Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T19:31:58.369Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Susan E. Gathercole*
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Cath Willis
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Hazel Emslie
Affiliation:
MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, England
Alan D. Baddeley
Affiliation:
MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, England
*
Dr. Susan Gathercole, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, England

Abstract

It has recently been suggested that the developmental association between nonword repetition performance and vocabulary knowledge reflects the contribution of phonological memory processes to vocabulary acquisition (e.g., Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989). An alternative account of the association is that the child uses existing vocabulary knowledge to support memory for nonwords. The present article tests between these two alternative accounts by evaluating the role of phonological memory and linguistic factors in nonword repetition. In a longitudinal database, repetition accuracy in 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds was found to be sensitive to two independent factors: a phonological memory factor, nonword length, and a linguistic factor, wordlikeness. To explain these combined influences, it is suggested that repeating nonwords involves temporary phonological memory storage which may be supported by either a specific lexical analogy or by an appropriate abstract phonological frame generated from structurally similar vocabulary items.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1985). Spelling ability in congenital dysarthria: Evidence against articulatory coding in translating between phonemes and graphemes. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 2, 229251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Byers Brown, B., & Robson, J. (in press). The relationship between phoneme discrimination, speech production and language comprehension in cerebral-palsied individuals. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research.Google Scholar
Bislacchi, P. S., Cipolotti, L., & Denes, G. (1989). Impairments in processing meaningless verbal material in several modalities: The relationship between short-term memory and phonological skills. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 293320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bub, D., Black, S., Howell, J., & Kertesz, A. (1987). Speech output processes and reading. In Coltheart, M., Sartori, S., & Job, R. (Eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., & Villa, G. (1986). The role of the (output) phonological buffer in reading, writing and repetition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 3776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Windsor: NFER-Nelson Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. D. (1983). British Abilities Scales. Windsor: NFER-Nelson Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1972). An associative basis for coding and organization in memory. In Melton, A. W. & Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory. Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Fletcher, P. (1987). Aspects of language development in preschool years. In Yule, W. & Rutter, M. (Eds.), Language development and disorders. London: Mac Keith Press.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1990). Working memory and language development: How close is the link? The Psychologist, 2, 5760.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990 a). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990 b). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning arbitrary names of toys. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674691.Google Scholar
Hitch, G. J., & Halliday, M. S. (1983). Working memory in children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London, B302, 325340.Google Scholar
Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C., & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, J., & Marcel, A. (1981). One process, not two, in reading aloud: Lexical analogies do the work of lexical rules. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 397414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A., & Nicely, P. E. (1955). An analysis of perceptual confusions among some English consonants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27, 338352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolson, R. (1981). The relationship between memory span and processing speed. In Friedman, M., Das, J. P., & O’Connor, N. (Eds.), Intelligence and learning. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Raven, J. (1984). Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Service, E. (1989). Phonological coding in working memory and foreign-language learning. University of Helsinki General Psychology Monographs, B9.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. (1987). Dyslexia. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Snowling, M., Goulandris, N., Bowlby, M., & Howell, P. (1986). Segmentation and speech perception in relation to reading skill: A developmental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 489507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H. G., Lean, D., & Schwartz, S. (1989). Pseudoword repetition ability in learning-disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 203219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A. D. (1984). Fractionation of working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for a short-term store. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, T. G., Power, M. A., & White, A. (1989). Morphological analysis: Implications for teaching and understanding vocabulary growth. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 283304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfus, B., Moscovitch, M., & Kinsbourne, M. (1980). Subgroups of developmental language impairment. Brain and Language, 10, 152171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed