Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-12T13:00:12.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why the Days Seem Shorter as We Get Older

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 March 2019

Adrian Bejan*
Affiliation:
Department of Mechanical Engineering and Materials Science, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina 27708-0300, USA. Email: abejan@duke.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Why does it feel that the time passes faster as we get older? What is the physical basis for the impression that some days are slower than others? Why do we tend to focus on the unusual (the surprise), not on the ever present? This article unveils the physics basis for these common observations. The reason is that the measurable ‘clock time’ is not the same as the time perceived by the human mind. The ‘mind time’ is a sequence of images, i.e. reflections of nature that are fed by stimuli from sensory organs. The rate at which changes in mental images are perceived decreases with age, because of several physical features that change with age: saccades frequency, body size, pathways degradation, etc. The misalignment between mental-image time and clock time serves to unite the voluminous observations of this phenomenon in the literature with the constructal law of evolution of flow architecture, as physics.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Academia Europaea 2019 

Perceptions

Among the most common human perceptions is that time passes faster as an individual becomes older. The days become shorter, and so do the years. We all have stories of this kind, from the long days of childhood and the never-ending class hours in elementary school, to days, months and years that now pass in a blur. The most common sayings convey this impression: Times flies; Where did the time go?; Last year was yesterday; Growing up took forever; A watched pot never boils; etc.

More subtle, and worth questioning is the impression that some days appear to pass more slowly than others. The ‘slower’ days are full of productivity, events, and memories of what happened. If you did not notice this difference between slow days and fast days, then you should pay attention to it, because in this difference lies the explanation for the lifelong puzzle sketched in the preceding paragraph. The hint is that productive days happen when the body and mind are rested, after periods of regular sleep, when in the morning you look in the mirror and you see a younger you, not a tired you.

Athletes learn the hard way the correlation between good rest and the speed of the passing time. Lack of rest makes you miss plays, unable to anticipate, unable to see the ball before it arrives. While sleep walking, the game is over before you know it.

Young students learn the same physical truth while taking exams during a fixed time interval. The rested mind has more time to go through the problems, to find mistakes, to go back to the beginning, and try again. Lack of sleep, due to cramming the night before the exam, makes the time pass faster during the exam period. Cramming does not pay, but rest does, which is why the good coach rests the team before the big game.

Here is why this is important to you, the reader. Today, many young people experience time distortion because they spend too much time on social media. This has serious consequences, ranging from sleep deprivation to mood changes and mental disorder. This is why an understanding of the physics basis of how humans perceive the passing of time is essential.

Physics

Time represents perceived changes in stimuli (observed facts), such as visual images.Reference Bejan 1 , Reference Bejan 2 The human mind perceives reality (nature, physics) through images that occur as visual inputs reach the cortex. The mind senses ‘time change’ when the perceived image changes. The time arrow in physics is the goal-oriented sequence of changes in flow configuration, the direction dictated by the constructal law.Reference Bejan 1 - Reference Bejan 11 The present is different from the past because the mental viewing has changed, not because somebody’s clock rings.

The ‘clock time’ that unites all the live flow systems, animate and inanimate, is measurable. The day–night period lasts 24 hours on all watches, wall clocks and bell towers. Yet, physical time is not mind time. The time that you perceive is not the same as the time perceived by another. Why? Because the young mind receives more images during one day than the same mind in old age. Said another way, if the lifespan is measured in terms of the number of images perceived during life, then the frequency of mental images at young age is greater than in old age (Figure 1). Here is why this should be:

Figure 1 The misalignment between perceived time and clock time during lifetime.

The sensory inputs that travel into the human body to become mental images – ‘reflections’ of reality in the human mind – are intermittent. They occur at certain time intervals (t 1), and must travel the body length scale (L) with a certain speed (V). In the case of vision, t 1 is the time interval between successive saccades. The time required by one mental image to travel from a sensory organ to the cortex is of order t 2 ~ L/V. During life, the body length scale (L) increases in proportion with the body mass M raised to the power 1/3, and, like all growth phenomena, the body mass increases over time in S-curve fashion,Reference Bejan and Lorente 12 monotonically, slow – fast – slow, cf. Figure 2.

Figure 2 All growth phenomena (spreading, collecting) exhibit an S-shaped history curveReference Lucia, Ponzetto and Deisboeck 8 : four flow systems where the size of the flow space increases monotonically, slow–fast–slow.

The length traveled by inputs from external sensors to the cortex is actually greater than L, and it increases with age. The reason is that the complexity of the flow path needed by one signal to reach one point on the cortex increases as the brain grows and the complexity of the tree-shaped flow paths increase, cf. Figure 3.Reference Wechsatol, Lorente and Bejan 13

Figure 3 The length of the flow path increases as the body size and complexity increase.Reference Lucia, Grazzini, Montrucchio, Grisolia, Borchiellini, Gervino, Castagnoli, Ponzetto and Silvagno 9

The broad trend then is that L increases with age. At the same time, V decreases because of the ageing (degradation) of the flow paths. The key feature is that the physical time (the combined effect of t 1 and t 2) required by the occurrence of one mental image increases monotonically during the life of the individual. The frequency of mental images decreases monotonically, and non-uniformly (i.e. not at constant rate). This trend is illustrated qualitatively in Figure 1. Two summarizing conclusions follow.

  1. (i) More of the recorded mental images should be from youth.

  2. (ii) The ‘speed’ of the time perceived by the human mind should increase over life. The rate at which the physical time clock ‘ticks’ during one change in the mental image increases with age.

Review

The misalignment of the clock ticks and the changes perceived by the mind (Figure 1) brings together numerous observations and measurements accumulated in the literature, especially in the study of vision and cognition.

First, to define the terms, Fischer and Weber explain that during natural viewing conditions a normal adult subject makes 3–5 saccades in a second separated by periods of 200-300 ms during which the eyes do not make large or fast movements.Reference Fischer and Weber 14 These periods are usually called ‘fixations’. If the retinal image, as a whole, is prevented from moving (by successful voluntary attempts not to move the eyes, or by technical means), vision rapidly becomes blurred and the perception of the retinal image fades away completely within 10 seconds. Fischer and WeberReference Fischer and Weber 14 explain that the highly inhomogeneous structure of the primate retina, with an extremely high density of receptor and ganglion cells in the center, a specialized fovea, and a rapid decline of the cell densities toward the periphery, makes it almost impossible to have a homogeneous and simultaneous percept of the total visual field without somehow moving the fovea to different positions and acquiring and integrating information from these successive ‘looks’. The existence of a fovea requires both eye movements and periods of fixation, that is, the active suppression of saccadic eye movements.

Although the reaction times of saccades is relatively stable (200–250 ms),Reference Butler, Zacks and Henderson 15 the infant fixation times are shorter than in adults. In primates, there is a constant relationship between the duration, peak velocity and amplitude of saccadic eye movement,Reference Boghen, Troost, Daroft, Dell’Osso and Birkett 16 Reference Harris and Wolpert 18 known as the ‘main sequence’, in which saccade trajectories have evolved toward optimizing the trade-off between accuracy and duration (speed) of the eye movement. This is also in accord with the physics basis for the human preference for displays shaped in ‘golden-ratio’ rectangular frames, which is the shape that is scanned the fastest by the two human eyes.Reference Bejan 2 As a result of an interaction between afferent, central and efferent neural processes we perceive a complete and stable visual field, which can serve as a frame within which we see motion and within which we move ourselves or parts of our body.Reference Fischer and Weber 14

Bahill and Stark showed that fatigue can produce overlapping saccades in which the high-frequency saccadic bursts should show large pauses, glissades in which the high-frequency bursts should be much shorter than appropriate for the size of the intended saccades, and low-velocity, long-duration, non-Main Sequence saccades in which the mononeuronal bursts should be of lower frequency and longer duration than normal.Reference Bahill and Stark 19 When the saccadic eye movement system fatigues, saccades become slower, and the neurological control signal stratagem changes. The term fatigue is used in a broad sense, as it was by McFarland: ‘a group of phenomena associated with impairment, or loss, of efficiency and skill’.Reference McFarland 20

The intuitive view that the world is processed as a seamless stream of ongoing perception has been challenged in the current literature. Herzog et al. discussed experimental evidence supporting the view that perception might be discrete, further supporting evidence for discrete theories.Reference Herzog, Kammer and Scharnowski 21 Visual information processing is similar to a sample and hold mechanism in engineering, as in analog/digital converters.

Herzog et al. also noted that the brain functions such that we consciously perceive only the most plausible solution, and not a confusing manifold of possibilities that occur during unconscious processing.Reference Herzog, Kammer and Scharnowski 21 The unconscious feature integration period is the period of sense-making. The discrete conscious perception is followed by unconscious processing over time. These two modes of absorbing inputs from the surroundings are analogous to all other flows from point (e.g. eye) to volume (e.g. brain).Reference Bejan 1 Observing fast and then letting it sink in slowly, is the same dynamic flow design as the long and fast, and short and slow that inhabits all nature,Reference Bejan 1 animate and inanimate. Conscious perception and the unconscious processing that follows are the ‘invasion’ and ‘consolidation’ phases of the universal S-curve phenomenon.Reference Bejan and Lorente 12

