Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T07:09:08.457Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Some Biobehavioral Insights into Persistent Effects of Emotional Trauma

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 July 2009

Mark E. Bouton
Affiliation:
Professor Department of Psychology, University of Vermont
Jaylyn Waddell
Affiliation:
Postdoctoral Associate Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, NJ
Laurence J. Kirmayer
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montréal
Robert Lemelson
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Mark Barad
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Get access

Summary

For many years, our laboratory has been interested in the biobehavioral mechanisms of classical conditioning and, in particular, the mechanisms behind the conditioning, extinction, and inhibition of fear (e.g., Bouton, 1993, 2002, 2004). In fear conditioning, an organism learns to associate environmental stimuli with a frightening event, and those stimuli consequently evoke fear. Fear can be defined as a loosely coordinated set of physiological, behavioral, and cognitive responses that are designed to get the organism ready for a future aversive event.

Fear conditioning has long been thought to play a role in many anxiety disorders (e.g., Barlow, 2002; Bouton, Mineka, & Barlow, 2001; Mineka & Zinbarg, 1996). It is not difficult to see its relevance when humans are exposed to the type of very intense emotional trauma that can lead to posttraumatic stress disorder, or PTSD (e.g., Pitman, Shalev, & Orr, 2000). Thus, a combat veteran might associate the sound of a helicopter with a horrific battle experience, or a person involved in a collision with a train might associate the blast of the horn and the flash of the headlight with the trauma of the crash. When any of these cues is later encountered or imagined, it might therefore trigger a number of physiological and cognitive responses, the so-called “reexperiencing criteria” used in diagnosing PTSD (e.g., Pitman et al., 2000). And further, the facts that fear elicited this way can motivate avoidance behavior (e.g., Rescorla & Solomon, 1967) and engage analgesic responses (e.g., Bolles & Fanselow, 1980) presumably contribute to the diagnostic “avoidance/numbing” criteria.

