Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T23:21:43.481Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The language phenotype of a small geographically isolated Russian-speaking population: Implications for genetic and clinical studies of developmental language disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 March 2012

NATALIA RAKHLIN
Affiliation:
Yale University
SERGEY A. KORNILOV
Affiliation:
Yale University, University of Connecticut, Haskins Laboratories, and Moscow State University
DEAN PALEJEV
Affiliation:
Yale University
ROMAN A. KOPOSOV
Affiliation:
University of Tromsø and Northern State Medical University, Arkhangelsk
JOSEPH T. CHANG
Affiliation:
Yale University
ELENA L. GRIGORENKO*
Affiliation:
Yale University, Haskins Laboratories, Moscow State University, and Columbia University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Elena L. Grigorenko, Child Study Center, 230 South Frontage Road, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06519-1124. E-mail: elena.grigorenko@yale.edu

Abstract

This article describes the results of an epidemiological study of developmental language disorder (DLD) in an isolated rural Russian population. We report an atypically high prevalence of DLD across all age groups when contrasted with a comparison population. The results are corroborated by a set of comparisons of school-aged children from the target population with their age peers and mean length of utterance matches from the comparison population. We also investigate the relationship between nonverbal cognition, verbal working memory, and expressive language performance in the population, and find statistically significant but small effect sizes. Finally, we describe the complex and heterogeneous structure of the phenotype in the population along with patterns of its vertical transmission on the basis of the exemplar pedigrees, and discuss the implications of our findings for genetic and clinical studies of DLD.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 493–414.Google Scholar
Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1996). Phonological working memory and spoken language development in children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 216233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Addis, L., Friederici, A. D., Kotz, S. A., Sabisch, B., Barry, J., Richter, N., et al. (2010). A locus for an auditory processing deficit and language impairment in an extended pedigree maps to 12p13.31–q14.3. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 9, 541561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Archibald, L., & Gathercole, S. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 970983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 919924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavin, E. L., Wilson, P., Maruff, P., & Sleeman, F. (2005). Spatio-visual memory of children with specific language impairment: Evidence for generalized processing problems. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 319332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M. (1992). The underlying nature of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M. (1994). Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations? Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 507550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). The role of genes in the etiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders; 35, 311328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., Adams, C. V., & Norbury, C. F. (2006). Distinct genetic influences on grammar and phonological short-term memory deficits: evidence from 6-year-old twins. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 5, 158169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D. V. M., Bright, P., James, J., Bishop, S., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2000). Grammatical SLI: A distinct subtype of developmental language impairment? Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 159181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Chan, J., Adams, C., Hartley, J., & Weir, F. (2000). Conversational responsiveness in specific language impairment: Evidence of disproportionate pragmatic difficulties in a subset of children. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 177199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1995). Genetic basis of specific language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 5671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N. (2002). Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 18, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botting, N. (2005). Non-verbal cognitive development and language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 317326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, J. (2001). Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 421432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bracken, B. A., & McCalum, R. S. (1998). Universal Nonverbal Intelligence Test (UNIT). Chicago: Riverside Publishing.Google Scholar
Brackenbury, T., & Pye, C. (2005). Semantic deficits in children with language impairments: Issues for clinical assessment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, T., & Keller, M. C. (2006). Endophenotypes in the genetic analyses of mental disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 267290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, R. B., & Cattell, A. K. S. (1963). A Culture-Fair Intelligence Test. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 11421157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Botting, N. (1999). Classification of children with specific language impairment: Longitudinal considerations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 11951204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 741748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., Crutchley, A., & Botting, N. (1997). The extent to which psychometric tests differentiate subgroups of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 765777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2007). Phonological short-term memory, language and literacy: Developmental relationships in early adolescence in young people with SLI. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 147156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deevy, P., & Leonard, L. B. (2004). The comprehension of wh-questions in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 802815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeThorne, L. S., & Watkins, R. V. (2006). Language abilities and nonverbal IQ in children with language impairment: Inconsistency across measures. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 20, 641658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falcaro, M., Pickles, A., Newbury, D. F., Addis, L., Banfield, E., Fisher, S. E., e al. (2008). Genetic and phenotypic effects of phonological short-term memory and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 7, 393402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falconer, D. S. (1965). The inheritance of liability to certain diseases estimated from the incidence among relatives. Annals of Human Genetics, 29, 5176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felsenfeld, S., & Plomin, R. (1997). Epidemiological and offspring analyses of developmental speech disorders using data from the Colorado adoption project. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 778791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fidler, L. J., Plante, E., & Vance, R. (2011). Identification of adults with developmental language impairments. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 20, 213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2004). The acquisition of relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: A study of SLI and normal development. Journal of Child Language, 31, 661681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2011). Which questions are most difficult to understand?: The comprehension of Wh questions in three subtypoes of SLI. Lingua, 121, 367382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1996). The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition. London: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M.B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 636645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., & Else-Quest, N. M. (2007). Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 177195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakansson, G., & Hansson, K. (2000). Comprehension and production of relative clauses: A comparison between Swedish impaired and unimpaired children. Journal of Child Language, 27, 313–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansson, K., & Nettelbladt, U. (2006). Wh-questions in Swedish children with SLI. Advances in Speech–Language Pathology, 8, 376383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 149171.Google ScholarPubMed
Horwitz, S. M., Irwin, J. R., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Bosson Heenan, J. M., Mendoza, J., & Carter, A. S. (2003). Language delay in a community cohort of young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 932940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamhi, A. G., Ward, M. F., & Mills, E. A. (1995). Hierarchical planning abilities in children with specific language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 11081116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kere, J. (2001). Human population genetics: Lessons from Finland. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 2, 103128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, C. S., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 413, 519523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P., Miller, C., Rauf, L., Charest, M., & Robert, K. (2003). Surface forms and grammatical functions: Past tense and passive participle use by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 4355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Wong, A. M.-Y., Deevy, P., Stokes, S. F., & Fletcher, P. (2006). The production of passives by children with specific language impairment: Acquiring English or Cantonese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 267299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maillart, C., & Parisse, C. (2006). Phonological deficits in French speaking children with SLI. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 253274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marinellie, S. A. (2004). Complex syntax used by school-age children with specific language impairment (SLI) in child–adult conversation. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 517533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, C. R., Marinis, T., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). Passive verb morphology: The effect of phonotactics on passive comprehension in typically developing and grammatical-SLI children. Lingua, 117, 14341447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 9981014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, C. A. (2001). False belief understanding in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 7386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, J. W. (1995). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: The role of phonological working memory. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 187199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moseev, I. I. (2005). Pomor'ska govoria. Kratkii slovar’ pomorskogo iazyka [Pomor'ska govoria. A brief dictionary of Pomor language]. Arkhangelsk: Pravda Severa.Google Scholar
Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning: AGS Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Newbury, D. F., Bishop, D. V. M., & Monaco, A. P. (2005). Genetic influences on language impairment and phonological short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 528534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newbury, D. F., & Monaco, A. P. (2010). Genetic advances in the study of speech and language disorders. Neuron, 68, 309320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newbury, D. F., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Paracchini, S., Buckingham, L.-L., Clark, A., et al. (2009). CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 2641272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Narrative skills of children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 287313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parisse, C., & Maillart, C. (2009). Specific language impairment as systematic developmental disorders. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 109122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plante, E. (1998). Criteria tor SLI: The Stark and Tallal legacy and beyond. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 451957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plante, E., & Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool language tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 1524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podvysotskii, A. (1885). Slovar’ oblastnogo arkhangelskogo narechia [Dictionary of regional Arkhangelsk dialect]. Saint-Petersburg: Publishing House of the Imperial Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Post, M. (2006). The dialect of Varzuga and its neighbors. Slavica Helsingiensia, 27, 309321.Google Scholar
