Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T03:02:43.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Care of Infants in the Past: Bridging evolutionary anthropological and bioarchaeological approaches

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2020

Siân Halcrow*
Affiliation:
Department of Anatomy, University of Otago, New Zealand
Ruth Warren
Affiliation:
Department of Anatomy, University of Otago, New Zealand
Geoff Kushnick
Affiliation:
School of Archaeology and Anthropology, Research School of Humanities and the Arts, ANU College of Arts and Social Sciences, Australian National University, Australia
April Nowell
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Victoria, Canada
*
*Corresponding author: E-mail: sian.halcrow@otago.ac.nz

Abstract

The importance of care of infants and children in palaeoanthropological and human behavioural ecological research on the evolution of our species is evident in the diversity of research on human development, alloparental care, and learning and social interaction. There has been a recent surge of interest in modelling the social implications of care provision for people with serious disabilities in bioarchaeology. However, there is a lack of acknowledgement of infant and child care in bioarchaeology, despite the significant labour and resources that are required, and the implications this has for health outcomes within societies. Drawing on the recent proliferation of studies on infancy and childhood in evolutionary anthropology and bioarchaeology, this paper presents ways the subdisciplines may draw on research developments from each field to advance a more holistic understanding of the evolutionary, social and health significance of infant and children care in the past.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Evolutionary Human Sciences

Media summary: A tool for evolutionary anthropology and bioarchaeology to produce a holistic understanding of infant care in the past.

1. Introduction

Over the past 15 years or so there has been a proliferation of research on infants and children in the bioarchaeological and human evolutionary context (Halcrow & Tayles, Reference Halcrow and Tayles2010; Lewis, Reference Lewis2007; Nowell, Reference Nowell, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016, Reference Nowell2020). However, there is a disconnect between evolutionary biological anthropology and bioarchaeological investigations of childhood, despite the complimentary approaches and theoretical developments adopted in these subfields. Like Lancy's (Reference Lancy2012) pictorial representation of the house of the Anthropology of Childhood, the rooms between the approaches, instead of being in an open plan architectural design, they have separate rooms with their doors firmly shut. As Lancy (Reference Lancy2012: 3) states:

The building might house the Anthropology of Childhood but it is ephemeral as all one sees are the separate doors/cubicles of the more narrowly focused enterprises with no interconnections. The best evidence I can offer for this claim is the rarity of cross-citations and very brief, shallow literature reviews in much of the published work – past and present.

Although there is limited communication between evolutionary anthropology and bioarchaeological approaches to childhood, the subfields have much in common. Clearly, there is a shared interest within the study of palaeoanthropology and human behavioural ecology and bioarchaeology. These fields all examine past childhoods and human experience from an anthropological perspective. Social aspects of care, alloparenting (care by someone other than the biological parent) and infant health are central to evolutionary anthropological and bioarchaeological reconstructions of the human life course (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Nowell, Reference Nowell, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016, Reference Nowell2020; Sear, Reference Sear2015). In bioarchaeology, infants and children are largely assessed from a palaeopathological context, with little investigation of care (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Le Roy & Murphy, Reference Le Roy, Murphy, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Tilley & Nystrom, Reference Tilley and Nystrom2019). However, the exploration of care is essential for interpreting the lived experience for infants from the past (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Powell et al., Reference Powell, Southwell-Wright and Gowland2016). This paper presents a review of research on infants and children in bioarchaeology and evolutionary anthropology and how we might start to open these closed doors between the approaches.

We review theoretical approaches of care of infants and children used in bioarchaeology and evolutionary anthropology, and highlight the contributions both subfields can make to a holistic appreciation of infant and child care within populations from the recent and deep evolutionary past. Within this review we present a comprehensive bioarchaeological theoretical model for the interpretation of evidence for infant and child care in the past, and demonstrate how this model can be extended with insights from human evolutionary (palaeoanthropological and human behavioural ecology) theory and research (Figure 1). This model assesses the social and health impacts that infant care provision and the mother–infant nexus have on past societies. It considers variables including maternal and infant health and mortality, infant feeding practices, fertility, family and social structure, and population size that may be useful for palaeoanthropologists to consider in their work. We further discuss how the evolutionary anthropological literature may be incorporated into current theoretical and analytical approaches for bioarchaeology. Here we show how bioarchaeological investigations of infant care can benefit from insights from human evolution to extend the model of infant care, including the consideration of the development of human infancy and childhood through the lens of life history theory, alloparental care, energetics, neurobehavioural development of shared intentionality, and human behavioural ecology theories of the relationship between parenting, subsistence and property inheritance.

Figure 1. Schematic of relationship between bioarchaeology and evolutionary anthropology.

2. Infant and childhood bioarchaeology

Infant and child bioarchaeology, by its very simple definition, is the study of infant and child human remains from the archaeological record (Halcrow & Tayles, Reference Halcrow and Tayles2008; Halcrow & Ward, Reference Halcrow, Ward and Smith2018; Lewis, Reference Lewis2007). Following Buikstra's (Reference Buikstra and Blakey1977) definition, the field of bioarchaeology is particularly interested in the study of the human biological consequences of environmental and social factors, through the investigation of elements such as health, disease, mortality and growth.

2.1. Brief history of infant and child bioarchaeology

Biological anthropology has had an interest in infants and children since its early beginnings with Boas’ (Reference Boas and Spiller1912) inquiries into human variation in the growth of children in response to different social and health environments. However, biological anthropologists were mainly interested in the measurement of bodies and skeletal remains, in particular craniometry. Infant and child crania were deemed useless in this endeavour because they were often found disarticulated in archaeological contexts as a result of their unossified joints. It was not until the 1960s that there began to be a focus on infant and child health experience within archaeological research. Francis Johnston was a pioneer in the bioarchaeological study of infants and children, investigating growth and development and mortality from the Indian Knoll skeletal remains (Johnston, Reference Johnston1962). However, it has only been for the past 15 years, approximately, that there has been consistent work in the field, including the development of social theoretical approaches to studying childhood in the past (e.g. Gowland, Reference Yannopoulos, Lyberatos, Theodossiou, Li, Valipour, Tamburrino and Angelakis2020; Inglis & Halcrow, Reference Inglis, Halcrow, Beauchesne and Agarwal2018).

To a large extent, the development of childhood archaeology was precipitated by the rise of feminist critiques within anthropology and archaeology throughout the 1970s, which led to a heightened awareness of aspects of gender and age identity in the past (Halcrow & Tayles, Reference Halcrow and Tayles2008; Lewis, Reference Lewis2007). Around 20 years ago, Kamp (Reference Kamp2001), Lillehammer (Reference Lillehammer1989, Reference Lillehammer2015) and others asked ‘where have all the children gone’ in archaeology (Kamp, Reference Kamp2001), and called for the inclusion of children in archaeological interpretations.

Since the mid-1990s there has been a rapid increase in the amount of research on children and childhood in the past from anthropological, archaeological and bioarchaeological perspectives (Baxter, Reference Baxter2005; Crawford et al., Reference Crawford, Hadley and Shepherd2018; Lillehammer, Reference Lillehammer2015; Mays et al., Reference Mays, Gowland, Halcrow and Murphy2017), and the start of the recognition of the value of assessing the young and the intricate relationship between the mother and infant (Blake, Reference Blake, Han, Betsinger and Scott2017; Gowland & Halcrow, Reference Gowland and Halcrow2020; Halcrow et al., Reference Halcrow, Tayles, Elliott, Han, Betsinger and Scott2017; Le Roy & Murphy, Reference Le Roy, Murphy, Gowland and Halcrow2020). The recognition of the wealth of information that can be gleaned from the study of infants and children has resulted in a large number of bioarchaeological studies investigating mortality, palaeopathology, growth and growth disruption (Lewis, Reference Lewis2007, Reference Lewis2017; Halcrow & Tayles, Reference Halcrow and Tayles2008). Recent advances in social bioarchaeological theory, including an integration of life-course theory and the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) hypothesis, highlight the pivotal place of early life stages in understanding past societies, and the intricate relationship between maternal and child health (Agarwal & Glencross, Reference Agarwal and Glencross2011; Halcrow & Ward, Reference Halcrow, Ward and Montgomery2017; Gowland, Reference Hodson, Gowland, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Gowland & Halcrow, Reference Gowland and Halcrow2020; Gowland & Knüsel, Reference Gowland and Knüsel2006; Sofaer, Reference Sofaer2006).

Despite the developments in the field of childhood archaeology and bioarchaeology (Inglis & Halcrow, Reference Inglis, Halcrow, Beauchesne and Agarwal2018; Lillehammer, Reference Lillehammer2015; Mays et al., Reference Mays, Gowland, Halcrow and Murphy2017), there is still a lack of consideration of social aspects of the human experience within bioarchaeology, especially around infant and child care (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). Bioarchaeology, by definition, has a bias towards the study of the physical or corporeal body, such as an assessment of manifestations of disease, so the subfield is understandably focused on these biological data (Halcrow & Tayles, Reference Halcrow and Tayles2010).

There has been a recent surge in modelling the implications of care provisioning for people with serious disabilities in the bioarchaeological and palaeopathological literature (Tilley, Reference Tilley2015; Tilley & Oxenham Reference Tilley and Oxenham2011; Tilley & Schrenk Reference Tilley and Schrenk2017). This work has provided essential consideration of social responses and compassion towards people with long-term care needs from disabilities and other health-related conditions. However, Tilley (Reference Tilley2015: 100) restricts her definition to health-related care provision, arguing:

some of the skill sets used in health-related caregiving undeniably overlap with some of those employed in assisting healthy women around pregnancy and in raising dependent infants … Nevertheless, caring for an individual with a specific, continuing disability entails qualitatively different sets of actions and motivations, and this is illustrated by comparing non-pathology-related maternal and infant care requirements … This is not to deny that some pregnant women, some mothers and probably many infants may be candidates for healthcare at various times. However, to explain health-related care provision as an extension of infant nurturing is to ignore the quite different, and perhaps more complex, cognitive demands involved in caring for an individual suffering the impacts of disease.

By viewing infant care as nurturing, and not as complex nor requiring the same cognitive demands as health-related care ignores a significant part of the picture of past societies and health-related care (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). As noted, bioarchaeology understandably has a focus on the physical manifestations of abnormal bone changes from pathology, and therefore disability. However, here we use a more holistic definition of care and caregiving based on the anthropological work on care and gender by Cancian and Oliker (Reference Cancian and Oliker1999: 2):

a feeling of affection and responsibility combines with actions that provide responsibility for an individual's personal needs or well-being, in a face-to-face relationship. Caregiving includes physical care, such as bathing or feeding a person, as well as emotional care, such as tender touch, supportive talk, empathy, and affection.

There are many reasons for the acknowledgement that care of infants in past societies is crucial to our interpretations of human experience in the past. Western thought discerns medical care as sophisticated and complex, but regards mothering and caring for infants as unskilled, where mothering is seen as ‘instinctual’. The assumption that infant care is unskilled ignores the significant care and resources that are invested in infant and child care and the health effects that this has on mothers and children, and undermines the very complex nature of infant care, including learning to breastfeed (Palmquist, Reference Palmquist, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Tomori et al., Reference Tomori, Palmquist and Quinn2017). This investment in care is even more significant when we recognise that human infants are born in the most immature (altricial) state of all primates, as is well recognised in human evolution research. Human infants demand 24 hour care, including breastfeeding, help with eating, toileting and regulating temperature, and a significant amount of holding and carrying (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). In some past societies, the number of infants and children would have been considerable, and required significant labour and resources within society. Infants and children also carry a high disease burden in societies (World Health Organization, 2019), so their health and care would have been a significant preoccupation of parents and caregivers in the past (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020).

2.2. The significance of infant and childhood care on life in the past

The act of caring for infants and children has repercussions on the adult and child caregivers in society, and the type of care has a direct effect on infant health and wellbeing (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Powell et al., Reference Powell, Southwell-Wright and Gowland2016). Young infants require significant care, yet an acknowledgement of the role of care for infants is lacking in the bioarchaeological literature (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). Possibly the biggest determinant of foetal and infant health is the care and support they receive, and this starts before birth with the care of the mother (Gowland, Reference Gowland2015; Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). Foetal and maternal health is arguably the most sensitive measure of population health, given the increased energetic requirements of pregnant and lactating mothers, and the energy requirements for fast-growing foetuses (Altmann & Samuels, Reference Altmann and Samuels1992; Bogin & Smith, Reference Bogin and Smith1996). The effects of malnutrition and many infections can be exacerbated during pregnancy. For example, infection with Plasmodium vivax or Plasmodium falciparum during pregnancy leads to chronic anaemia and placental malaria infection, reducing the birth weight and increasing the risk of neonatal death (Brabin et al., Reference Brabin, Romagosa, Abdelgalil, Menéndez, Verhoeff, McGready and Ordi2004). The pivotal role of care as a determinant of maternal and infant health is further illustrated if we consider the DOHaD hypothesis (Barker & Osmond, Reference Barker and Osmond1986). This considers how environmental impacts on a mother induce physiological changes in foetal and child growth and development that have long-term consequences on later health and disease risk. Furthermore, human infants have underdeveloped immune systems coupled with fast growth rates, resulting in their vulnerability to environmental stress (Halcrow & Tayles, Reference Halcrow and Tayles2008; Lewis, Reference Lewis2007).

Tilley provides a multiple-step system within her model of care to determine whether people in the past needed long-term health-related care and to interpret the nature of that care within a biosocial context (Tilley & Oxenham, Reference Tilley and Oxenham2011). Presenting a case study of an individual with palaeopathological evidence for quadriplegia in prehistoric Vietnam, Tilley and Oxenham (Reference Tilley and Oxenham2011) argue that the survival and good mental health of a person with a serious disability necessitated the provision of long-term, skilled and consistent care, probably involving multiple group members, including the allocation of food/a special diet, water, shelter, bedding, a hazard-free environment, and help with eating and drinking and managing hygiene (removal of wastes, bathing). Interestingly, if we consider human infant care, infants have the same requirements as an individual with a significant disability, and in addition require breastfeeding, special preparation of foods (such as the pre-mastication of food), holding, carrying, rocking, sleeping, massaging and assistance to keep cool/warm (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). The recent model proposed for considering the impact of care for infants and children in the past considers multiple factors of past societies (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). This model (Figure 2) considers, in addition to the environmental factors that Tilley's model proposes, evidence for infant and maternal health and mortality within past populations, specific diseases through the investigation of pathology (i.e. genetic diseases, and chronic illness from within specific environmental contexts), infant feeding practices (bottle feeding, introduction of solids), demographic information (e.g. fertility and maternal health, size and number of children in families and the community), and family and social structure (through the investigation of migration, gender relationships and health) (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). Further extension of this model can be gained through the consideration of evolutionary anthropological approaches that are discussed in Section 4.

Figure 2. Bioarchaeological model of infant and childcare in the past.

3. Childhood and evolutionary anthropology

Palaeoanthropology is the study of prehistoric hominins and their primate relatives, and assesses the fossil record within an evolutionary context. The study of juvenile fossil remains has always been central to palaeoanthropological investigations, as they are recognised as being crucial for understanding ontogeny and the evolution of our species (Bogin & Smith, Reference Bogin and Smith1996; Reference Bogin, Smith, Stinson, Bogin and O'Rourke2012; Bogin et al., Reference Bogin, Varea, Hermanussen and Scheffler2018; Bolter et al., Reference Bolter, Elliott, Hawks and Berger2020). As such, there is not a dedicated ‘palaeoanthropology of infancy and childhood’ such as what developed in bioarchaeology in response to the neglect of research on this subject. The topic of care of infants and social learning also takes a central place in palaeoanthropological investigation of the evolution of our species (Högberg & Gärdenfors, Reference Högberg and Gärdenfors2015; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Nowell & Kurki, Reference Nowell, Kurki, Gowland and Halcrow2020). Human behavioural ecology analyses human behaviour and cultural diversity through the principles of evolutionary theory, and life histories of humans within an ecological and adaptive context (Nettle et al. Reference Nettle, Gibson, Lawson and Sear2013). Palaeoanthropology and human behavioural ecology draw on an impressive variety of studies to understand the early life course and what makes us human, including using developmental biology, neurobiological development and behaviour, ethnographic, history and primate studies of care to model the past (Bogin & Smith, Reference Bogin and Smith1996; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Konner, Reference Konner, Hewlett and Lamb2005, Reference Konner2010).

3.1. Brief history of infant and child palaeoanthropology

Palaeoanthropological research is hampered by a rare and fragmentary fossil record. It is hardly surprising, therefore, that spanning over 7 million years of hominin history, only 200–300 infant and juvenile fossil hominin individuals have been discovered (Nowell & Kurki, Reference Nowell, Kurki, Gowland and Halcrow2020), very few of which represent early modern humans. The scant nature of the juvenile fossil evidence for these early species limits a comprehensive study of ontogenetic development of hominin evolution, but that does not sideline the importance of these rare discoveries and the contributions they have made to the field. The Taung Child, the type specimen for Australopithicus africanus, was integral to the debate that Africa was the cradle of humankind (Tobias, Reference Tobias1984). The recent discovery of a 3.3 million-year-old Australopithecus afarensis juvenile from Dikika, Ethiopia, represents the earliest and most complete partial skeleton of a child ever found (Alemseged et al., Reference Alemseged, Spoor, Kimbel, Bobe, Geraads, Reed and Wynn2006). Analysis of this juvenile is illuminating our understanding of the growth and development of Australopithecus afarensis (Alemseged et al., Reference Alemseged, Spoor, Kimbel, Bobe, Geraads, Reed and Wynn2006). The Mojokerto child has been central to the assessment of brain growth in Homo erectus (Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Quade, Levin, Butler, Dupont-Nivet, Everett and Semaw2008; Cofran & DeSilva, Reference Cofran and DeSilva2015), and new research on several fossils excavated from Drimolen cave includes a skull fragment believed to belong to a Homo erectus child, who was two or three years old at the time of death (Herries et al., Reference Herries, Martin, Leece, Adams, Boschian, Joannes-Boyau and Menter2020). A more substantial number of Neanderthal foetuses, infants and children have been excavated, providing sufficient evidence to investigate Neanderthal growth and development. Interdisciplinary analyses of these fossils enable palaeoanthropologists to gain insight into the complex interactions of growth and organisation of an evolving brain, the birthing process, social systems, weaning, diet, energetics and locomotion (Nowell & Kurki, Reference Nowell, Kurki, Gowland and Halcrow2020).

3.2. Palaeoanthropology, care and social development

The care of infants and children is central to palaeoanthropological understandings of the evolution of our species (DeSilva, Reference DeSilva2011; Foley, Reference Foley, Pryce, Martin and Skuse1995; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Piantadosi & Kidd, Reference Piantadosi and Kidd2016; Nowell & Kurki, Reference Nowell, Kurki, Gowland and Halcrow2020). The investigation of infant care and survivorship is paramount to the human evolutionary context. During hominin evolution there has been increased encephalisation, the birth of more altricial babies, and related to this, the development of alloparenting and cooperative care (Foley, Reference Foley, Pryce, Martin and Skuse1995; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Sear, Reference Sear2015). Nowell and Kurki (Reference Nowell, Kurki, Gowland and Halcrow2020: 182) recently provided a review of the evolutionary history of infant care and the centrality of its consideration in the evolution of human experience and behaviour. Human infants are unique because they are born with the smallest brain size in proportion to adult size, and across the hominin lineage the absolute brain size in neonates increases over time. These authors draw on Cofran and DeSilva's (Reference Cofran and DeSilva2015: 41) observations that the evolution of the human brain enables, but also requires, the cultural capacities (such as food procurement and cooperation) to energetically sustain it. This is shown by Piantadosi and Kidd (Reference Piantadosi and Kidd2016), who propose a model of how natural selection for large brains may lead to premature and particularly helpless newborns. Caring for these infants and children requires more intelligence and therefore even larger brains. The dynamics can be self-reinforcing and lead to selection for higher intelligence and helpless newborns (Piantadosi & Kidd, Reference Piantadosi and Kidd2016).