Type
Chapter
Information
Understanding Trauma
Integrating Biological, Clinical, and Cultural Perspectives
, pp. 41 - 59
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akins, C. K., Domjan, M., & Gutierrez, G. (1994). Topography of sexually conditioned behavior in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) depends on the conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 199–209.Google Scholar
Ayres, J. J. B., Berger-Gross, P., Kohler, E. A., Mahoney, W. J., & Stone, S. (1979).Some orderly nonmonotonicities in the trial-by-trial acquisition of conditioned suppression: Inhibition with reinforcement?Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, 174–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety and panic (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Bolles, R. C., & Fanselow, M. S. (1980). A perceptual-defensive-recuperative model of fear and pain. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 3, 291–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouton, M. E. (1984).Differential control by context in the inflation and reinstatement paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 56–74.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E. (1988). Context and ambiguity in the extinction of emotional learning: Implications for exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 26, 137–149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E. (2002). Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 976–986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 485–494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E. (2005). Behavior systems and the contextual control of anxiety, fear and panic. In Barrett, L. F., Niedenthal, P., & Winkelman, P. (Eds.), Emotion: Conscious and unconscious (pp. 205–227).New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E., & Bolles, R. C. (1979a). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation, 10, 445–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouton, M. E., & Bolles, R. C. (1979b). Role of conditioned contextual stimuli in reinstatement of extinguished fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 368–378.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E., & Brooks, D. C. (1993). Time and context effects on performance in a Pavlovian discrimination reversal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 165–179.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E., Kenney, F. A., & Rosengard, C. (1990). State-dependent fear extinction with two benzodiazepine tranquilizers. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 44–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E., & King, D. A. (1983). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear: Tests for the associative value of the context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 248–265.Google ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E., & King, D. A. (1986). Effect of context on performance to conditioned stimuli with mixed histories of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 4–15.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S., & Barlow, D. H. (2001). A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder. Psychological Review, 108, 4–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bouton, M. E., & Nelson, J. B. (1994).Context-specificity of target versus feature inhibition in a feature-negative discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 51–65.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E., & Peck, C. A. (1989). Context effects on conditioning, extinction, and reinstatement in an appetitive conditioning preparation. Animal Learning & Behavior, 17, 188–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouton, M. E., & Peck, C. A. (1992). Recovery in cross-motivational transfer (counterconditioning). Animal Learning & Behavior, 20, 313–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouton, M. E., & Ricker, S. T. (1994). Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 317–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1991). Sources of relapse after extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 123–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremner, J. D., Licinio, J., Darnell, A., Krystal, J. H., Owens, M., Southwick, S. M., et al. (1997). Elevated CSF corticotrophin-releasing factor concentrations in posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 624–629.Google ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. D., Southwick, S. M., Johnson, D. R., Yehuda, R., & Charney, D. S. (1993). Childhood physical abuse and combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 235–239.Google ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N., & Davis, G. C. (1992).Posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults: Risk factors for chronicity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 671–675.Google Scholar
Brooks, D. C. (2000). Recent and remote extinction cues reduce spontaneous recovery. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53B, 25–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, D. C., & Bouton, M. E. (1993). A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates spontaneous recovery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 19, 77–89.Google ScholarPubMed
Brooks, D. C., & Bouton, M. E. (1994). A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates response recovery (renewal) caused by a return to the conditioning context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 366–379.Google Scholar
Brooks, D. C., Hale, B., Nelson, J. B., & Bouton, M. E. (1995).Reinstatement after counterconditioning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 23, 383–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. S., Kalish, H. I., & Farber, I. E. (1951). Conditioned fear as revealed by magnitude of startle response to an auditory stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 317–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, B. N., & Brandon, T. H. (2002). Effects of extinction context and retrieval cues on alcohol cue reactivity among nonalcoholic drinkers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 390–397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corcoran, K. A., & Maren, S. (2004). Factors regulating the effects of hippocampal inactivation on renewal of conditional fear after extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 598–603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cullinan, W. E., Herman, J. P., & Watson, S. J. (1993). Ventral subicular interaction with the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus: Evidence for a relay in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 332, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, C. L. (1979). Alcohol as a cue for extinction: State dependency produced by conditioned inhibition. Animal Learning & Behavior, 7, 45–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M., & Lee, Y. (1998).Fear and anxiety: Possible roles of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 277–305.Google Scholar
Davis, M., & Shi, C. (1999).The extended amygdala: Are the central nucleus of the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis differentially involved in fear versus anxiety?Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 877, 281–291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M., Walker, D. L., & Lee, Y. (1997).Roles for the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in fear and anxiety measured with the acoustic startle reflex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 821, 305–331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domjan, M. (1994). Formulation of a behavior system for sexual conditioning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 421–428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanselow, M. S. (2000). Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus. Behavioural Brain Research, 110, 73–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanselow, M. S., & Lester, L. S. (1988). A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. In Bolles, R. C. & Beecher, M. D.(Eds.), Evolution and learning (pp. 185–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Francis, D. D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Maternal care and the development of stress response. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 128–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frohardt, R. J., Guarraci, F. A., & Bouton, M. E. (2000). The effects of neurotoxic hippocampal lesions on two effects of context after fear extinction. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 227–240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitchcock, J. M., & Davis, M. (1991). Efferent pathway of the amygdala involved in conditioned fear as measured with the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 826–842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, H., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1990). A change of context can enhance performance of an aversive but not of an appetitive conditioned response. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42B, 113–134.Google Scholar