Rakhlin, N., Kornilov, S. A., Reich, J., Babyonyshev, M., Koposov, R. A., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2011). The relationship between syntactic development and Theory of Mind: Evidence from a small-population study of a developmental language disorder. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24, 476496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapin, I., & Allen, D. A. (1983). Developmental language disorders: Nosological considerations. In Kirk, U. (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language, reading and spelling (pp. 155184). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Oetting, J. B. (1992). Morphological deficits of children with SLI: Evaluation of number marking and agreement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 12491257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., Tomblin, B., Hoffman, L., Richman, A., & Marquis, J. (2004). Grammatical tense deficits in children with SLI and nonspecific language impairment: Relationships with nonverbal IQ over time. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 816834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 12391257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 14121431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., Marquis, J., & Hershberger, S. (2000). Acquisition of irregular past tense by children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 11261145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J., Rescorla, L., Giroux, J., & Stevens, L. (1998). Phonological skills of children with specific expressive language impairment (SLI-E). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 374384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, S. (2003). Auditory processing in dyslexia and specific language impairment: Is there a deficit? What is the nature? Does it explain anything? Journal of Phonetics, 31, 509527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samorodova, N. V. (2004). Fonemicheskije i Morfologicheskije Razlichija v Govorax Arkhangelskoj Oblasti (Phonological and morphological distinctions in the dialects of Archangelsk region). Doctoral dissertation, Pomorskij State University, Archangelsk, Russia.Google Scholar
Schuele, C. M., & Nicholls, L. (2000). Relative clauses: Evidence of continued linguistic vulnerability in children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 14, 563585.Google Scholar
Skarakis-Doyle, E., & Mentis, M. (1991). A discourse approach to language disorders: Investigating complex sentence production. In Gallagher, T. (Ed.), Pragmatics of language: Clinical practice issues (pp. 307332). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Spitz, R. V., Tallal, P., Flax, J., & Benasich, A. A. (1997). Look who's talking: A prospective study of familial transmission of language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 9901001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stavrakaki, S. (2001). Comprehension of reversible relative clauses in specifically language impaired and normally developing Greek children. Brain and Language, 77, 419431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stromswold, K. (1998). Genetics of spoken language disorders. Human Biology, 70, 297324.Google ScholarPubMed
Stromswold, K. (2001). The heritability of language: A review and metaanalysis of twin, adoption and linkage studies. Language, 77, 647723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swisher, L., Plante, E., & Lowell, S. (1994). Nonlinguistic deficits of children with language disorders complicate the interpretation of their nonverbal IQ scores. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 235240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tallal, P., Hirsch, L. S., Realpe-Bonilla, T., Miller, S., Brzustowicz, L., & Flax, J. F. (2001). Familial aggregation in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 44, 11721182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature, 241, 468469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Stark, R., Kallman, C., & Mellits, D. (1981). A re-examination of some non-verbal perceptual abilities of language-impaired and normal children as a function of age and sensory modality. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 351357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The SLI Consortium. (2002). A genomewide scan identifies two novel loci involved in specific language impairment. American Journal of Human Genetics, 70, 384398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The SLI Consortium. (2004). Highly significant linkage to the SLI1 locus in an expanded sample of individuals affected by specific language impairment. American Journal of Human Genetics, 74, 12251238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Freese, P., & Records, N. (1992). Diagnosing specific language impairment in adults for the purpose of pedigree analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 35, 832843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X., Smith, E., & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 12451260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M., & Pierpont, E. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (1996). Specifically language impaired and normally developing children: Verbal passive vs. adjectival passive sentence interpretation. Lingua, 98, 243272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insights from grammatical-SLI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 5359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Battell, J. (2003). Wh-movement in children with grammatical SLI: A test of the RDDR Hypothesis. Language, 79, 153181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J., & Howard, D. (1993). Specifically language impaired children: Linguistic impairment or short term memory deficit? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 11931207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lely, H. K. J., Rosen, S., & McClelland, A. (1998). Evidence for a grammar-specific deficit in children. Current Biology, 8, 12531258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vernes, S. C., Newbury, D. F., Brett, S., Abrahams, L. W., Nicod, J., Groszer, M., et al. (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359, 23372345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wright, B. A., Bowen, R. W., & Zecker, S. G. (2000). Nonlinguistic perceptual deficits associated with reading and language disorders. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 482486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed