Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa

Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorder

  • Atsushi Senju (a1), Victoria Southgate (a1), Yui Miura (a2), Tomoko Matsui (a2), Toshikazu Hasegawa (a3), Yoshikuni Tojo (a4), Hiroo Osanai (a5) and Gergely Csibra (a1) (a6)...
Abstract
Abstract

Recently, a series of studies demonstrated false belief understanding in young children through completely nonverbal measures. These studies have revealed that children younger than 3 years of age, who consistently fail the standard verbal false belief test, can anticipate others' actions based on their attributed false beliefs. The current study examined whether children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), who are known to have difficulties in the verbal false belief test, may also show such action anticipation in a nonverbal false belief test. We presented video stimuli of an actor watching an object being hidden in a box. The object was then displaced while the actor was looking away. We recorded children's eye movements and coded whether they spontaneously anticipated the actor's subsequent behavior, which could only have been predicted if they had attributed a false belief to her. Although typically developing children correctly anticipated the action, children with ASD failed to show such action anticipation. The results suggest that children with ASD have an impairment in false belief attribution, which is independent of their verbal ability.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Atsushi Senju, Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, School of Psychology, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK; E-mail: a.senju@bbk.ac.uk.
Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

F. Abell , F. Happe , & U. Frith (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15, 116.

R. Adolphs , F. Gosselin , T. W. Buchanan , D. Tranel , P. Schyns , & A. R. Damasio (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature, 433, 6872.

J. W. Astington , & J. M. Jenkins (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 13111320.

S. Baron-Cohen , T. Jolliffe , C. Mortimore , & M. Robertson (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813822.

S. Baron-Cohen , A. M. Leslie , & U. Frith (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.

S. Baron-Cohen , S. Wheelwright , J. Hill , Y. Raste , & I. Plumb (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241251.

S. K. Berument , M. Rutter , C. Lord , A. Pickles , & A. Bailey (1999). Autism Screening Questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444451.

S. A. Birch , & P. Bloom (2003). Children are cursed: An asymmetric bias in mental-state attribution. Psychological Science, 14, 283286.

P. Bloom , & T. P. German (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77, B25B31.

G. Dawson , S. J. Webb , & J. McPartland (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 403424.

C. Fine , J. Lumsden , & R. J. R. Blair (2001). Dissociation between “theory of mind” and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain, 124, 287298.

F. G. Happé (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843855.

M. H. Johnson (2005). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 766774.

A. Klin , D. J. Lin , P. Gorrindo , G. Ramsay , & W. Jones (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459, 257261.

A. M. Leslie , O. Friedman , & T. P. German (2004). Core mechanisms in “theory of mind.” Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 528533.

K. H. Onishi , & R. Baillargeon (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255258.

S. Ozonoff , & J. N. Miller (1995). Teaching theory of mind: A new approach to social skills training for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 415433.

S. Ozonoff , B. F. Pennington , & S. J. Rogers (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 10811105.

J. Perner (1995). The many faces of belief: Reflections on Fodor's and the child's theory of mind. Cognition, 57, 241269.

J. Perner , & T. Ruffman (2005). Psychology. Infants' insight into the mind: how deep? Science, 308, 214216.

T. Ruffman , W. Garnham , & P. Rideout (2001). Social understanding in autism: Eye gaze as a measure of core insights. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 10831094.

J. Russell , R. Saltmarsh , & E. Hill (1999). What do executive factors contribute to the failure on false belief tasks by children with autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 859868.

N. Russo , T. Flanagan , G. Iarocci , D. Berringer , P. D. Zelazo , & J. A. Burack (2007). Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: Implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain and Cognition, 65, 7786.

R. Saxe , S. Carey , & N. Kanwisher (2004). Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 87124.

R. T. Schultz (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 125141.

A. Senju , V. Southgate , S. White , & U. Frith (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883885.

V. Southgate , A. Senju , & G. Csibra (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587592.

M. L. Spezio , P.-Y. S. Huang , F. Castelli , & R. Adolphs (2007). Amygdala damage impairs eye contact during conversations with real people. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 39943997.

V. E. Stone , S. Baron-Cohen , A. Calder , J. Keane , & A. Young (2003). Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41, 209220.

H. Tager-Flusberg (1981). On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 4556.

H. Wimmer , & J. Perner (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.

P. D. Zelazo , C. Jacques , J. A. Burack , & D. Frye (2002). The relation between theory of mind and rule use: Evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development, 11, 171195.

G. Csibra , & V. Southgate (2006). Evidence for infants understanding of false beliefs should not be dismissed. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 45.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Development and Psychopathology
  • ISSN: 0954-5794
  • EISSN: 1469-2198
  • URL: /core/journals/development-and-psychopathology
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×