Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T08:43:13.740Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Young children's knowledge about the links between writing and language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2017

REBECCA TREIMAN*
Affiliation:
Washington University in St. Louis
KELLY BOLAND
Affiliation:
Washington University in St. Louis
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Rebecca Treiman, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Campus Box 1125, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130. E-mail: rtreiman@wustl.edu

Abstract

The present study tested the hypothesis (Byrne, 1996) that young children who do not yet understand that the elements of alphabetic writing represent phonemes link writing to language at the level of morphemes. We asked US preschoolers to write words that varied in the number of morphemes and the number of syllables that they contained. We identified a group of 50 children who used letters that represented phonemes in the intended words no more often than expected by chance (mean age = 4 years, 9 months). These prephonological spellers did not produce longer spellings for two-morpheme words such as teacup than for one-morpheme words such as napkin, although the length of their spellings was affected by the number of letters that they used to spell the previously presented word and by the order of the word in the experiment. The results suggest that the length of prephonological spellers’ productions is not influenced by the linguistic length of a message in phonemes, syllables, or morphemes, and they do not support the idea that these children show a special sensitivity to morphemes.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. J., & Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowles, R. P., Pentimonti, J. M., Gerde, H. K., & Montroy, J. J. (2013). Item response analysis of uppercase and lowercase letter name knowledge. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 32, 146156. doi:10.1177/0734282913490266 Google Scholar
Byrne, B. (1996). The learnability of the alphabetic principle: Children's initial hypotheses about how print represents spoken language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 401426. doi:10.1017/S0142716400008171 Google Scholar
Cardoso-Martins, C., Corrêa, M. F., Lemos, S., & Napoleão, R. F. (2006). Is there a syllabic stage in spelling development? Evidence from Portuguese-speaking children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 628641. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.628 Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E. (1978). What is written in a written sentence? A developmental answer. Journal of Education, 160, 2539.Google Scholar
Ferreiro, E., & Teberosky, A. (1982). Literacy before schooling. New York: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Gerde, H. K., Bingham, G. E., & Wasik, B. A. (2012). Writing in early childhood classrooms: Guidance for best practices. Early Childhood Education Journal, 40, 351359. doi:10.1007/s10643-012-0531-z Google Scholar
Kato, Y., Ueda, A., Ozaki, K., & Mukaigawa, Y. (1999). Japanese preschoolers’ theories about the “hiragana” system of writing. Linguistics and Education, 10, 219232. doi:10.1016/S0898-5898(99)80109-0 Google Scholar
Kessler, B. (2009). Ponto [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://spell.psychology.wustl.edu/ponto Google Scholar
Kontos, S. (1988). Development and interrelationships of reading knowledge and skills during kindergarten and first grade. Reading Research and Instruction, 27, 1328. doi:10.1080/19388078809557931 Google Scholar
Levin, I., & Korat, O. (1993). Sensitivity to phonological, morphological, and semantic cues in early reading and writing in Hebrew. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 213232.Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M. (1999). The reading researcher and the reading teacher need the right theory of speech. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 95111. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0302_1 Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., Anthony, J. L., & Barker, T. A. (1998). Development of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 294311. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.294 Google Scholar
Lundberg, I., & Tornéus, M. (1978). Nonreaders’ awareness of the basic relationship between spoken and written words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 25, 404412. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(78)90064-4 Google Scholar
Manning, M., Manning, G., Long, R., & Kamii, C. (1993). Preschoolers’ conjectures about segments of a written sentence. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 8, 511. doi:10.1080/02568549309594850 Google Scholar
Mortensen, J. A., & Burnham, M. M. (2012). Preschool children's understanding of the graphic features of writing. Child Studies in Diverse Contexts, 2, 4560. doi:10.5723/csdc.2012.2. 1.045 Google Scholar
Phillips, B. M., Piasta, S. B., Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., & Francis, D. J. (2012). IRTs of the ABCs: Children's letter name acquisition. Journal of School Psychology, 50, 461481. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2012.05.002 Google Scholar
Piasta, S. B., Petscher, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2012). How many letters should preschoolers in public programs know? The diagnostic efficiency of various preschool letter-naming benchmarks for predicting first-grade literacy achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104, 945958. doi:10.1037/a0027757 Google Scholar
Pollo, T. C., Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2009). Statistical patterns in children's early writing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 410426. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.07.003 Google Scholar
Puranik, C. S., & Lonigan, C. J. (2011). From scribbles to scrabble: Preschool children's developing knowledge of written language. Reading and Writing, 24, 567589. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9220-8 Google Scholar
Puranik, C. S., Petscher, Y., & Lonigan, C. J. (2014). Learning to write letters: Examination of student and letter factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 152170. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.07.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Bressman, B., & Taft, M. (1974). Do children understand the basic relationship between speech and writing? The mow-motorcycle test. Journal of Literacy Research, 6, 327334. doi:10.1080/10862967409547105 Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Decker, K., Kessler, B., & Pollo, T. C. (2015). Variation and repetition in the spelling of young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 99110. doi:10.1016/j.jecp. 2014.12.008 Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Hompluem, L., Gordon, J., Decker, K., & Markson, L. (2016). Young children's knowledge of the symbolic nature of writing. Child Development, 87, 583592. doi:10.1111/cdev.12478 Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2004). The case of case: Children's knowledge and use of upper- and lowercase letters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 413428. doi:10.1017/S0142716404001195 Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Kessler, B., Decker, K., & Pollo, T. C. (2016). How do prephonological writers link written words to their objects? Cognitive Development, 38, 8998. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.02.002 Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Pollo, T. C., Cardoso-Martins, C., & Kessler, B. (2013). Do young children spell words syllabically? Evidence from learners of Brazilian Portuguese. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 873890. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.002 Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Yin, L. (2011). Early differentiation between drawing and writing in Chinese children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 786801. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.013 Google Scholar
Zhang, L., & Treiman, R. (2015). Writing dinosaur large and mosquito small: Prephonological spellers’ use of semantic information. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19, 434445. doi:10.1080/10888438.2015.1072820 Google Scholar