Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T00:24:50.162Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Detecting Different Types of Reading Difficulties: A Comparison of Tests

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 November 2012

Danielle M. Moore
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
Melanie A. Porter*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
Saskia Kohnen
Affiliation:
ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
Anne Castles
Affiliation:
ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
*
Correspondence: Melanie Porter, Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, NSW 2109, Australia. E-mail: melanie.porter@mq.edu.au

Abstract

The focus of this paper is on the assessment of the two main processes that children must acquire at the single word reading level: word recognition (lexical) and decoding (nonlexical) skills. Guided by the framework of the dual route model, this study aimed to (1) investigate the impact of item characteristics on test performance, and (2) determine to what extent widely used reading measures vary in their detection of lexical and nonlexical reading difficulties. Thirty children with reading difficulties were administered selected reading subtests from the Woodcock-Johnson III, the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test – Second Edition, the Castles and Coltheart Reading Test 2 (CC2), as well as a measure of nonverbal IQ. Both within-subjects analyses and descriptive data are presented. Results suggest that in comparison to a pure measure of irregular word reading, children with reading difficulties perform better on word identification subtests containing both regular and irregular word items. Furthermore, certain characteristics (e.g., length, similarity to real words) appear to influence the level of difficulty of nonword items and tests. The CC2 subscales identified the largest proportions of children with reading difficulties. Differences between all test scores were of statistical and clinical significance. Clinical and theoretical implications are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority (ACARA). (2008). National Assessment Program Literacy and Numeracy: Achievement in reading, writing, language conventions and numeracy. Retrieved June 15, 2011, from http://www.nap.edu.au/_Documents/National%20Report/2ndStageNationalReport_18Dec_v2.pdfGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Baayen, R.H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Balota, D.A., Yap, M.J., Cortese, M.J., Hutchinson, K.A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., . . . Treiman, R. (2007a). The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445459. doi:10.3758/BF03193014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balota, D.A., Yap, M.J., Cortese, M.J., Hutchinson, K.A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., . . . Treiman, R. (2007b). The English Lexicon Project web site. Washington University. Retrieved from http://elexicon.wustl.eduGoogle Scholar
Berninger, V.W., Nielsen, K.H., Abbott, R.D., Wijsman, E., & Raskind, W. (2008). Gender differences in severity of writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 151172. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broom, Y.M., & Doctor, E.A. (1995a). Developmental phonological dyslexia: A case study of the efficacy of a remediation programme. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 725766. doi:10.1080/02643299508251400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, Y.M., & Doctor, E.A. (1995b). Developmental surface dyslexia: A case study of the efficacy of a remediation programme. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 69110. doi:10.1080/02643299508251992CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunsdon, R.K., Hannan, T.J., Coltheart, M., & Nickels, L. (2002). Treatment of lexical processing in mixed dyslexia: A case study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 385418. doi:10.1080/09602010244000174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunsdon, R.K., Hannan, T.J., Nickels, L., & Coltheart, M. (2002). Successful treatment of sublexical reading deficits in a child with dyslexia of the mixed type. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12, 199229. doi:10.1080/09602010244000048CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A. (2006). The dual route model and the developmental dyslexias. London Review of Education, 4, 4961. doi:10.1080/13603110600574454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., Bates, T.C., & Coltheart, M. (2006). John Marshall and the developmental dyslexias. Aphasiology, 20, 871892. doi:10.1080/02687030600738952CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149180. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90003-ECrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castles, A., Coltheart, M., Larsen, L., Jones, P., Saunders, S., & McArthur, G. (2009). Assessing the basic components of reading: A revision of the Castles and Coltheart test with new norms. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 14, 6788. doi:10.1080/19404150902783435CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colenbrander, D., Nickels, L., & Kohnen, S. (2011). Nonword reading tests: A review of the available resources. Australasian Journal of Special Education, 35, 137172. doi:10.1375/ajse.35.2.137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1987). Functional architecture of the language-processing system. In Coltheart, M., Sartori, G., & Job, R. (Eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language (pp. 126). London, England: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2005). Analysing developmental disorders of reading. Australian Journal of Learning Disabilities, 10 (3/4), 5362. doi:10.1080/19404150509546799CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2006). The genetics of learning to read. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 124132. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00296.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100, 589608. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., & Leahy, J. (1996). Assessment of lexical and nonlexical reading abilities in children: Some normative data. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, 136140. doi:10.1080/00049539608259520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, V., & Leahy, J. (1996). Procedures used by beginning and skilled readers to read unfamiliar letter strings. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, 124129. doi:10.1080/00049539608259518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204256. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.108.1.204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehri, L.C. (1999). Phases of development in learning to read words. In Oakhill, J. & Beard, R. (Eds.), Reading development and the teaching of reading (pp. 79108). Oxford, England: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Farrington-Flint, L., Coyne, E., Stiller, J., & Heath, E. (2008). Variability in children's early reading strategies. Educational Psychology, 28, 643661. doi:10.1080/01443410802140958CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felton, R.H., & Wood, F.B. (1992). A reading level match study of nonword reading skills in poor readers with varying IQ. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 318326. doi:10.1177/002221949202500506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K.E., Marshall, J.C., & Coltheart, M. (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301330). London, England: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goulandris, N.K., & Snowling, M. (1991). Visual memory deficits: A plausible cause of developmental dyslexia? Evidence from a single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 127154. doi:10.1080/02643299108253369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U., Ziegler, J.C., Dalton, L., & Schneider, W. (2003). Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235247. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanley, J.R., Hastie, K., & Kay, J. (1992). Developmental surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: An orthographic processing impairment. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 44, 285319. doi:10.1080/02724989243000046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harcourt Assessment. (2007). The Wechsler Individual Achievement Test – Second Edition – Australian Standardised Edition. Sydney, Australia: Author.Google Scholar
Holligan, C., & Johnston, R.S. (1988). The use of phonological information by good and poor readers in memory and reading tasks. Memory & Cognition, 16, 522532. doi:10.3758/BF03197053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, D., & Best, W. (1996). Developmental phonological dyslexia: Real word reading can be completely normal. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 887934. doi:10.1080/026432996381854CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, N.E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure: Toward an integrated cognitive psychology of atypical reading. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Jones, K., Castles, A., & Kohnen, S. (2011). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia: Recent developments and directions for treatment. Acquiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing, 13 (2), 7983.Google Scholar
Manis, F.R., Custodio, R., & Szeszulski, P.A. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 6486. doi:10.1006/jecp.1993.1026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S., Doi, L.M., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157195. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00679-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterson, J., Stuart, M., Dixon, M., Lovejoy, D., & Lovejoy, S. (2002). Children's printed word database (Version 1.3). Colchester, UK: University of Essex. Retrieved from http://www.essex.ac.uk/psychology/cpwdGoogle Scholar
Morton, J., & Patterson, K. (1980). A new attempt at an interpretation, or, an attempt at a new interpretation. In Coltheart, M., Patterson, K., & Marshall, J.C. (Eds.), Deep dyslexia (pp. 91118). London, England: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Olson, R., Forsberg, H., Wise, B., & Rack, J. (1994). Measurement of word recogntion, orthographic, and phonological skills. In Lyon, G.R. (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 229242). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Plaut, D.C., McClelland, J.L., Seidenberg, M.S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rack, J.P., Snowling, M.J., & Olson, R.K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A Review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 2853. doi:10.2307/747832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rathvon, N. (2004). Early reading assessment: A practitioner's handbook. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rowse, H.J., & Wilshire, C.E. (2007). Comparison of phonological and whole-word treatments for two contrasting cases of developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 817842. doi:10.1080/02643290701764207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schrank, F.A., & Woodcock, R.W. (2009). Woodcock-Johnson III Normative Update: Compuscore and Profiles Program, Australian Adaptation: Itasca, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar
Siegel, L.S. (1992). An evaluation of the discrepancy definition of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 618629. doi:10.1177/002221949202501001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, L.S., & Himel, N. (1998). Socioeconomic status, age and the classification of dyslexics and poor readers: The dangers of using IQ scores in the definition of reading disability. Dyslexia, 4, 90104. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0909(199806)4:2<90::AID-DYS105>3.0.CO;2-33.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R.S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children's thinking. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M.J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychological Research, 43, 219234. doi:10.1007/BF00309831CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M.J., Goulandris, N., & Defty, N. (1996). A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 653669. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.88.4.653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M.J., & Hulme, C. (1989). A longitudinal case study of developmental phonological dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 379401. doi:10.1080/02643298908253289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, K. (2010). Predicting children's word-reading accuracy for common English words: The effect of word transparency and complexity. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 519543. doi:10.1348/000712609X470752CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanaka, H., Black, J.M., Hulme, C., Stanley, L.M., Kesler, S.R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., . . . Hoeft, F. (2011). The brain basis of the phonological deficit in dyslexia is independent of IQ. Psychological Science, 22, 14421451. doi:10.1177/0956797611419521CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treiman, R., Goswami, U., & Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implications for reading development and theory. Memory & Cognition, 18, 559567. doi:10.3758/BF03197098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M., & Lyon, G.R. (2000). Differentiating between difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers: More evidence against the IQ-achievement discrepancy definition of reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 223238. doi:10.1177/002221940003300302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth Edition – Australian Standardised Edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wheldall, K., & Limbrick, L. (2010). Do more boys than girls have reading problems? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 418429. doi:10.1177/0022219409355477CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R.W., McGrew, K.S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar