Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T16:51:24.090Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adolescents’ neural reactivity to acute psychosocial stress: dysfunctional regulation habits are linked to temporal gyrus response

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2021

Sabrina Golde*
Affiliation:
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
Tobias Gleich
Affiliation:
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany
Lydia Romund
Affiliation:
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany
Anna Stippl
Affiliation:
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany
Patricia Pelz
Affiliation:
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany
Diana Raufelder
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Science, University Greifswald, Greifswald, Germany
Robert C. Lorenz
Affiliation:
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany
Anne Beck
Affiliation:
Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Campus Charité Mitte, Berlin, Germany Health and Medical University Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany
*
Author for Correspondence: Sabrina Golde, Freie Universität Berlin, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany; Email: sabrina.golde@fu-berlin.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Mid-adolescence is a critical time for the development of stress-related disorders and it is associated with significant social vulnerability. However, little is known about normative neural processes accompanying psychosocial stress at this time. Previous research found that emotion regulation strategies critically influence the relationship between stress and the development of psychiatric symptoms during adolescence. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we examined neural responses to acute stress and analyzed whether the tendency to use adaptive or maladaptive emotion regulation strategies is related to neural and autonomic stress responses. Results show large linear activation increases from low to medium to high stress levels mainly in medial prefrontal, insulae and temporal areas. Caudate and subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, neural areas related to reward and affective valuations, showed linearly decreasing activation. In line with our hypothesis, the current adolescent neural stress profile resembled social rejection and was characterized by pronounced activation in insula, angular and temporal cortices. Moreover, results point to an intriguing role of the anterior temporal gyrus. Stress-related activity in the anterior temporal gyrus was positively related to maladaptive regulation strategies and stress-induced autonomic activity. Maladaptive coping might increase the social threat and reappraisal load of a stressor, relating to higher stress sensitivity of anterior temporal cortices.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In adolescence, onsets of stress-associated disorders such as depression or substance use disorders peak (Kessler & Wang, Reference Kessler and Wang2008; Paus, Keshavan, & Giedd, Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008). In this context, restructuring of important functional brain networks such as the prefrontal–amygdala circuitry (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Jones, Levita, Libby, Pattwell, Ruberry and Somerville2010; Tottenham & Galván, Reference Tottenham and Galván2016), rapid gray matter changes (Giedd, Reference Giedd2004; Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd, Lusk, Hayashi, Greenstein, Vaituzis and Thompson2004) and imbalances in neuroendocrine axes and resulting hormonal output (Romeo, Reference Romeo2010; Romeo & McEwen, Reference Romeo and McEwen2007) are believed to create a window of neural stress vulnerability.

In addition, the transition into adulthood brings about significant environmental challenges. Adolescents begin exploring new social groups and networks outside their familiar home (Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, Reference Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure and Pine2005). At school, they are confronted with increasing academic pressure (Kouzma & Kennedy, Reference Kouzma and Kennedy2004). Simultaneously, high emotional reactivity (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Jones, Levita, Libby, Pattwell, Ruberry and Somerville2010; Dahl & Gunnar, Reference Dahl and Gunnar2009) and hypersensitivity to social acceptance and rejection (Platt, Kadosh, & Lau, Reference Platt, Kadosh and Lau2013; Sebastian, Viding, Williams, & Blakemore, Reference Sebastian, Viding, Williams and Blakemore2010) further raise the potency of (psychosocial) stressors on the adolescent nervous system.

Consequently, the adolescent brain is becoming strikingly sensitive to stress-induced alterations (Gee & Casey, Reference Gee and Casey2015). Acute stress negatively affects working memory and performance (Gärtner, Rohde-Liebenau, Grimm, & Bajbouj, Reference Gärtner, Rohde-Liebenau, Grimm and Bajbouj2014). In the long run, severe or chronic stress-related alterations during adolescence show close links to the emergence of depressive and anxiety disorders in adulthood (Dahl & Gunnar, Reference Dahl and Gunnar2009). As such, stress-related processes in the adolescent brain are of pivotal relevance for an individual's mental health. In general, the literature on stress-related functional brain changes as investigated in adults suggests neural resources shift towards areas involved in salience detection (e.g., anterior insula) and affective processing (e.g., medial prefrontal cortex [mPFC]), while modulating neural top-down regulation (e.g., ventrolateral PFC) in a specific and time-dependent manner (for a review see van Oort et al., Reference van Oort, Tendolkar, Hermans, Mulders, Beckmann, Schene and van Eijndhoven2017). Stress, moreover, changes activation and processing of stimuli in reward-related areas such as the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC; Porcelli, Lewis, & Delgado, Reference Porcelli, Lewis and Delgado2012). For example, Porcelli et al. (Reference Porcelli, Lewis and Delgado2012) exposed participants to acute stress prior to examining neural responses to monetary rewards and punishments. They could show that participants exposed to acute stress demonstrated decreased dorsal striatum and OFC reward sensitivity. Behaviorally, acute stress appears to increase attention towards threat-related cues and heighten the salience of negative emotional material (Hermans, Henckens, Joëls, & Fernández, Reference Hermans, Henckens, Joëls and Fernández2014; Lupien, Maheu, Tu, Fiocco, & Schramek, Reference Lupien, Maheu, Tu, Fiocco and Schramek2007; Oei et al., Reference Oei, Veer, Wolf, Spinhoven, Rombouts and Elzinga2012). However, neural correlates of acute stress vary with the type of stress induced, as well as with the paradigm employed (for a review, see Noack, Nolte, Nieratschker, Habel, & Derntl, Reference Noack, Nolte, Nieratschker, Habel and Derntl2019). Reviews summarizing functional brain changes associated with acute psychosocial stress induced by the Montreal imaging stress task (MIST; Dedovic, D'Aguiar, & Pruessner, Reference Dedovic, D'Aguiar and Pruessner2009; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Dedovic, Pruessner, Lord, Buss, Collins and Lupien2010) reported increased activation in the left medial PFC, cingulum, occipital cortex and left premotor area and deactivation of the limbic system, particularly in the medio-orbitofrontal cortex, hippocampus, and hypothalamus. Meta-analytic data moreover show that psychosocial stress in adults is associated with heightened activation in the right superior temporal gyrus as well as the inferior frontal gyrus extending into insula across tasks. Convergent decreased activation was observed in the caudate (Kogler et al., Reference Kogler, Müller, Chang, Eickhoff, Fox, Gur and Derntl2015).

In adolescence, these neural systems are undergoing dynamic changes. Therefore, the stress response might be unique (Romeo, Reference Romeo2010). Highly plastic and developing brain systems are more sensitive to adverse and challenging environmental stimuli (Eiland & Romeo, Reference Eiland and Romeo2013; Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). For instance, cortical volume increases in frontal and temporal areas are observed from childhood to puberty, followed by cortical thinning during adolescence (Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd, Lusk, Hayashi, Greenstein, Vaituzis and Thompson2004). Importantly, current models of adolescent neural development propose that descending projections from the prefrontal (particularly ventromedial prefrontal cortex [vmPFC]) to limbic subcortical areas (e.g., amygdala) are maturing at this time. A shift from positive to negative cortico–subcortical coupling is still being completed during adolescence (Casey, Heller, Gee, & Cohen, Reference Casey, Heller, Gee and Cohen2019), which is likely to impact reactivity to and regulation of psychosocial stress during adolescence.

As studies on stress in adolescence are rare, previous investigations of social rejection as a stressor in adolescence might be informative regarding relevant neural mechanisms. Here, models assume that, on the one hand, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, fusiform gyrus, superior temporal sulcus, and anterior temporal gyrus detect social threat, while, on the other, amygdala, mPFC, and subgenual ACC, as well as areas of the neural reward system (orbitofrontal cortex, striatum), are involved in affective responses (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2008; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure and Pine2005; Sebastian et al., Reference Sebastian, Viding, Williams and Blakemore2010; Somerville, Reference Somerville2013; Somerville, Whalen, & Kelley, Reference Somerville, Whalen and Kelley2010). More specifically, during socially evaluative and stressful situations, dorsal ACC and insula are directly involved in the detection of negative social stimuli and generation of negative affect in adolescents and adults (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Eisenberger, Borofsky, Pfeifer, McNealy, Mazziotta and Dapretto2009; Slavich, Way, Eisenberger, & Taylor, Reference Slavich, Way, Eisenberger and Taylor2010). For lower level processing, interpretation and reappraisal of socio-emotional stimuli, superior temporal cortex and fusiform gyrus play important roles (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2008; Pfeifer & Blakemore, Reference Pfeifer and Blakemore2012). Affective reactivity and processing in these challenging social situations have been linked with subgenual ACC and ventromedial PFC activation. Reactivity in these areas is, moreover, believed to be higher during adolescence compared to childhood and adulthood (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2008; Gunther Moor, van Leijenhorst, Rombouts, Crone, & Van der Molen, Reference Gunther Moor, van Leijenhorst, Rombouts, Crone and Van der Molen2010; Somerville, Reference Somerville2013). While research in adults suggests significant involvement of the lateral PFC including the inferior frontal cortex in the response to acute stress (Kogler et al., Reference Kogler, Müller, Chang, Eickhoff, Fox, Gur and Derntl2015), there are indications that these regulatory brain areas might not be recruited in the adolescent stress response (Sebastian et al., Reference Sebastian, Tan, Roiser, Viding, Dumontheil and Blakemore2011).

Furthermore, acute psychosocial stress is negatively linked to activation in orbitofrontal and striatal regions in adults (Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, D'Aguiar and Pruessner2009; Kogler et al., Reference Kogler, Müller, Chang, Eickhoff, Fox, Gur and Derntl2015; Porcelli et al., Reference Porcelli, Lewis and Delgado2012), and similar interactions might be relevant for adolescents (Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Pisoni, Bondy, Kumar, Singleton, Hajcak and Auerbach2019). The striatum is a core area of the mesocorticolimbic reward circuit (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Bell, Flores, Gulley, Willing and Paul2017) and has been found to moderate reward sensitivity (Van Leijenhorst et al., Reference Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, Van Meel, Westenberg, Rombouts and Crone2010) and reward-related learning (Delgado, Reference Delgado2007). The orbitofrontal cortex is believed to be involved in reward and hedonic experience (Kringelbach, Reference Kringelbach2005).

Taken together, despite the critical role of psychosocial stress during adolescence for mental health, the amount of studies on the effects of acute stress on the adolescent brain is limited. Previous neuroimaging research on social rejection has pointed to effects of rejection stress on widespread neural areas associated with socio-emotional processing during adolescence. In some areas, such as subgenual ACC and vmPFC, neural reactivity has been suggested to be particularly high, whereas in others, such as inferior frontal gyrus, reactivity might tend to be lower. In the current study, we therefore aimed to advance this existing knowledge by employing an effective parametric design that systematically varied stress level to examine neural correlates linearly associated with acute psychosocial stress in the adolescent brain.

Furthermore, despite immanent instabilities and vulnerabilities during adolescence, it is also a time of intense learning. For example, a second wave of synaptic pruning (after infancy) is believed to underlie rapid gray matter changes during adolescence (Selemon, Reference Selemon2013). Moreover, whereas in children effective prefrontal regulation of amygdala reactivity seems to depend on maternal presence, adolescents’ effective neural emotion regulation in the prefrontal–amygdala circuit has been found to be more independent (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Gabard-Durnam, Telzer, Humphreys, Goff, Shapiro and Tottenham2014). With increasing developmental age, PFC-related amygdala modulation and inter-connections within the PFC increasingly facilitate cognitive reappraisal of emotion (Silvers et al., Reference Silvers, Insel, Powers, Franz, Helion, Martin and Ochsner2017). Similarly, from a cognitive perspective, new abilities allow the use of complex strategies aimed at regulating emotions (LeBlanc, Essay, & Ollendick, Reference LeBlanc, Essay, Ollendick, Essay, LeBlanc and Ollendick2017), which modulate physiological stress responses (Krkovic, Clamor, & Lincoln, Reference Krkovic, Clamor and Lincoln2018; Nasso, Vanderhasselt, Demeyer, & De Raedt, Reference Nasso, Vanderhasselt, Demeyer and De Raedt2018). These emotion regulation strategies are often defined as “processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions, especially their intensive and temporal features, to accomplish one's goals” (Thompson, Reference Thompson1994). Different theoretical models have conceptualized functional strategies as adaptive (e.g., re-evaluation) and dysfunctional as maladaptive (e.g., rumination), dating back to literature on stress coping (e.g., Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, Reference Carver, Scheier and Weintraub1989) and early cognitive behavioral therapists (e.g., Beck, Reference Beck1979). For developmental and clinical researchers alike, a central criterion for categorizing strategies as adaptive or maladaptive has been their association with health outcome, that is, psychological well-being versus the development of psychopathology such as depression or anxiety disorders (Saarni, Campos, Camras, & Witherington, Reference Saarni, Campos, Camras and Witherington2006; Schäfer, Naumann, Holmes, Tuschen-Caffier, & Samson, Reference Schäfer, Naumann, Holmes, Tuschen-Caffier and Samson2017; Zeman, Klimes-Dougan, Cassano, & Adrian, Reference Zeman, Klimes-Dougan, Cassano and Adrian2007). Usage of adaptive versus maladaptive emotion regulation strategies has been found to be the critical factor for the relationship between stress experience and the development of psychiatric symptoms during adolescence (Kullik & Petermann, Reference Kullik and Petermann2013; Nolen-Hoeksema & Aldao, Reference Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao2011; Shapero, Stange, McArthur, Abramson, & Alloy, Reference Shapero, Stange, McArthur, Abramson and Alloy2019; Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, Reference Silk, Steinberg and Morris2003; Stevens et al., Reference Stevens, Gerhart, Goldsmith, Heath, Chesney and Hobfoll2013). In addition to systematically investigating linear neural effects of acute stress by employing an effective parametric design, it was, therefore, another important aim to analyze associations between neural stress reactivity and trait level emotion regulation tendencies.

To address these two research aims as described, we administered an adapted version of the MIST (Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, Renwick, Mahani, Engert, Lupien and Pruessner2005; Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg, & Ströhle, Reference Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg and Ströhle2015) to a sample of mid-adolescent individuals during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Owing to the heightened salience of social stimuli at this developmental stage (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure and Pine2005), we expected that the neural profile of psychosocial stress would be similar to the one found for social rejection during adolescence. Therefore, we hypothesized significantly increased neural reactivity in the medial prefrontal and dorsal ACC, insula, fusiform gyrus, superior temporal sulcus and anterior temporal gyrus under stress. Moreover, based on previous studies using the MIST in adults (Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, D'Aguiar and Pruessner2009; van Oort et al., Reference van Oort, Tendolkar, Hermans, Mulders, Beckmann, Schene and van Eijndhoven2017) and studies investigating the effects of acute stress on reward processing (Porcelli et al., Reference Porcelli, Lewis and Delgado2012; Van Leijenhorst et al., Reference Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, Van Meel, Westenberg, Rombouts and Crone2010), we expected neural activity in limbic and reward-related areas, particularly in the hippocampus, hypothalamus, orbitofrontal cortex, and striatum, to decrease. Although previous research is rare, we further speculated that use of maladaptive strategies might be linked to more stress-associated activation in limbic areas and adaptive strategies to attenuated neural stress response.

Method

Participants and procedure

The study was approved by the ethics committee of the German Psychological Society; all participants as well as legal guardians provided written informed consent. Fifty-three adolescents from 14 to 17 years took part in the experiment (M = 15.70 ± 0.61, 24 girls).

Participants were screened for the following exclusion criteria via standardized (in-house) questionnaires: (a) adverse health conditions, (b) neurological or psychological disorders, (c) use of medication that influences central nervous system, (d) nonremovable ferromagnetic material. Before the fMRI experiment, habitual use of emotion regulation strategies was measured by self-report (see section on emotion regulation strategies below). Afterwards, participants performed a psychosocial stress task (adapted MIST) in the MRI scanner. Pulse oximetry was used to record heart rate (HR) over the course of the experiment (see section on subjective and autonomic stress response measures for a detailed description). Ten participants were excluded from the fMRI sample due to excessive head movement (>3° mm translation or 3° rotation) and two due to technical problems during fMRI recordings. Thus, fMRI-based analyses relate to a sample of N = 41 adolescents (M = 15.63 ± 0.59, 22 girls); 1 left-handed and 40 right-handed (according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory) (Oldfield, Reference Oldfield1971).

Emotion regulation strategies

We selected the German FEEL-KJ questionnaire (Fragebogen zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation bei Kindern und Jugendlichen; Grob & Smolenski, Reference Grob and Smolenski2005) to measure emotion regulation strategies. It assesses a wide range of emotion regulation strategies and was specifically developed for children and adolescents from 10 to 19 years of age. 90 items rated on a 5-point scale (ranging from 1 = never to 5 = always) index adaptive and maladaptive strategies in response to feelings of anxiety, sadness, and anger. Adaptive strategies consist of problem solving, distraction, forgetting, acceptance, humor enhancement, cognitive problem solving, and re-evaluation, whereas giving up, withdrawal, rumination, self-devaluation, and aggressive actions represent the maladaptive strategies. The mean values of the relative use of adaptive and maladaptive strategies are the two main outcome values. The questionnaire has been shown to be reliable and to reflect a valid self-report measure of emotion regulation strategies in adolescents (Cracco, Van Durme, & Braet, Reference Cracco, Van Durme and Braet2015). The scales also showed excellent internal consistency in the current study (adaptive strategies α = .92, maladaptive strategies α = .88).

Psychosocial stress task

The MIST (Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, Renwick, Mahani, Engert, Lupien and Pruessner2005; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Dedovic, Khalili-Mahani, Engert, Pruessner, Buss and Lupien2008) as modified by Zschucke et al. (Reference Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg and Ströhle2015) was employed. In summary, the aim of the MIST is to induce psychosocial stress during a mental arithmetic challenge (with difficulty and time limit being manipulated to increase individual failure rates) in combination with an artificial social evaluative threat. Each trial began with the depiction of the mathematical equation. The equation was presented either until the participant submitted a response by choosing an integer via button press or the time allotted was elapsed. Afterwards, the feedback screen was presented for 2.5 s until the next trial started. Each trial lasted from the depiction of the equation until the end of the feedback period.

In detail, the general interface of the modified MIST consisted of the following elements: a mental arithmetic question, a horizontal line of integers from 0 to 9 (used for response submission), a text field that provides feedback on the submitted response (“correct,” “incorrect” or “timeout”) and a time bar indicating how much time is left for the current trial. The task had three conditions: (a) low stress, (b) moderate stress, and (c) high stress, presented in a fixed sequence. In the low-stress condition (40 trials), participants were given sufficient time (20 s) to solve each arithmetic problem while being told that this part served for training only. These 40 trials were used to calculate participants’ individual baseline response time. During the moderate stress condition (32 trials), participants were told that responses were now being recorded and monitored by the experimenter. Participants were given 25% less time than their baseline reaction time to solve each question. Two performance indicators were also added to the interface. One indicator tracked the participants’ performance and the other one displayed a faked average of all previous study participants. This faked group average was programmed to be consistently above the current participant's performance. After the moderate stress condition, the experimenter delivered scripted negative feedback over the microphone. Participants were told to improve their performance for a last run, otherwise their data would not be usable. During the high-stress condition (32 trials), the algorithm further restricted the allotted time and increased task difficulty to raise failure rates to approximately 50%. See Zschucke et al. (Reference Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg and Ströhle2015) for a detailed description of the version of the MIST that was used.

Participants used the outer button (relative to participant's body) of a four-button fiber-optic response box (Current Design Inc., Philadelphia, USA) in their left hand to move the highlight to the left, and the right-hand button box to move the highlight to the right (outer button) and to submit the answer (inner button). After the task, participants were fully debriefed, told that the task was designed to be impossible to accomplish, and that it did not access their math abilities.

Subjective and autonomic stress response measures

Participants were asked (via microphone) immediately before and after the MIST to evaluate how (a) stressed and (b) strained they felt on a scale from 1 to 10. In addition, we used pulse oximetry to record HR to validate autonomic arousal over the course of the MIST. For this purpose, the integrated photoplethysmograph of the Siemens Physiological Monitoring Unit with an infrared emitter was placed under the pad of the left index finger. Processing of the pulse oximetry data was performed with the TAPAS PhysIO toolbox, version r671, implemented in SPM12 (Kasper et al., Reference Kasper, Bollmann, Diaconescu, Hutton, Heinzle, Iglesias and Stephan2017). The adaptive “auto-matched” peak detection algorithm of the PhysIO toolbox was used for peak detection. Diagnostic plots of the heartbeat interval time course (as provided by the toolbox) were used to check for missed and wrongly detected heart beats (due to movement or scanner pulses). All data sets with more than three wrongly detected heart beats (as indicated by the toolbox) were excluded from the analyses.

Statistical analysis of behavioral and autonomic stress measures

Two paired sample t tests were calculated to assess changes in subjective stress and strain. With regard to HR measurements, we used data derived from peak detection to calculate mean beats per minute (bpm) for all three conditions (low, moderate, and high stress). A repeated analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factor condition was computed. Furthermore, we calculated mean values for the usage of adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation. Given previous reports of gender- and age-related differences in emotion regulation (e.g., Zimmermann & Iwanski, Reference Zimmermann and Iwanski2014), we compared usage of emotion regulation strategies between genders in a mixed-effects ANOVA adding gender (girls, boys) as a group factor and emotion regulation (adaptive, maladaptive) as a between-subject factor. In addition (b), we calculated bivariate correlations between emotion regulation and age of the adolescents.

fMRI acquisition

The fMRI data were acquired in a 3 Tesla Magnetom Trio scanner system (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) with a 12-channel radiofrequency head coil. The experiment was programmed with Presentation software (Version 14.9, Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Albany, CA, USA) and projected onto a screen at the end of the magnet bore by a video projector (NEC GT950, NEC Corporation, Itasca, IL, USA, resolution 1,024 × 768 pixels), which participants viewed via a mirror on the head coil. A T1-weighted three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo sequence with 192 continuous sagittal slices (image matrix = 256 × 256, repetition time [TR] = 1,900 ms, echo time [TE] = 2.52 ms, flip angle = 9°, field of view [FoV] = 256 × 256, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3) was conducted. Functional images were collected using axial aligned T2*-weighted gradient echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence sensitive to blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (33 transversal slices, image matrix = 64 × 64, TR = 2,000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 78°, FoV = 192 × 192, voxel size = 3 × 3×3 mm3).

fMRI data processing and analysis

Preprocessing

The fMRI data were analyzed using SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping software package; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). EPIs were corrected for acquisition time delay and motion artifacts and transformed into the stereotactic normalized standard space of the Montreal Neuroimaging Institute (MNI) using the unified segmentation algorithm as implemented in SPM12. Finally, EPIs were resampled (voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3 mm3) and smoothed with a three-dimensional Gaussian kernel of 7 mm full-width-at-half-maximum.

Single subject analysis

The time series were high-pass filtered (1/256 Hz cut-off frequency). Aliasing effects and serial correlations were modelled using an autoregressive model of first order and the parameters of the model were estimated with a restricted maximum likelihood algorithm as implemented in SPM. A general linear model was implemented at the single-subject level, in accordance with the approach described in Zschucke et al. (Reference Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg and Ströhle2015). We modelled the different conditions convolved with a hemodynamic response function as explanatory variables on a voxel-by-voxel basis. Further, we used the constant of the general linear model (i.e., the mean signal within each run) as our main neural parameter representing stress-associated neural activity of each condition. Motor response, task and feedback screen, as well as the six movement parameters were additionally included as regressors of no interest. For flexible factorial modelling at the group level, we generated three different contrast images at the individual level, which represented the sustained mean signal of the (a) low-, (b) medium-, or (c) high-stress run, respectively. In addition, we generated two contrast images of parameter estimates that compared the high- and low-stress run at the individual level (i.e., [high > low], [low > high]). These were then used in a one-sample t test and voxel-wise regression analysis at the group level (see next section).

Group analysis

Individual contrast images were taken to fMRI group random effects analyses using the flexible factorial modelling procedure that included the within-subject factor stress level (low, moderate, high stress). For correction of multiple comparison, voxel-wise familywise error (FWE) correction with p < .05 at the peak level was applied for whole brain analyses. It is important to note that this conservative, voxel-wise approach was chosen due to reported issues around cluster-based thresholding (Eklund, Nichols, & Knutsson, Reference Eklund, Nichols and Knutsson2016) and because it appears to provide an appropriate balance between false positives and false negatives for social neuroscience studies (Han & Glenn, Reference Han and Glenn2017). In addition, we only report resulting clusters with a minimum size of k ≥ 5 voxels. We then conducted a whole brain voxel-wise regression to analyze the relationship between neural stress reactivity and maladaptive emotion regulation (FEEL-KJ). In a follow-up analysis, we explored potential effects of gender or age differences. A two-sample t test with differential contrast images (low vs. high stress) from individual subjects contrasting both genders was computed. By means of a voxel-wise regression analysis with the same contrast images, we tested for associations between age- and stress-induced neural activation. Results were FWE-corrected with p < .05 at the peak level within the regions that showed a main effect of stress and only report clusters with a minimum cluster size of k ≥ 5 voxels.

For completeness, we examined the relationship between significant neural activation clusters in response to stress and subjective as well as autonomic stress response (HR) by extracting mean parameter estimates from these regions for exploratory, post-hoc analysis.

For the purpose of extracting individual beta values contrasting two conditions (low vs. high stress), a one-sample t test with differential contrast images (low vs. high) from individual subjects was computed. This more conservative approach offers several advantages in terms of error partitioning (Henson, Reference Henson2015; McFarquhar, Reference McFarquhar2019). From all resulting suprathreshold activation clusters (after whole brain voxel-wise FWE-correction with p < .05 at the peak level) that were 5 voxels or larger, mean parameter estimates for the whole cluster were extracted for each participant for correlation with subjective stress and HR values in SPSS (statistical package for the social sciences) software.

Results

Subjective and autonomic stress response validation, emotion regulation statistics

Self-reported stress and strain significantly increased after performance of the MIST task (stress: M pre = 2.96 ± 2.33, M post = 6.10 ± 2.59, t (49)stress = 9.17, p < .001; strain: M pre = 3.44 ± 2.19, M post 6.23 ± 2.28, t (46)strain = 8.46, p < .001).

Excessive hand movement corrupted the pulse oximetry data of 17 participants (see Supplementary Table 1 in the Supplementary Material for information of sex ratio and mean age in this subsample). The repeated measures ANOVA on the remaining 36 participants showed a main effect of condition (M low = 64.95 ± 11.68 bpm, M moderate = 70.35 ± 14.38 bpm, M high = 76.73 ± 18.50 bpm, F (2) = 17.45, p < .001). Bonferroni corrected post-hoc paired t tests confirmed significant increases in HR from baseline to moderate (t (35) = 2.97, p = .010 adjusted) and from moderate to high stress condition (t (37) = 3.70, p = .002 adjusted).

The mean value for usage of adaptive emotion regulation strategies, which was rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = never to 5 = always, was M adaptive = 3.26 ± 0.49 (variance = 0.19, range = 1.87), the mean value for maladaptive strategies M maladaptive = 2.68 ± 0.43 (variance = 0.20, range = 2.02). The mixed-effects ANOVA regarding emotion regulation showed no significant effect of gender on type of emotion regulation (non-significant main and interaction effects). Age of participants did not significantly correlate (Pearson) with emotion regulation (for both maladaptive and adaptive emotion regulation).

Whole brain stress-induced neural activation

We compared neural activation across the brain between the three MIST conditions (i.e., low, moderate, and high stress) in a flexible factorial design. Linearly increasing activation from low to moderate to high stress was observed in the mPFC/ACC, insula, inferior frontal gyrus, middle orbital gyrus, angular gyrus, anterior temporal gyrus/temporal pole, middle and superior occipital gyrus, and thalamus. Decreasing activation was found in the caudate gyrus and subgenual ACC (Figure 1). Details (laterality, MNI coordinates, and t values) for the comparison between the low- and high-stress condition can be found in Table 1. Please refer to Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 in the Supplementary Material for details on contrasts between moderate- and high- as well as low-stress conditions. One-sample t tests with differential contrast images from individual subjects (low vs. high) resulted in activation clusters within highly similar brain areas (see Supplementary Table 4 in the Supplementary Material).

Figure 1. (a) Depiction of significant activation clusters from the contrast high > low stress with flexible factorial analysis of variance (whole brain familywise error [FWE] corrected with p < .05 at the peak level, k ≥ 5 voxels). (b) Depiction of significant activation clusters from the contrast low > high stress with flexible factorial analysis of variance (same statistical threshold as in (a)). (c) Contrast estimates for peak voxels of the main activation clusters from the flexible factorial analysis of variance showing stress-associated activation increases (left) or decreases (right). Low = low stress condition; moderate = moderate stress condition; high = high stress condition; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; Ins (R) = right insular cortex; Ins (L) = left insular cortex; mid/inf temp = middle/inferior temporal gyrus; angular = angular gyrus; ant temp = anterior temporal gyrus; caud (R) = right caudate nucleus; caud (L) = left caudate nucleus.

Table 1. Comparison between high and low stress conditions with flexible factorial analysis of variance

Note. In bold type, peak voxel coordinates and t values for significant activation clusters at p < .05 familywise error (FWE) corrected across the whole brain, cluster size > 5 voxels; in non-bold type, peak voxel coordinates and t values of local maxima within significant activation clusters. ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex; H = hemisphere; L = left; R = right; MNI = Montreal Neurological Institute.

Voxel-wise regression analysis with differential contrast images from individual subjects (low vs. high) and mean scores of the tendency to use maladaptive emotion regulation showed a significant positive correlation between the tendency of using maladaptive emotion regulation strategies and right temporal pole activation (MNI peak 51/8/−29, t = 4.29, p < .035, k = 11). There was no significant association with adaptive emotion regulation strategies. Adding age as a covariate did not significantly change the results (MNI peak 51/8/−29, t = 3.80, p < .041, k = 11).

The two-sample t test comparing genders did not yield any significant results. Similarly, we did not observe any significant associations between age- and stress-induced neural activation (whole brain voxel-wise regression). When gender and age were added as covariates to the one-sample t test with differential contrast images (low vs. high stress), results were highly similar (see Supplementary Table 5 in the Supplementary Material).

Post-hoc correlations: Neural activation areas, HR and subjective response

Beta values were extracted from the one-sample t test (from all eight significant activation clusters) for exploratory post-hoc correlations with stress-related HR increases. We observed negative correlations of stress-induced right temporal gyrus activation [high > low stress] with stress-induced HR increase (MNI peak 60/−13/−23: Pearson r = −.502, p = .006; MNI peak 54/5/−29<: Pearson r = −.377, p = .048, Figure 2). There were no significant associations between subjective stress, stress-induced neural activity or between subjective stress and stress-induced HR activity.

Figure 2. (a) Voxel-wise regression analysis: significant positive correlation between stress-associated anterior temporal gyrus activation and the tendency to use maladaptive strategies to regulate negative emotions. Depicted are activation clusters after whole brain familywise error (FWE) correction with p < .05 at the peak level (brain) and extracted parameter estimated from 5 mm sphere around peak (scatter). (b) Significant positive Pearson correlation between stress-associated temporal gyrus activation and stress-induced increase in HR. +Please note that the correlation between HR and activation cluster in the temporal gyrus peaking at Montreal Neuroimaging Institute (MNI) 60/−13/−23 did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Mid/inf temp = middle/inferior temporal gyrus; ant temp = anterior temporal gyrus; HR = heart rate.

Discussion

Mid-adolescence is a critical period for the development of stress-associated disorders and characterized by vulnerability to social stressors. We therefore investigated neural response to acute psychosocial stress in a sample of mid-adolescent individuals. Largely in line with our hypothesized regions, results showed linear activation increases from low to medium to high psychosocial stress levels in the mPFC/ACC, insula, angular gyrus, inferior/anterior temporal gyrus, and middle occipital gyrus, among other regions. Caudate and subgenual ACC showed linearly decreasing activation. Furthermore, stress-related anterior temporal gyrus activation increase was positively linked to the use of maladaptive emotion regulation and negatively linked to autonomic arousal. In contrast to our expectation, adaptive strategies of emotion regulation were not significantly associated with brain responses to acute stress. Results support the possibility that gender might not significantly affect the response to psychosocial stress at this developmental stage.

Stress-associated pattern of linearly increasing activation

We found increasing activation in mPFC and occipital gyrus and decreasing activation in the reward system. This general activation pattern is in line with the findings that were reported by earlier studies that employed the MIST paradigm in adult participants (for reviews see Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, D'Aguiar and Pruessner2009; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Dedovic, Pruessner, Lord, Buss, Collins and Lupien2010). In line with our expectation, the observed neural circuitry of psychosocial stress in adolescents is similar to adolescents experiencing the distress of being socially rejected (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Eisenberger, Borofsky, Pfeifer, McNealy, Mazziotta and Dapretto2009; Sebastian et al., Reference Sebastian, Tan, Roiser, Viding, Dumontheil and Blakemore2011; Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Way, Eisenberger and Taylor2010). We assume that – at this developmental stage – situations of increased performance pressure that include negative social feedback may be processed similarly to situations of social rejection, leading to the same neural responses. Concordantly, a study by Slavich et al. (Reference Slavich, Way, Eisenberger and Taylor2010) highlights the overlap between biological systems for social threat and rejection. They found that in young adults, inflammatory response to a social stressor was linked to neural responses to social rejection in the ACC and insula. Moreover, adolescents show high vigilance to social evaluation and several studies report that social evaluative situations lead to greater self-consciousness (for a review see Somerville, Reference Somerville2013). In line with this idea, the insula is a region known to be involved in visceral and social pain perception, as well as in negative affect attributions (Orenius et al., Reference Orenius, Raij, Nuortimo, Näätänen, Lipsanen and Karlsson2017; Phillips, Drevets, Rauch, & Lane, Reference Phillips, Drevets, Rauch and Lane2003; Wiech et al., Reference Wiech, Lin, Brodersen, Bingel, Ploner and Tracey2010) and represents a key region affected by the experience of traumatic stress (Stark et al., Reference Stark, Parsons, Van Hartevelt, Charquero-Ballester, McManners, Ehlers and Kringelbach2015). Furthermore, mPFC is believed to serve the processing of affectively important, self-relevant information in adults and adolescents (Etkin, Egner, & Kalisch, Reference Etkin, Egner and Kalisch2011; Pfeifer & Blakemore, Reference Pfeifer and Blakemore2012; Romund et al., Reference Romund, Golde, Lorenz, Raufelder, Pelz, Gleich and Beck2017).

Stress-associated deactivation

Contrary to our hypothesis and earlier MIST studies in adults (Dedovic et al., Reference Dedovic, Renwick, Mahani, Engert, Lupien and Pruessner2005; Khalili-Mahani, Dedovic, Engert, Pruessner, & Pruessner, Reference Khalili-Mahani, Dedovic, Engert, Pruessner and Pruessner2010; Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Dedovic, Khalili-Mahani, Engert, Pruessner, Buss and Lupien2008; Soliman et al., Reference Soliman, O'Driscoll, Pruessner, Joober, Ditto, Streicker and Dagher2011), we did not find large limbic deactivation. In contrast, the adolescent stress pattern was largely characterized by pronounced neural activation rather than deactivation. However, there are also studies in adults using the MIST or similar designs that did not find the frequently reported limbic deactivation in adults (Lederbogen et al., Reference Lederbogen, Kirsch, Haddad, Streit, Tost, Schuch and Meyer-Lindenberg2011; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Rao, Wetmore, Furlan, Korczykowski, Dinges and Detre2005; Zschucke et al., Reference Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg and Ströhle2015), and results of several imaging studies show a mixed pattern of increasing and decreasing neural activation in limbic areas during psychosocial stress (e.g., Fan et al., Reference Fan, Pestke, Feeser, Aust, Pruessner, Böker and Grimm2015; Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Pestke, Feeser, Aust, Weigand, Wang and Bajbouj2014). However, the predominance of neural activation rather than deactivation and, in particular, the activation of anterior temporal cortices is intriguing and should be explored in future studies.

Of note, however, increasing intensity of stress was associated with a cluster of linearly decreasing activation in the caudate nucleus encompassing the subgenual ACC and reaching into the medial orbital gyrus. These areas are involved in different components of the reward-related neural circuitry in adolescence (Silverman, Jedd, & Luciana, Reference Silverman, Jedd and Luciana2015). This converges with earlier studies in adults, where reduced reward processing was observed under stress (Born et al., Reference Born, Lemmens, Rutters, Nieuwenhuizen, Formisano, Goebel and Westerterp-Plantenga2010; Porcelli et al., Reference Porcelli, Lewis and Delgado2012). Subgenual ACC moreover plays an important role in affective valuations, mood states, and depression (Hamani et al., Reference Hamani, Mayberg, Stone, Laxton, Haber and Lozano2011; Holtzheimer et al., Reference Holtzheimer, Husain, Lisanby, Taylor, Whitworth, McClintock and Mayberg2017; Laxton et al., Reference Laxton, Neimat, Davis, Womelsdorf, Hutchison, Dostrovsky and Lozano2013). Previous studies also point to its dysregulation in adolescent depression (Ho et al., Reference Ho, Yang, Wu, Cassey, Brown, Hoang and Yang2014, Reference Ho, Connolly, Henje Blom, LeWinn, Strigo, Paulus and Yang2015). The largest proportion and peak of this decreasing activation cluster was, however, located in the right caudate nucleus. This part of the dorsal striatum is mainly involved in reward-related learning (Delgado, Reference Delgado2007). Associations between a response, on the one hand, and reward versus punishment, on the other, have been found to be represented here (Burton, Nakamura, & Roesch, Reference Burton, Nakamura and Roesch2015; Spreckelmeyer et al., Reference Spreckelmeyer, Krach, Kohls, Rademacher, Irmak, Konrad and Gründer2009). Deactivation under psychosocial stress might thus lead to a decreased association between a response and (social) reward or punishment when an adolescent is under stress (Porcelli et al., Reference Porcelli, Lewis and Delgado2012). In other words, since the caudate is involved in coding reward prediction during goal-directed behavior (Delgado, Miller, Inati, & Phelps, Reference Delgado, Miller, Inati and Phelps2005; Haruno & Kawato, Reference Haruno and Kawato2006), the negative effect of acute stress on caudate activation might indicate a deterioration of reward expectation and reinforcement of goal-directed behavior under stress. This interpretation would be in line with prior studies reporting lower caudate response for reward anticipation in adolescents with stress-related disorders (i.e., major depression) (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Christopher May, Siegle, Ladouceur, Ryan, Carter and Dahl2006, Reference Forbes, Hariri, Martin, Silk, Moyles, Fisher and Dahl2009). More generally, acute stress has been shown to reduce reward responsiveness, which is a promising candidate mechanism linking stressful experience to depression (Bogdan & Pizzagalli, Reference Bogdan and Pizzagalli2006). Overall, these results concur with a body of previous adult and animal research suggesting that exposure to acute stress affects reward sensitivity (e.g., Cao et al., Reference Cao, Covington, Friedman, Wilkinson, Walsh, Cooper and Han2010; Ironside, Kumar, Kang, & Pizzagalli, Reference Ironside, Kumar, Kang and Pizzagalli2018).

Furthermore, in light of heightened social vulnerability in adolescence (Dahl, Reference Dahl2004; Dahl & Gunnar, Reference Dahl and Gunnar2009), it seems likely that this effect might be particularly pronounced for psychosocial stress compared to other kinds of stress. A meta-analysis by Kogler et al. (Reference Kogler, Müller, Chang, Eickhoff, Fox, Gur and Derntl2015), which directly compared the effects of psychosocial and physiological stress across tasks, lends support to this idea. The authors reported that this deactivation of the caudate is more strongly associated with acute psychosocial than physiological stress. In addition, a recent study on the effects of psychosocial stress on reward processing during adolescence similarly observed decreased activation in the dorsal striatum under acute stress (Lincoln et al., Reference Lincoln, Pisoni, Bondy, Kumar, Singleton, Hajcak and Auerbach2019).

The role of anterior temporal gyrus in the adolescent stress response and its link to emotion regulation

Our results indicate that the anterior temporal gyrus may play a significant role in the adolescent psychosocial stress response. Voxel-wise regression showed a positive association with maladaptive emotion regulation. In addition, its activation was negatively correlated with stress-related autonomic arousal (HR increase). Adolescents that report using maladaptive strategies such as self-devaluation, aggression or simply giving up (not responding) more frequently show higher activation of the anterior temporal gyrus during stress induction. This partly confirms our hypothesis by showing that maladaptive emotion regulation strategies are associated with more intense neural stress response. Contrary to our expectation, regions such as amygdala or mPFC that are often primarily linked with emotional responses were not linked to maladaptive strategies. There is, however, substantive evidence from research in adults suggesting a role of the anterior temporal gyrus, including the temporal pole, in socio-emotional processing and regulation (for a review see Olson, Plotzker, & Ezzyat, Reference Olson, Plotzker and Ezzyat2007). Moreover, a study by McRae et al. (Reference McRae, Gross, Weber, Robertson, Sokol-Hessner, Ray and Ochsner2012) who compared neural bases of emotion regulation across the life span found that, compared to children and adults, activation in the anterior temporal gyrus in adolescents more strongly serves the purpose of reappraising negative stimuli and emotions. It is possible that in those adolescents who tentatively revert to maladaptive strategies to regulate negative affect that arises during psychosocial stress induction, socio-emotional threat is more salient and possibly reappraisal facilitated by anterior temporal gyrus is more effortful, resulting in more intense activation increase. However, this interpretation is highly speculative and needs to be further explored in future studies. Moreover, we should note that in the current type of research, which records neural activity to an acute stressor, it remains fundamentally challenging to clearly disambiguate neural reactivity from regulation. There is no scientific consensus on how and whether these two can be usefully distinguished (here) at all. Prominent psychological perspectives see generation and regulation as closely intertwined in these kinds of responses, and not mechanistically distinguishable from one another. Some might argue that they can be differentiated by subtle aspects such as whether the individual has a sense of agency during the experience or at what point in time the activity occurs (e.g., at the very beginning of an emotional episode vs. later on) (Gross & Feldman Barrett, Reference Gross and Feldman Barrett2011), which can hardly be determined in this type of study design. It is thus difficult to ultimately conclude whether heightened anterior temporal gyrus activation (that was found to be correlated with maladaptive emotion regulation habits) reflects heightened regulatory activity (i.e., reappraisal) or heightened stress reactivity.

We moreover found a negative correlation between stress-associated anterior temporal gyrus activation and autonomic activation. In general, the anterior temporal gyrus has been linked to autonomic reactivity based on its anatomical connections to the hypothalamus. For example, temporal gyrus seizures, which consist of excessive and uncontrolled activity in the temporal gyrus, commonly impair autonomic cardiovascular regulation, particularly in response to stress (e.g., Allendorfer & Szaflarski, Reference Allendorfer and Szaflarski2014; Choudhary et al., Reference Choudhary, Deepak, Chandra, Bhatia, Sagar, Jaryal and Tripathi2017; Weil, Arnold, Eisensehr, & Noachtar, Reference Weil, Arnold, Eisensehr and Noachtar2005). Consistent with the current results, the anterior temporal gyrus is believed to integrate inputs from complex, emotionally evocative stimuli and to assist with regulating the resulting visceral emotional responses (Kondo, Saleem, & Price, Reference Kondo, Saleem and Price2003; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Plotzker and Ezzyat2007).

In contrast to our expectation, we did not observe any correlation between the habit of using adaptive emotion regulation strategies and neural stress responses. Adaptive strategies assessed here might not have a significant effect on negative emotionality that was created by the current MIST task, and therefore did not correlate with neural stress. This would be in line with a previous study by Silk et al. (Reference Silk, Steinberg and Morris2003) observing little effect of primary control strategies, such as problem solving, on feelings of anxiety and anger during adolescence. Moreover, coping literature suggests that these strategies are more helpful in stressful situations that are controllable rather than uncontrollable (Compas, Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Thomsen, & Wadsworth, Reference Compas, Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Thomsen and Wadsworth2001). It is also possible that the adaptive strategies measured could not be sufficiently used. This would be in line with studies finding low levels of adaptive emotion regulation, and increased use of maladaptive emotion regulation during adolescence (Cracco, Goossens, & Braet, Reference Cracco, Goossens and Braet2017; Zimmermann & Iwanski, Reference Zimmermann and Iwanski2014). In addition, adolescents might not have been able to apply strategies such as cognitive problem solving or humor enhancement (categorized as adaptive) within the context of the MIST. Future studies might examine effects of habitual maladaptive and adaptive strategy use on different kinds of stressors and associated emotional reactions.

Associations between subjective and physiological (autonomic/neural) stress responses

We did not find a significant association between subjectively experienced stress and physiological measures (HR/neural activation). In contrast, an early study on the neural correlates of psychological stress reported a positive correlation between subjective stress ratings and neural responses in several areas, including the right ventral PFC, the left insula/putamen, and the superior temporal gyrus (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Rao, Wetmore, Furlan, Korczykowski, Dinges and Detre2005). However, while several studies examining neural stress responses to the MIST report task-related increases in subjective stress (Boehringer et al., Reference Boehringer, Tost, Haddad, Lederbogen, Wust, Schwarz and Meyer-Lindenberg2015; Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Pestke, Feeser, Aust, Weigand, Wang and Bajbouj2014; Lederbogen et al., Reference Lederbogen, Kirsch, Haddad, Streit, Tost, Schuch and Meyer-Lindenberg2011; Zschucke et al., Reference Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg and Ströhle2015), very few explicitly examine the correspondence between neural and subjective stress responses. A study by Ali, Nitschke, Cooperman, and Pruessner (Reference Ali, Nitschke, Cooperman and Pruessner2017) found that suppression of autonomic and endocrine stress responses through pharmacological interventions did not lower participants’ subjective stress responses. This points to a considerable dissociation between subjective experience of stress and physiological stress systems. It is also in line with a previous review by Campbell and Ehlert (Reference Campbell and Ehlert2012) that reported an association between biological and subjective measures in only about one quarter of all studies. As has previously been suggested (Ali et al., Reference Ali, Nitschke, Cooperman and Pruessner2017; Andrews & Pruessner, Reference Andrews and Pruessner2013), this might be due to several methodological issues. Explicit measurements of subjective stress responses, like the one applied in the current study, are often not found to be linked with physiological stress responses. A promising approach towards gaining a better understanding between subjective stress responses and physiological measures might be to use implicit measurements, which have been found to correspond more consistently with biological stress systems (e.g., Mossink, Verkuil, Burger, Tollenaar, & Brosschot, Reference Mossink, Verkuil, Burger, Tollenaar and Brosschot2015; Verkuil, Brosschot, & Thayer, Reference Verkuil, Brosschot and Thayer2014; Wegner, Schüler, Schulz Scheuermann, Machado, & Budde, Reference Wegner, Schüler, Schulz Scheuermann, Machado and Budde2015).

Strengths and limitations

Finally, we would like to offer some additional considerations concerning our study. In our approach to study the neural substrates of psychological stress, we assumed that stress induces tonic changes of neural activity. First, we did not collect endocrinological data on activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis or sympathetic nervous system during the psychosocial stress induction, which is a clear limitation of the current study. Nonetheless, the MIST task is a well-validated task for psychosocial stress induction and subjective stress ratings, as well as the increase in HR clearly confirms a pronounced stress response to the MIST. Furthermore, previous studies have reported baseline drifts and carry-over effects of stress-induced psychoneurendocrine activation (cf. Wang et al., Reference Wang, Rao, Wetmore, Furlan, Korczykowski, Dinges and Detre2005). We therefore followed Zschucke et al. (Reference Zschucke, Renneberg, Dimeo, Wüstenberg and Ströhle2015) and refrained from using a classical block design BOLD contrast, but rather chose a design of successively increasing stress levels. However, this might somewhat reduce comparability of the current study with effects of other studies in adults using more conventional approaches. Furthermore, when interpreting the present results, we compared with other studies in adults, since information about acute stress processing in adolescents is sparse or even absent. Although our findings indicate a difference in the neural circuitry of acute psychosocial stress and associations with emotion regulation in adolescents versus adults, this assumption must be explored in future studies considering direct comparisons of adolescents and adults. Moreover, there was a relatively high amount of movement in our data, leading to the exclusion of 10 participants from the fMRI sample and 17 from HR analysis. This is rather typical and might be related to intrinsic behavioral properties of the sample in terms of age. Movement artefacts are a common problem in stress induction paradigms and likely to be more pronounced in developmental samples. We used a relatively conservative threshold for fMRI correction (whole brain, voxel-wise FWE correction with p < .05 at the peak level) and conservative exclusion criterion for HR artefacts to minimize inadvertent effects. Lastly, variance of the FEEL-KJ data was relatively low, which is likely to have factored into non-significant results, particularly when analyzing associations between stress-related neural activations and adaptive emotion regulation strategies.

Conclusion and Outlook

The neural profile of psychosocial stress highlights the close affinity of psychosocial stress and social rejection experiences for the individual during mid-adolescence. The extent of pronounced linear neural activation rather than deactivation, particularly in the insula, angular, and anterior temporal cortices, emerges when contrasting with adult MIST studies. Based on observed associations with maladaptive regulation and autonomic activity, the anterior temporal gyrus is likely to play a more central role in psychosocial stress processing during mid-adolescence. Its activation during acute psychosocial stress might facilitate adaptive stress processing by means of reappraisal and by binding complex stressors to regulate visceral emotional responses. We speculate that the usage of maladaptive emotion regulation might increase the social threat and reappraisal load of an acute psychosocial stressor. We suggest that future studies should explore the role of the anterior temporal gyrus in stress buffering during adolescence.

Overall, neural stress response and processing of associated emotions during adolescence is likely to impact mental health over the lifetime (Young, Sandman, & Craske, Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019). For example, lower temporal gyrus activation to negative emotional stimuli has been associated with depression during adolescence (Quevedo et al., Reference Quevedo, Harms, Sauder, Scott, Mohamed, Thomas and Smyda2018). In addition, the reward-mediation model poses that stress potentiates the risk of depression via impairing reward processing (Auerbach, Admon, & Pizzagalli, Reference Auerbach, Admon and Pizzagalli2014). Future research should probe whether neural response to acute psychosocial stressors at this vulnerable developmental stage predicts development of depression and anxiety disorders and whether dysfunctional strategies to regulate emotions influence this potential link.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000572

Acknowledgments

We thank students, principals, and teachers, as well as parents for their cooperation in making this study possible.

Funding Statement

Funding for this study was provided by the Volkswagen Foundation (Schumpeter Fellowship, II/84 452). S.G. was supported by a NeuroCure PhD Fellowship (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, EXC257). A.B. and P.P. were in part supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG, project ID 402170461 – TRR 265). A.B. was additionally in part supported by the German Ministry for Education and Research (BMBF 01ZX1311E/1611E) and the German Ministry of Health (BMG, ZMVI1-2516DSM223).