Evolutionary trends of growth and development can inform us of caregiver–infant interactions and aspects of their social systems. DeSilva (Reference DeSilva2011) argues that more physically immature, and therefore less mobile, infants would have necessitated greater parental involvement and energetics in their transportation (Hosfield, Reference Hosfield2020). Carrying a relatively large infant both pre- and postnatally has important ramifications for birthing strategies and locomotion (DeSilva, Reference DeSilva2011). The loss of body hair probably started with Australopiths (Dávid-Barrett & Dunbar, Reference Dávid-Barrett and Dunbar2016), and a more upright posture would have necessitated a change in the position of infant carrying from the back to the arms, allowing for more interaction between infants and their caregivers and the development of shared intentionality (Dávid-Barrett & Dunbar, Reference Dávid-Barrett and Dunbar2016), something that is unique to humans (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2014; Tomasello & Carpenter, Reference Tomasello and Carpenter2007). Shared intentionality refers to a suite of social-cognitive and social-motivational skills (e.g. gaze following and joint attention, and social manipulation) that result in collaborative interactions where people share psychological states (Tomasello & Carpenter, Reference Tomasello and Carpenter2007).

The development of shared intentionality was an important component in the development of the complex social structure required for the care of human babies. Research in developmental psychology shows the importance of social interaction as a developmental mechanism that infants use to support their understanding of intentionality through interactions with social partners (Brandone et al., Reference Brandone, Stout and Moty2020). Hrdy (Reference Hrdy2009) argues that during human evolution it was those infants who were proficient with ascertaining intentionality who were selected for and that this social interaction was central for the development of alloparenting. Alloparenting helps mothers conserve energy, stay better nourished and remain safer, leading to their increased survival and greater reproductive success and evolutionary fitness, despite the long human developmental phase (Bogin & Smith, Reference Bogin and Smith1996; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009). Although alloparenting is not unique to humans, in non-human primates this is normally carried out by juvenile females (Smith, Reference Smith2010), while for humans, alloparenting is done by a wider range of people in the community, including fathers and grandparents (Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Palmquist, Reference Palmquist, Gowland and Halcrow2020).

Recent work by Nowell (Reference Nowell, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016) has highlighted the importance of play in human neurodevelopment, where the extended period of development in humans allows additional years to learn, transmit, practise and modify social behaviours. Symbolic material cultural studies are used alongside psychological and neurobiological research to understand cognitive development in the past (Nowell, Reference Nowell, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016). Nowell (Reference Nowell, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016: 95) states ‘the key to understanding their cognitive abilities lies not in absolute brain size or encephalization quotients but rather in our ability to reconstruct their childhoods and to use the biological and archaeological evidence to piece together how they spent that time’.

4. Opening the doors between bioarchaeology and evolutionary anthropology in the ephemeral house of the anthropology of childhood

What can bioarchaeology draw from palaeoanthropology and human behavioural ecology in the study of care in the past?