Cicchini et al. review the classical model of time perception, which considers a single centralized clock that ticks at a constant rate.Reference Cichini and Morrone 22 They point out that much experimental evidence seems to cast doubt on this model. The ability to pay ‘attention’ could modulate the tick rate and hence the duration of the events.Reference Levin and Zakay 23 Many studies found that the most surprising stimulus within a train of events is perceived longer, probably because it engages more transient attention or because the event is less predictable. Bruno et al. show that the apparent duration of moving visual objects is greater at higher than at lower speeds.Reference Bruno, Ayhan and Johnston 24

Pöppel et al. argued that cognitive processes cannot be understood without their temporal dynamics; furthermore, certain logistical problems the brain has to deal with require an understanding of temporal processing.Reference Pöppel 25 Eagleman et al. discussed the flash-lag illusion, where a flash and a moving object in the same location appear to be offset.Reference Eagleman and Sejnowski 26 They proposed an alternative in which visual awareness is neither predictive nor online but is postdictive, so that the percept attributed to the time of the flash is a function of events that happen in the ~80 ms after the flash. Interpolation of the past is the only framework that provides a unified explanation for the flash-lag phenomenon.

VanRullen and Koch reconciled the unduly abandoned topic of discrete perception with current views and advances in neuroscience.Reference VanRullen and Koch 27 Hainline et al. showed that for both infants and adults, linear relationships were found between the peak velocities of fast eye movements and their amplitudes (main sequences).Reference Hainline, Turkel, Abramov, Lemerise and Harris 28

With regard to the effect of aging, Sharpe and Zackon investigated horizontal saccades in young, middle-aged and elderly normal subjects.Reference Sharpe and Zackon 29 Saccades were elicited in response to three target conditions: predictable amplitude direct and timing; unpredictable amplitudes and directions at regular intervals; and unpredictably timed targets of predictable amplitude and direction. Peak velocities were significantly reduced in the elderly when target amplitude and direction were predictable. Latencies were prolonged in the elderly under all conditions. Saccadic accuracy was significantly decreased in elderly subjects. Support for the thesis that age-related cognitive slowing is global is provided by Myerson et al.Reference Myerson, Hale, Wagstaff, Poon and Smith 30 Although older adults perform worse than younger adults in complex decision-making scenarios, prior experience should be taken into account in aging studies.Reference Blanco, Love, Ramscar and Otto 31

Conclusion

Summing up, we conclude that the perceived misalignment between mental-image time and clock time (Figure 1) is in accord with and unifies the growing number of observations that describe aspects of this phenomenon in the literature. The physics basis is captured by the constructal law of evolution in nature (see Figures 2 and 3).

Acknowledgement

Professor Adrian Bejan’s research was supported by the US National Science Foundation.

Adrian Bejan is the J.A. Jones Distinguished Professor at Duke University, and is a member of Academia Europaea. He is a graduate of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (BS 1971, MS 1972, PhD 1975) and a recipient of the Benjamin Franklin Medal (2018). He has received 18 honorary doctorates from universities in 11 countries. He has published widely on thermodynamics, evolution, and the constructal law of organization in nature, as physics.