Kim, J. J., & Fanselow, M. S. (1992). Modality specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science, 256, 675–677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinzie, J. D., Boehnlein, M. D., Riley, M. A., & Sparr, L. (2002). The effects of September 11 on traumatized refugees: Reactivation of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 437–441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotional circuits in the brain. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 23, 155–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, Y., & Davis, M. (1997).Role of the hippocampus, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and the amygdala in the excitatory effect of corticotropin-releasing hormone on the acoustic startle reflex. Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 6434–6446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, D., Tannenbum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A., Sharma, S.,. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277, 1659–1662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McHugh, S. B., Deacon, R. M. J., Rawlins, J. N. P., & Bannerman, D. M. (2004). Amygdala and ventral hippocampus contribute differentially to mechanisms of fear and anxiety. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 63–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (1996). Conditioning and ethological models of anxiety disorders: Stress-in-dynamic-context anxiety models. InHope, D. A.(Ed.), Current theory and research in motivation: Vol. 43. Perspectives on anxiety, panic, and fear (pp. 135–210).Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Mystkowski, J. L., Craske, M. G., & Echiverri, A. M. (2002). Treatment context and return of fear in spider phobia. Behavior Therapy, 33, 399–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mystkowski, J. L., Craske, M. G., Echiverri, A. M., & Labus, J. S. (2006). Mental reinstatement of context and return of fear in spider-fearful participants. Behavior Therapy, 37, 49–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. B., & Bouton, M. E. (1997). The effects of a context switch following serial and simultaneous feature-negative discriminations. Learning and Motivation, 28, 56–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orr, S. P., Metzger, L. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., Peri, T., & Pitman, R. K. (2000). De novo conditioning in trauma-exposed individuals with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 290–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overmier, J. B., Payne, J., Brackbill, R. M., Linder, B., & Lawry, J. A. (1979). On the mechanism of the post-asymptotic conditioned response decrement phenomenon. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 39, 603–620.Google ScholarPubMed
Overton, D. A. (1985). Contextual stimulus effects of drugs and internal states. In Balsam, P. D. & Tomie, A. (Eds.), Context and learning (pp. 357–384). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peck, C. A., & Bouton, M. E. (1990). Context and performance in aversive-to-appetitive and appetitive-to-aversive transfer. Learning and Motivation, 21, 1–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, R. G., & LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106, 274–285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitman, R. K., Shalev, A. Y., & Orr, S. P. (2000). Posttraumatic stress disorder: Emotion, conditioning and memory. InGazzanig, M. S. (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 1133–1147). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Heth, C. (1975). Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 88–96.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Solomon, R. L. (1967).Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychological Review, 74, 151–182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosas, J. M., & Bouton, M. E. (1997). Renewal of a conditioned taste aversion upon return to the conditioning context after extinction in another one. Learning and Motivation, 28, 216–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosas, J. M., Vila, N. J., Lugo, M., & Lopez, L. (2001).Combined effect of context change and retention interval on interference in causality judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 153–164.Google ScholarPubMed
Shalev, A. Y., & Ursano, R. (2003). Mapping the multidimensional picture of acute responses to traumatic stress. In Orner, R. & Schnyder, V.(Eds.), Reconstructing early intervention after trauma: Innovations in the care of survivors (pp. 118–129). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M., & Vela, E. (2001). Environmental context–dependent memory: A review and meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 203–220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, A., Garb, R., Bleich, A., & Grupper, D. (1987). Reactivation of trauma-related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 51–55.Google Scholar
Sullivan, G. M., Apergis, J., Bush, D. E. A., Johnson, L. R., Hou, M., & LeDoux, J. E. (2004). Lesions in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis disrupt corticosterone and freezing responses elicited by a contextual but not a specific cue-conditioned fear stimulus. Neuroscience, 128, 7–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, N., Roberts, L., & Cassell, M. D. (1991). Rat central amygdaloid nucleus projections to the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Brain Research Bulletin, 27, 651–662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timberlake, W. (2001). Motivational modes in behavior systems. In Mowrer, R. R. & Klein, S. B.(Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories (pp. 155–210). Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Toren, P., Wolmer, L., Weizman, R., Magal-Vardi, O., & Laor, N. (2002).Retraumatization of Israeli civilians during a reactivation of the Gulf War threat. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190, 43–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treit, D., Pesold, C., & Rotzinger, S. (1993). Dissociating the anti-fear effects of septal and amygdaloid lesions using two pharmacologically validated models of rat anxiety. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 770–785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanDerCar, D. H., & Schneiderman, N. (1967). Interstimulus interval functions in different response systems during classical discrimination conditioning of rabbits. Psychonomic Science, 9, 9–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vigorito, M., & Ayres, J. J. B. (1987).Effect of naloxone on conditioned suppression in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 101, 576–586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waddell, J., Falls, W. A., Bouton, M. E., & Lowe, H. B. (2006). Central antagonism of central corticotropin-releasing factor blocks reinstatement of extinguished fear. Manuscript in preparation.
Waddell, J., Morris, R., & Bouton, M. E. (2006). Effects of bed nucleus of the stria terminalis lesions on conditioned anxiety: Reinstatement and aversive conditioning with long-duration conditional stimuli. BehavioralNeuroscience, 120, 324–336.Google ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A. R., & Brandon, S. E. (1989). Evolution of a structured connectionist model of Pavlovian conditioning (AESOP). In Klein, S. B. & Mowrer, R. R. (Eds.), Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the states of traditional learning theory (pp. 149–189). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wilson, A., Brooks, D. C., & Bouton, M. E. (1995). The role of the rat hippocampal system in several effects of context in extinction. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 828–836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford,CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Yehuda, R., Giller, E. L., Southwick, S. M., Lowy, M. T., & Mason, J. W. (1991).Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 30, 1031–1048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R., Golier, J. A., Halligan, S. L., Meaney, M., & Bierer, L. M. (2004). The adrenocorticotropic hormone response to dexamethasone in PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1397–1403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R., Schmeidler, J., Wainberg, M., Binder-Brynes, K., & Duvdevani, T. (1998). Vulnerability to posttraumatic stress disorder in adult offspring of Holocaust survivors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1163–1171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, R., Southwick, S. M., Nussbaum, E. L., Giller, E. L., & Mason, J. W. (1991). Low urinary cortisol in PTSD. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 366–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×