misuses of history. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Sciences, 617(1), 4257.Google Scholar
Li, Q., and Brewer, M. B. (2004). What does it mean to be an American? Patriotism, nationalism, and American identity after 9/11. Political Psychology, 25(5), 727739.Google Scholar
Liu, J. H., and Hilton, D. J. (2005). How the past weighs on the present: Social representations of history and their role in identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44(4), 537556.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7(4), 295321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5(2), 100122.Google Scholar
McFarland, C., and Alvaro, C. (2000). The impact of motivation on temporal comparisons: Coping with traumatic events by perceiving personal growth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 327343.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M., and Crystal, D. S. (1997). Revolutions, samurai, and reductions: The paradoxes of change and continuity in Iran and Japan. Political Psychology, 18(2), 355384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mummendey, A., Mielke, R., Wenzel, M., and Kanning, U. (1992). Die Roller sozialer Vergleiche bei der Bewertung der eigenen Lebenssituation in Ostdeutschland [The role of social comparisons in evaluating one's own living conditions in Eastern Germany]. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Okazaki, S., David, E. J. R., and Abelmann, N. (2008). Colonialism and the psychology of culture. Social and Personality Compass, 2(1), 90106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., Kemmelmeier, M., and Coon, H. M. (2002). Cultural psychology, a new look: Reply to Bond (2002), Fiske (2002), Kitayama (2002), and Miller (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 110117.Google Scholar
Parsons, T. (1964). The Social System. New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pelletier-Dumas, M., and de la Sablonnière, R. (2017). Dramatic personal change impairs personal identity clarity: The moderating role of identity integration. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Pelletier-Dumas, M., de la Sablonnière, R., and Duchesne-Beauchamp, M. (2016). Dramatic social change and its impact on cultural identity clarity. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., and Banasik, B. L. (1997). On the creation and maintenance of collective memories: History as social psychology. In Paez, D., Pennebaker, J. W., and Rimé, B. (Eds.), Collective Memory of Political Events: Social Psychological Perspectives (pp. 320). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Paez, D., and Rimé, B. (Eds.) (1997). Collective Memory of Political Events: Social Psychological Perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pinquart, M., and Silbereisen, R. K. (2004). Human development in times of social change: Theoretical considerations and research needs. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(4), 289298.Google Scholar
Pyszczynski, T., Wicklund, R. A., Floresku, S., Koch, H., Gauch, G., Solomon, S., and Greenberg, J. (1996). Whistling in the dark: Exaggerated consensus estimates in response to incidental reminders of mortality. Psychological Science, 7(6), 332336.Google Scholar
Reicher, S. D. (2008). Making a past fit for the future: The political and ontological dimensions of historical continuity. In Sani, F. (Ed.), Self-Continuity: Individual and Collective Perspectives (pp. 145158). London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Reicher, S. D., and Hopkins, N. (2001). Self and Nation. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Ross, M., Hein, S. J., Wilson, A. E., and Sugimori, S. (2005). Cross-cultural discrepancies in self-appraisals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(9), 11751188.Google Scholar
Ross, M., and Wilson, A. E. (2002). It feels like yesterday: Self-esteem, valence of personal past experiences, and judgments of subjective distance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(2), 792803.Google Scholar
Sani, F., Herrera, M., and Bowe, M. (2009). Perceived collective continuity and ingroup identification as defence against death awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(1), 242245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9), 657661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, S. J., Kim, S. Y., Whitbourne, S. K., Zamboanga, B. L., Weisskirch, R. S., Forthun, L. F., et al. (2013). Converging identities: Dimensions of acculturation and personal identity status among immigrant college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 19(2), 155165.Google Scholar
Smeekes, A., and Verkuyten, M. (2015). The presence of the past: Identity continuity and group dynamics. European Review of Social Psychology, 26(1), 162202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stinson, D. A., Wood, J. V., and Doxey, J. R. (2008). In search of clarity: Self-esteem and domains of confidence and confusion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(11), 15411555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E. A., DeVinney, L. C., Star, S. A., and Williams, R. M., Jr. (1949). The American Soldier: Adjustment during Army Life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Sussman, N. M. (2000). The dynamic nature of cultural identity throughout cultural transitions: Why home is not so sweet. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(4), 355373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Austin, W. G. and Worchel, S. (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 3347). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. M. (1997). The quest for collective identity: The plight of disadvantaged ethnic minorities. Canadian Psychology, 38(3), 174189.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. M. (2002). The Quest for Identity: From Minority Groups to Generation Xers. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. M., Debrosse, R. G., Cooper, M., and Kachanoff, F. (2013). Cultural identity clarity. In Sammut, G., Daanen, P., and Moghaddam, F. M. (Eds.), Understanding the Self and Others: Explorations in Intersubjectivity and Interobjectivity (pp. 143160). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. M., and de la Sablonnière, R. (2014). Towards Constructive Change in Aboriginal Communities: A Social Psychology Perspective. Montreal: McGill-Queen's Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, D. M., and Usborne, E. (2010). When I know who “We” are, I can be “Me”: The primary role of cultural identity clarity for psychological well-being. Transcultural Psychiatry, 47(1), 93111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilly, C. (1996). European Revolutions: 1492–1992. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social-cognitive theory of group behaviour. In Lawler, E. J. (Ed.), Advances in Group Processes: Theory and Research (pp. 2:77122). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Usborne, E., and Taylor, D. M. (2010). The role of cultural identity clarity for self-concept clarity, self-esteem, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(7), 883897.Google Scholar
Usborne, E., and Taylor, D. M. (2012). Using computer-mediated communication as a tool for exploring the impact of cultural identity clarity on psychological well-being. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), 183191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. (1997). Narrative tools of history and identity. Culture and Psychology, 3(1), 520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, A., and Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People's appraisals of their earlier and present selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(4), 572584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoder, D. (1926). Current definitions of revolution. American Journal of Sociology, 32(3), 433441.Google Scholar
Zagefka, H., and Brown, R. (2005). Comparisons and perceived deprivation in ethnic minority settings. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(4), 467482.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×