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

Ali, N., Nitschke, J. P., Cooperman, C., & Pruessner, J. C. (2017). Suppressing the endocrine and autonomic stress systems does not impact the emotional stress experience after psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 78, 125130. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allendorfer, J. B., & Szaflarski, J. P. (2014). Contributions of fMRI towards our understanding of the response to psychosocial stress in epilepsy and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy & Behavior, 35, 1925. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.03.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2013). The combined propranolol/TSST paradigm–a new method for psychoneuroendocrinology. PLoS One, 8, e57567. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057567CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auerbach, R. P., Admon, R., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Adolescent depression: Stress and reward dysfunction. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 22, 139148. doi:10.1097/HRP.0000000000000034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (1979). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York, NY: Penguin.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 267277. doi:10.1038/nrn2353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boehringer, A., Tost, H., Haddad, L., Lederbogen, F., Wust, S., Schwarz, E., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2015). Neural correlates of the cortisol awakening response in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 22782285. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogdan, R., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: Implications for depression. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 11471154. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Born, J. M., Lemmens, S. G., Rutters, F., Nieuwenhuizen, A. G., Formisano, E., Goebel, R., & Westerterp-Plantenga, M. S. (2010). Acute stress and food-related reward activation in the brain during food choice during eating in the absence of hunger. International Journal of Obesity, 34, 172181. doi:10.1038/ijo.2009.221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, A. C., Nakamura, K., & Roesch, M. R. (2015). From ventral-medial to dorsal-lateral striatum: Neural correlates of reward-guided decision-making. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 117, 5159. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2014.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, J., & Ehlert, U. (2012). Acute psychosocial stress: Does the emotional stress response correspond with physiological responses? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 11111134. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.12.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cao, J.-L., Covington, H. E., Friedman, A. K., Wilkinson, M. B., Walsh, J. J., Cooper, D. C., … Han, M.-H. (2010). Mesolimbic dopamine neurons in the brain reward circuit mediate susceptibility to social defeat and antidepressant action. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1645316458. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3177-10.2010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Heller, A. S., Gee, D. G., & Cohen, A. O. (2019). Development of the emotional brain. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 2934. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., Levita, L., Libby, V., Pattwell, S., Ruberry, E., … Somerville, L. H. (2010). The storm and stress of adolescence: Insights from human imaging and mouse genetics. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 225235. doi:10.1002/dev.20447Google ScholarPubMed
Choudhary, N., Deepak, K. K., Chandra, P. S., Bhatia, S., Sagar, R., Jaryal, A. K., … Tripathi, M. (2017). Comparison of autonomic function before and after surgical intervention in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Epilepsy Research, 7, 8998. doi:10.14581/jer.17014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomsen, A. H., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2001). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 87127. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.87CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cracco, E., Goossens, L., & Braet, C. (2017). Emotion regulation across childhood and adolescence: Evidence for a maladaptive shift in adolescence. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 909921. doi:10.1007/s00787-017-0952-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cracco, E., Van Durme, K., & Braet, C. (2015). Validation of the FEEL-KJ: An instrument to measure emotion regulation strategies in children and adolescents. PLoS One, 10(9), e0137080. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.013708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. E. (2004). Adolescent brain development: A period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 122. doi:10.1196/annals.1308.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. E., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Heightened stress responsiveness and emotional reactivity during pubertal maturation: Implications for psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 16. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dedovic, K., D'Aguiar, C., & Pruessner, J. C. (2009). What stress does to your brain: A review of neuroimaging studies. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie, 54, 615. doi:10.1177/070674370905400104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dedovic, K., Renwick, R., Mahani, N. K., Engert, V., Lupien, S. J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2005). The Montreal imaging stress task: Using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 30, 319325.Google ScholarPubMed
Delgado, M. R. (2007). Reward-related responses in the human striatum. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1104, 7088. doi:10.1196/annals.1390.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, M. R., Miller, M. M., Inati, S., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). An fMRI study of reward-related probability learning. NeuroImage, 24, 862873. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eiland, L., & Romeo, R. D. (2013). Stress and the developing adolescent brain. Neuroscience, 249, 162171. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.10.048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eklund, A., Nichols, T. E., & Knutsson, H. (2016). Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 79007905. doi:10.1073/pnas.1602413113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etkin, A., Egner, T., & Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 8593. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, Y., Pestke, K., Feeser, M., Aust, S., Pruessner, J. C., Böker, H., … Grimm, S. (2015). Amygdala–hippocampal connectivity changes during acute psychosocial stress: Joint effect of early life stress and oxytocin. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 27362744. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, E. E., Christopher May, J., Siegle, G. J., Ladouceur, C. D., Ryan, N. D., Carter, C. S., … Dahl, R. E. (2006). Reward-related decision-making in pediatric major depressive disorder: An fMRI study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 10311040. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01673.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, E. E., Hariri, A. R., Martin, S. L., Silk, J. S., Moyles, D. L., Fisher, P. M., … Dahl, R. E. (2009). Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 6473. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gärtner, M., Rohde-Liebenau, L., Grimm, S., & Bajbouj, M. (2014). Working memory-related frontal theta activity is decreased under acute stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 43, 105113. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.02.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., & Casey, B. J. (2015). The impact of developmental timing for stress and recovery. Neurobiology of Stress, 1, 184194. doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2015.02.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, D. G., Gabard-Durnam, L., Telzer, E. H., Humphreys, K. L., Goff, B., Shapiro, M., … Tottenham, N. (2014). Maternal buffering of human amygdala-prefrontal circuitry during childhood but not during adolescence. Psychological Science, 25, 20672078. doi:10.1177/0956797614550878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giedd, J. N. (2004). Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 7785. doi:10.1196/annals.1308.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., … Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 81748179. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402680101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, S., Pestke, K., Feeser, M., Aust, S., Weigand, A., Wang, J., … Bajbouj, M. (2014). Early life stress modulates oxytocin effects on limbic system during acute psychosocial stress. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 18281835. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grob, A., & Smolenski, C. (2005). Fragebogen zur Erhebung der Emotionsregulation bei Kindern und Jugendlichen (FEEL-KJ). Bern: Verlag Hans Huber.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two depends on your point of view. Emotion Review, 3, 816. doi:10.1177/1754073910380974CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunther Moor, B., van Leijenhorst, L., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Crone, E. A., & Van der Molen, M. W. (2010). Do you like me? Neural correlates of social evaluation and developmental trajectories. Social Neuroscience, 5, 461482. doi:10.1080/17470910903526155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamani, C., Mayberg, H., Stone, S., Laxton, A., Haber, S., & Lozano, A. M. (2011). The subcallosal cingulate gyrus in the context of major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 301308. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, H., & Glenn, A. L. (2017). Evaluating methods of correcting for multiple comparisons implemented in SPM12 in social neuroscience fMRI studies: An example from moral psychology. Social Neuroscience, 111. doi:10.1080/17470919.2017.1324521Google ScholarPubMed
Haruno, M., & Kawato, M. (2006). Different neural correlates of reward expectation and reward expectation error in the putamen and caudate nucleus during stimulus-action-reward association learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 95, 948959. doi:10.1152/jn.00382.2005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henson, R. (2015). Analysis of variance (ANOVA). Brain Mapping, 1, 477481. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00319-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermans, E. J., Henckens, M. J. A. G., Joëls, M., & Fernández, G. (2014). Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends in Neurosciences, 37, 304314. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2014.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., Connolly, C. G., Henje Blom, E., LeWinn, K. Z., Strigo, I. A., Paulus, M. P., … Yang, T. T. (2015). Emotion-dependent functional connectivity of the default mode network in adolescent depression. Biological Psychiatry, 78, 635646. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., Yang, G., Wu, J., Cassey, P., Brown, S. D., Hoang, N., … Yang, T. T. (2014). Functional connectivity of negative emotional processing in adolescent depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 155, 6574. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.10.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holtzheimer, P. E., Husain, M. M., Lisanby, S. H., Taylor, S. F., Whitworth, L. A., McClintock, S., … Mayberg, H. S. (2017). Subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: A multisite, randomised, sham-controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4, 839849. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30371-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ironside, M., Kumar, P., Kang, M.-S., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2018). Brain mechanisms mediating effects of stress on reward sensitivity. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 22, 106113. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasper, L., Bollmann, S., Diaconescu, A. O., Hutton, C., Heinzle, J., Iglesias, S., … Stephan, K. E. (2017). The PhysIO toolbox for modeling physiological noise in fMRI data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 276, 5672. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.10.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., & Wang, P. S. (2008). The descriptive epidemiology of commonly occurring mental disorders in the United States. Annual Review of Public Health, 29, 115129. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090847CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khalili-Mahani, N., Dedovic, K., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., & Pruessner, J. C. (2010). Hippocampal activation during a cognitive task is associated with subsequent neuroendocrine and cognitive responses to psychological stress. Hippocampus, 20, 323334. doi:10.1002/hipo.20623CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kogler, L., Müller, V. I., Chang, A., Eickhoff, S. B., Fox, P. T., Gur, R. C., & Derntl, B. (2015). Psychosocial versus physiological stress — meta-analyses on deactivations and activations of the neural correlates of stress reactions. NeuroImage, 119, 235251. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kondo, H., Saleem, K. S., & Price, J. L. (2003). Differential connections of the temporal pole with the orbital and medial prefrontal networks in macaque monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 465, 499523. doi:10.1002/cne.10842CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kouzma, N. M., & Kennedy, G. A. (2004). Self-reported sources of stress in senior high school students. Psychological Reports, 94, 314316. doi:10.2466/pr0.94.1.314-316CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 691702. doi:10.1038/nrn1747CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krkovic, K., Clamor, A., & Lincoln, T. M. (2018). Emotion regulation as a predictor of the endocrine, autonomic, affective, and symptomatic stress response and recovery. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 94, 112120. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.04.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kullik, A., & Petermann, F. (2013). Attachment to parents and peers as a risk factor for adolescent depressive disorders: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 44, 537548. doi:10.1007/s10578-012-0347-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laxton, A. W., Neimat, J. S., Davis, K. D., Womelsdorf, T., Hutchison, W. D., Dostrovsky, J. O., … Lozano, A. M. (2013). Neuronal coding of implicit emotion categories in the subcallosal cortex in patients with depression. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 714719. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.03.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeBlanc, S., Essay, C. A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2017). In Essay, C. A., LeBlanc, S. & Ollendick, T. H. (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lederbogen, F., Kirsch, P., Haddad, L., Streit, F., Tost, H., Schuch, P., … Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2011). City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature, 474, 498501. doi:10.1038/nature10190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lincoln, S. H., Pisoni, A., Bondy, E., Kumar, P., Singleton, P., Hajcak, G., … Auerbach, R. P. (2019). Altered reward processing following an acute social stressor in adolescents. PLoS One, 14, e0209361. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0209361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., Maheu, F., Tu, M., Fiocco, A., & Schramek, T. E. (2007). The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: Implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain and Cognition, 65, 209237. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 434445. doi:10.1038/nrn2639CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Pfeifer, J. H., McNealy, K., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2009). Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: Understanding the distress of peer rejection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 143157. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McFarquhar, M. (2019). Modeling group-level repeated measurements of neuroimaging data using the univariate general linear model. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 352. doi:10.3389/fnins.2019.00352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McRae, K., Gross, J. J., Weber, J., Robertson, E. R., Sokol-Hessner, P., Ray, R. D., … Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The development of emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children, adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 1122. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr093CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mossink, J. C. L., Verkuil, B., Burger, A. M., Tollenaar, M. S., & Brosschot, J. F. (2015). Ambulatory assessed implicit affect is associated with salivary cortisol. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nasso, S., Vanderhasselt, M. A., Demeyer, I., & De Raedt, R. (2018). Autonomic regulation in response to stress: The influence of anticipatory emotion regulation strategies and trait rumination. Emotion, doi:10.1037/emo0000448Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E. B., & Pine, D. S. (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 163174. doi:10.1017/S0033291704003915CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noack, H., Nolte, L., Nieratschker, V., Habel, U., & Derntl, B. (2019). Imaging stress: An overview of stress induction methods in the MR scanner. Journal of Neural Transmission, doi:10.1007/s00702-018-01965-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 704708. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oei, N. Y. L., Veer, I. M., Wolf, O. T., Spinhoven, P., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Elzinga, B. M. (2012). Stress shifts brain activation towards ventral “affective” areas during emotional distraction. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 403412. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97113. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, I. R., Plotzker, A., & Ezzyat, Y. (2007). The enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain, 130, 17181731. doi:10.1093/brain/awm052CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orenius, T. I., Raij, T. T., Nuortimo, A., Näätänen, P., Lipsanen, J., & Karlsson, H. (2017). The interaction of emotion and pain in the insula and secondary somatosensory cortex. Neuroscience, 349, 185194. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.02.047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T., Keshavan, M., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 947957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfeifer, J. H., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2012). Adolescent social cognitive and affective neuroscience: Past, present, and future. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 110. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr099CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, M. L., Drevets, W. C., Rauch, S. L., & Lane, R. (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 504514. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00168-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platt, B., Kadosh, K. C., & Lau, J. Y. F. (2013). The role of peer rejection in adolescent depression. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 809821. doi:10.1002/da.22120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porcelli, A. J., Lewis, A. H., & Delgado, M. R. (2012). Acute stress influences neural circuits of reward processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 157. doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Dedovic, K., Khalili-Mahani, N., Engert, V., Pruessner, M., Buss, C., … Lupien, S. J. (2008). Deactivation of the limbic system during acute psychosocial stress: Evidence from positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 234240. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Dedovic, K., Pruessner, M., Lord, C., Buss, C., Collins, L., … Lupien, S. J. (2010). Stress regulation in the central nervous system: Evidence from structural and functional neuroimaging studies in human populations – 2008 Curt Richter award winner. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 179191. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quevedo, K., Harms, M., Sauder, M., Scott, H., Mohamed, S., Thomas, K. M., … Smyda, G. (2018). The neurobiology of self face recognition among depressed adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 229, 2231. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2017.12.023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romeo, R. D. (2010). Adolescence: A central event in shaping stress reactivity. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 244253. doi:10.1002/dev.20437Google ScholarPubMed
Romeo, R. D., & McEwen, B. S. (2007). Stress and the adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 202214. doi:10.1196/annals.1376.022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romund, L., Golde, S., Lorenz, R. C., Raufelder, D., Pelz, P., Gleich, T., … Beck, A. (2017). Neural correlates of the self-concept in adolescence—A focus on the significance of friends. Human Brain Mapping, 38, 987996. doi:10.1002/hbm.23433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L. A., & Witherington, D. (2006). Emotional development: Action, communication, and understanding. Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons Inc.Google Scholar
Schäfer, , Naumann, E., Holmes, E. A., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Samson, A. C. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46, 261276. doi:10.1007/s10964-016-0585-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastian, C. L., Tan, G. C. Y., Roiser, J. P., Viding, E., Dumontheil, I., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2011). Developmental influences on the neural bases of responses to social rejection: Implications of social neuroscience for education. NeuroImage, 57, 686694. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.063CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebastian, C. L., Viding, E., Williams, K. D., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 72, 134145. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selemon, L. D. (2013). A role for synaptic plasticity in the adolescent development of executive function. Translational Psychiatry, 3, e238. doi:10.1038/tp.2013.7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapero, B. G., Stange, J. P., McArthur, B. A., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2019). Cognitive reappraisal attenuates the association between depressive symptoms and emotional response to stress during adolescence. Cognition and Emotion, 33, 524535. doi:10.1080/02699931.2018.1462148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74, 18691880. doi:10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, M. H., Jedd, K., & Luciana, M. (2015). Neural networks involved in adolescent reward processing: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 122, 427439. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.083CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvers, J. A., Insel, C., Powers, A., Franz, P., Helion, C., Martin, R. E., … Ochsner, K. N. (2017). vlPFC–vmPFC–amygdala interactions underlie age-related differences in cognitive regulation of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 27, 35023514. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw073Google ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Taylor, S. E. (2010). Neural sensitivity to social rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 1481714822. doi:10.1073/pnas.1009164107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soliman, A., O'Driscoll, G. A., Pruessner, J., Joober, R., Ditto, B., Streicker, E., … Dagher, A. (2011). Limbic response to psychosocial stress in schizotypy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Schizophrenia Research, 131, 184191. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.05.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, L. H. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 121127. doi:10.1177/0963721413476512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, L. H., Whalen, P. J., & Kelley, W. M. (2010). Human bed nucleus of the stria terminalis indexes hypervigilant threat monitoring. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 416424. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreckelmeyer, K. N., Krach, S., Kohls, G., Rademacher, L., Irmak, A., Konrad, K., … Gründer, G. (2009). Anticipation of monetary and social reward differently activates mesolimbic brain structures in men and women. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 158165. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, E. A., Parsons, C. E., Van Hartevelt, T. J., Charquero-Ballester, M., McManners, H., Ehlers, A., … Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Post-traumatic stress influences the brain even in the absence of symptoms: A systematic, quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 56, 207221. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.07.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, N. R., Gerhart, J., Goldsmith, R. E., Heath, N. M., Chesney, S. A., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2013). Emotion regulation difficulties, low social support, and interpersonal violence mediate the link between childhood abuse and posttraumatic stress symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 44, 152161. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2012.09.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 2552. doi:10.2307/1166137CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tottenham, N., & Galván, A. (2016). Stress and the adolescent brain: Amygdala-prefrontal cortex circuitry and ventral striatum as developmental targets. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 217227. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C. S., Westenberg, P. M., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Crone, E. A. (2010). What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating reward sensitivity across adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 6169. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp078CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Oort, J., Tendolkar, I., Hermans, E. J., Mulders, P. C., Beckmann, C. F., Schene, A. H., … van Eijndhoven, P. F. (2017). How the brain connects in response to acute stress: A review at the human brain systems level. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 281297. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., & Thayer, J. F. (2014). Cardiac reactivity to and recovery from acute stress: Temporal associations with implicit anxiety. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 92, 8591. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, D. M., Bell, M. R., Flores, C., Gulley, J. M., Willing, J., & Paul, M. J. (2017). Adolescence and reward: Making sense of neural and behavioral changes amid the chaos. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 1085510866. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1834-17.2017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, J., Rao, H., Wetmore, G. S., Furlan, P. M., Korczykowski, M., Dinges, D. F., & Detre, J. A. (2005). Perfusion functional MRI reveals cerebral blood flow pattern under psychological stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 1780417809. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503082102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, M., Schüler, J., Schulz Scheuermann, K., Machado, S., & Budde, H. (2015). The implicit power motive and adolescents’ salivary cortisol responses to acute psychosocial stress and exercise in school. CNS & Neurological Disorders – Drug Targets, 14, 12191224. doi:10.2174/1871527315666151111123122CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weil, S., Arnold, S., Eisensehr, I., & Noachtar, S. (2005). Heart rate increase in otherwise subclinical seizures is different in temporal versus extratemporal seizure onset: Support for temporal lobe autonomic influence. Epileptic Disorders, 7, 199204.Google ScholarPubMed
Wiech, K., Lin, C.-s., Brodersen, K. H., Bingel, U., Ploner, M., & Tracey, I. (2010). Anterior insula integrates information about salience into perceptual decisions about pain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1632416331. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2087-10.2010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, S. K., Sandman, F. C., & Craske, G. M. (2019). Positive and negative emotion regulation in adolescence: Links to anxiety and depression. Brain Sciences, 9), doi:10.3390/brainsci9040076CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeman, J., Klimes-Dougan, B., Cassano, M., & Adrian, M. (2007). Measurement issues in emotion research with children and adolescents. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 14, 377401. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.2007.00098.xGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, P., & Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 182194. doi:10.1177/0165025413515405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zschucke, E., Renneberg, B., Dimeo, F., Wüstenberg, T., & Ströhle, A. (2015). The stress-buffering effect of acute exercise: Evidence for HPA axis negative feedback. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 51, 414425. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. (a) Depiction of significant activation clusters from the contrast high > low stress with flexible factorial analysis of variance (whole brain familywise error [FWE] corrected with p < .05 at the peak level, k ≥ 5 voxels). (b) Depiction of significant activation clusters from the contrast low > high stress with flexible factorial analysis of variance (same statistical threshold as in (a)). (c) Contrast estimates for peak voxels of the main activation clusters from the flexible factorial analysis of variance showing stress-associated activation increases (left) or decreases (right). Low = low stress condition; moderate = moderate stress condition; high = high stress condition; ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; Ins (R) = right insular cortex; Ins (L) = left insular cortex; mid/inf temp = middle/inferior temporal gyrus; angular = angular gyrus; ant temp = anterior temporal gyrus; caud (R) = right caudate nucleus; caud (L) = left caudate nucleus.

Figure 1

Table 1. Comparison between high and low stress conditions with flexible factorial analysis of variance

Figure 2

Figure 2. (a) Voxel-wise regression analysis: significant positive correlation between stress-associated anterior temporal gyrus activation and the tendency to use maladaptive strategies to regulate negative emotions. Depicted are activation clusters after whole brain familywise error (FWE) correction with p < .05 at the peak level (brain) and extracted parameter estimated from 5 mm sphere around peak (scatter). (b) Significant positive Pearson correlation between stress-associated temporal gyrus activation and stress-induced increase in HR. +Please note that the correlation between HR and activation cluster in the temporal gyrus peaking at Montreal Neuroimaging Institute (MNI) 60/−13/−23 did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Mid/inf temp = middle/inferior temporal gyrus; ant temp = anterior temporal gyrus; HR = heart rate.

Supplementary material: File

Golde et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Golde et al. supplementary material(File)
File 36.6 KB