References

1. Bejan, A. (2016) The Physics of Life (New York: St. Martin’s Press).Google Scholar
2. Bejan, A. (2009) The golden ratio predicted: Vision, cognition and locomotion as a single design in nature. International Journal of Design & Nature and Ecodynamics, 4(2), pp. 97104.10.2495/DNE-V4-N2-97-104Google Scholar
3. Basak, T. (2013) The law of life: The bridge between Physics and Biology. Physics of Life Reviews, 8, pp. 249252.10.1016/j.plrev.2011.07.003Google Scholar
4. Miguel, A.F. (2011) The physics principle of the generation of flow configuration. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(3), pp. 243244.10.1016/j.plrev.2011.07.006Google Scholar
5. Reis, A.H. (2011) Design in nature, and the laws of physics. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(3), pp. 255256.10.1016/j.plrev.2011.07.001Google Scholar
6. Wang, L. (2011) Universality of design and its evolution. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(3), pp. 257258.10.1016/j.plrev.2011.08.003Google Scholar
7. Lorenzini, G. and Biserni, C. (2011) The constructal law: From design in nature to social dynamics and wealth as physics. Physics of Life Reviews, 8(3), pp. 259260.10.1016/j.plrev.2011.08.002Google Scholar
8. Lucia, U., Ponzetto, A. and Deisboeck, T.S. (2014) A thermo-physical analysis of the proton pump vacuolar-ATPase: The constructal approach. Scientific Reports, 4, p. 6763.10.1038/srep06763Google Scholar
9. Lucia, U., Grazzini, G., Montrucchio, B., Grisolia, G., Borchiellini, R., Gervino, G., Castagnoli, C., Ponzetto, A. and Silvagno, F. (2015) Constructal thermodynamics combined with infrared experiments to evaluate temperature differences in cells. Scientific Reports, 5, p. 11587.10.1038/srep11587Google Scholar
10. Lucia, U., Ponzetto, A. and Deisboeck, T.S. (2016) Constructal approach to cell membranes transport: Amending the ‘Norton-Simon’ hypothesis for cancer treatment. Scientific Reports, 6, p. 19451.10.1038/srep19451Google Scholar
11. Bejan, A. (2012) Why the bigger live longer and travel farther: Animals, vehicles, rivers and the winds. Scientific Reports, 2, no. 594.10.1038/srep00594Google Scholar
12. Bejan, A. and Lorente, S. (2011) The constructal law origin of the logistics S curve. Journal of Applied Physics, 110, 024901.10.1063/1.3606555Google Scholar
13. Wechsatol, W., Lorente, S. and Bejan, A. (2002) Optimal tree-shaped networks for fluid flow in a disc-shaped body. International Journal of Heat and Mass Transfer, 45, pp. 49114924.10.1016/S0017-9310(02)00211-9Google Scholar
14. Fischer, B. and Weber, H. (1993) Express saccades and visual attention. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, pp. 553610.10.1017/S0140525X00031575Google Scholar
15. Butler, K.M., Zacks, R.T. and Henderson, J.M. (1999) Suppression of reflexive saccades in younger and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 27(4), pp. 584591.10.3758/BF03211552Google Scholar
16. Boghen, D., Troost, B.T., Daroft, R.B., Dell’Osso, L.F. and Birkett, J.E. (1974) Velocity characteristics of normal human saccades. Investigative Ophthalmology, 13(8), pp. 619623.Google Scholar
17. Harris, C.M., Hainline, L., Abramov, L., Lemerise, E. and Camenzuli, C. (1998) The distribution of fixation durations in infants and naïve adults. Vision Research, 28(3), pp. 419432.10.1016/0042-6989(88)90184-8Google Scholar
18. Harris, C.M. and Wolpert, D.M. (2006) The main sequence of saccades optimizes speed-accuracy trade-off. Biological Cybernetics, 95, pp. 2129.10.1007/s00422-006-0064-xGoogle Scholar
19. Bahill, A.T. and Stark, L. (1975) Overlapping saccades and glissades are produced by fatigue in the saccadic eye movement system. Experimental Neurology, 48, pp. 95106.10.1016/0014-4886(75)90225-3Google Scholar
20. McFarland, R.A. (1971) Understanding fatigue in modern life. Ergonomics, 14, pp. 110.10.1080/00140137108931216Google Scholar
21. Herzog, M.H., Kammer, T. and Scharnowski, F. (2016) Time slices: What is the duration of a percept? PLOS Biology, 12 April 2016. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002433.10.1371/journal.pbio.1002433Google Scholar
22. Cichini, G.M. and Morrone, M.C. (2009) Shifts in spatial attention affect the perceived duration of events. Journal of Vision, 9(1), pp. 113.10.1167/9.1.9Google Scholar
23. Levin, I. and Zakay, D. (Eds) (1989) Subjective time and attentional resource allocation: An integrated model of time estimation. Time and Human Cognition: A Life-span Perspective (Amsterdam: North-Holland), pp. 365397.Google Scholar
24. Bruno, A., Ayhan, I. and Johnston, A. (2015) Changes in apparent duration following shifts in perpetual timing. Journal of Vision, 15(6), pp. 118.10.1167/15.6.2Google Scholar
25. Pöppel, E. (1997) A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(2), p. 56.10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01008-5Google Scholar
26. Eagleman, D.M. and Sejnowski, T.J. (2000) Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. Science, 287, pp. 20362038.10.1126/science.287.5460.2036Google Scholar
27. VanRullen, R. and Koch, C. (2003) Is perception discrete or continuous? TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), pp. 207213.10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00095-0Google Scholar
28. Hainline, L., Turkel, J., Abramov, I., Lemerise, E. and Harris, C.M. (1984) Characteristics of saccades in human infants. Vision Research, 24(12), pp. 17711780.10.1016/0042-6989(84)90008-7Google Scholar
29. Sharpe, J.A. and Zackon, D.H. (1987) Senescent saccades. Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh), 104, pp. 422428.10.3109/00016488709128270Google Scholar
30. Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L.W. and Smith, G.A. (1990) The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97(4), pp. 475487.10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.475Google Scholar
31. Blanco, N.J., Love, B.C., Ramscar, M. and Otto, A.R. (2016) Exploratory decision-making as a function of lifelong experience, not cognitive decline. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(3), pp. 284297.10.1037/xge0000133Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 The misalignment between perceived time and clock time during lifetime.

Figure 1

Figure 2 All growth phenomena (spreading, collecting) exhibit an S-shaped history curve8: four flow systems where the size of the flow space increases monotonically, slow–fast–slow.

Figure 2

Figure 3 The length of the flow path increases as the body size and complexity increase.9