4.1. Fitness costs and benefits of care

Evolutionary anthropological approaches recognise that, although there are many evolutionary forces shaping human bodies and behaviour, natural selection is central in explaining adaptations (Winterhalder & Smith, Reference Winterhalder and Smith2000). Evolutionary anthropology approaches, therefore, view parenting adaptations as biological features that serve the inclusive-fitness interests of parents. Although a given parental behaviour may be in the best interest of the offspring, parental and offspring fitness interests are not always perfectly aligned (Parker et al., Reference Parker, Royle and Hartley2002). Evolutionary ecology models of parenting rest on the assumption that parental actions incur fitness benefits and costs, and thus parents should behave in ways that optimise this trade-off (Borgerhoff Mulder, Reference Borgerhoff Mulder, Smith and Winterhalder1992). The problem for translating this set of tools to bioarchaeological and palaeontological contexts is that the benefits and costs are difficult, if not impossible, to study in these settings. By looking at coarser-grained differences, the evolutionary anthropological approach may be useful. For instance, we might expect very different parenting strategies in societies where care-independent sources of mortality are higher (owing to, for instance, the presence of epidemics or other disease-related factors), compared with those where they are lower. Increases in care-independent sources of child mortality lead to a non-linear decrease in the fitness-maximizing level of parental effort (Harpending et al., Reference Harpending, Draper, Pennington, Armelagos and Swedlund1990; Quinlan, Reference Quinlan2007). For example, Quinlan (Reference Quinlan2007) studied the effect of extrinsic (care-independent) mortality risk on parental behaviour using comparative evidence from an existing cross-cultural dataset and specific evidence from ethnographic work. He found a curvilinear relationship between various population-level measures of parental care (sleeping proximity, age at weaning, and others) and proxy measures of extrinsic mortality risk including warfare, and pathogen load. With low to moderate levels of increasing environmental risk, parental care increased, but further increases in environmental risk led to decreases in parental care. Quinlan (Reference Quinlan2007) argues that this supports the theory that humans show reduced parental effort in environments where parenting cannot improve offspring survival. He found similar effects studying changes in extrinsic mortality and parental care over a span of 75 years in a rural community in Dominica (Quinlan, Reference Quinlan2010).

4.2. Life history growth, energetics and trade-offs near the start of life

As discussed, palaeoanthropological studies have highlighted that humans have extremely fast foetal-like postnatal growth compared with other hominins and non-human primates. We argue that it is important for bioarchaeologists to consider developmental biological phases of the early life course in the interpretations of their health data. A consideration of human early developmental life stages is encapsulated within Life History Theory. This model, originating from biology, has been subsequently developed and applied to evolutionary anthropology by Hill (Reference Hill1993), McDade (Reference McDade2003) and others. This is a branch of evolutionary biology that explains variation in human developmental rates and mortality across the life cycle stages of gestation, infancy, childhood, adolescence and adulthood (Hill, Reference Hill1993; McDade, Reference McDade2003). Within this model a main tenet is that resources are limited, and invested in three primary areas of growth, maintenance and reproduction, and that resources allocated for one purpose cannot also be used for another, thus making trade-offs inevitable. During different developmental stages there are varying demands on resources, e.g. during infancy there are a lot of resources put into growth and development of immunity. This is highly relevant to infant and child bioarchaeology because the risk of infection and death from infectious disease is dramatically elevated in the first year of life; the energetic demands of infancy are extreme and trade-offs can be expected to be especially severe, particularly in low-resource settings; immune deficits following under-nutrition early in life have been shown to persist for weeks, and in some cases years; and it is likely that exposure to infectious disease early in life has long-term consequences for immune function (Bourke et al., Reference Bourke, Berkley and Prendergast2016). We also argue that this model could be extended by integrating evidence for cultural and social determinants (e.g. food allocation and therefore nutrition) in the context of the social life-course approach. For example, a study that uses a social life-course approach to investigate dietary isotopes during childhood (i.e. cultural factors that could influence weaning and food allocation at different parts of the life course), could be greatly enhanced by the life history approach. Here an integration of the life history approach takes into account the energy (and therefore food) demands for children at different ages, and also other constraints on infants including when potential pathogens are introduced with the introduction of non-maternal food resources at a time of significant immune development (Inglis & Halcrow, Reference Inglis, Halcrow, Beauchesne and Agarwal2018).

4.3. Alloparenting

The importance of alloparenting for our species and evolutionary history may be useful to consider in interpretations of maternal and infant and child care in bioarchaeology. Human maternal energy expended during pregnancy and lactation is significant (Altmann & Samuels, Reference Altmann and Samuels1992). Alloparenting is argued to be selected for, at least in part, in response to the energetic demands of a developmental life phase, potentially causing significant energetic burden in a mother for several years (Hosfield, Reference Hosfield2020). For humans, as mentioned, alloparenting in humans is done by a wider range of individuals in the community, such as siblings, fathers and grandparents (Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2009; Mace & Sear, Reference Mace, Sear, Voland, Chasiotis and Schiefenhoevel2005; Palmquist, Reference Palmquist, Gowland and Halcrow2020). The African proverb ‘it takes a village to raise a child’ is particularly apt when considering past care practices. The loss of this village could be extended to the loss of cultural and social support that we see in many Western societies today and in the past (Cunningham, Reference Cunningham2005), which can have detrimental effects on mothers and the care of their children, and therefore their health and wellbeing. Similarly, we can consider in certain social contexts in the past, where insufficient resources and support led to the disintegration of alloparenting within families. For example, Hodson and Gowland (Reference Hodson, Gowland, Gowland and Halcrow2020) found in a study in post-medieval London that when maternal resources were low, infants had significant growth stunting and pathological indicators on their skeletons. In these cases of impoverishment, babies and young children were often left with older siblings who may not have had the means to adequately care for them, or left by themselves, while their mothers and fathers were forced to work in low-paid jobs (Hodson & Gowland, Reference Hodson, Gowland, Gowland and Halcrow2020).

4.4. Shared intentionality

Shared intentionality may be useful for bioarchaeologists to consider, particularly during the early years of life, which is a period of significant neuro-behavioural development. Tomasello and Carpenter (Reference Tomasello and Carpenter2007) argue for the important role of shared intentionality in theories of human development. They highlight the infant gaze leading to joint attention, and social manipulation leading to communication. This is seen as central to the evolution of mother–infant communication when the parent requires cryptic information about the offspring's ‘need’ (Kushnick, Reference Kushnick2009). There may be some level of manipulation of the parent by the offspring, but this can be mitigated when there is a fitness cost to a response to infant crying (Kushnick, Reference Kushnick2009).

There is notable variation cross-culturally and among social groups in adult–infant communication patterns. For example, although some WEIRD (Western, educated, industrialised, rich and democratic) societies have a focus on communication with infants through gaze (Akhtar & Gernsbacher, Reference Akhtar and Gernsbacher2008), others have far more kinesthetic and tactile communication through close body contact (Akhtar & Gernsbacher, Reference Akhtar and Gernsbacher2008). Touch, posture and vocal cues are argued to be important cues to intentions (Akhtar & Gernsbacher, Reference Akhtar and Gernsbacher2008), and therefore central in communication development. For infants, touch is the first sensory modality to develop (Montagu, Reference Montagu1986), and is arguably the most important contributor in infant development of communication, attachment and emotional regulation (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Moore and McGlone2019; Hertenstein, Reference Hertenstein2002).

Psychological literature shows that during early childhood, care through feeding (and therefore touch and potentially visual factors) represents a pivotal experience for the development of the relationship and synchrony between mother and child, in which emotional signals promote the communication of needs and desires (Feldman, Reference Feldman2007; Lieberman & Slade, Reference Lieberman, Slade, Osofsky and Fitzgerald2000). Through feeding there is a development of close emotional engagement and a ‘conversational’ setting, where parents make sense of their baby's expressiveness and communicate their empathy and understanding, laying the foundations for their future affective and social communication (Feldman, Reference Feldman2007). This is important to consider in bioarchaeology when social contexts may lead to a loss of this interaction for infants and children, leading to psychological and health deficits. An example of this is the structural violence occurring against the poor and destitute in the workhouses during the Great Irish Famine, where infants were ‘nursed out’ of the workhouses by other women for payment of care, or infants were separated from their parents within the institutions (Geber, Reference Geber2005). This undoubtedly led to emotional suffering for both parents and infant, and potentially psychological deficits and/or poor health outcomes later in life.

4.5. Subsistence, fertility, property and parental care

The Neolithic transition is often viewed as an important turning point in the history of human societies, ushering in pervasive changes across the human social landscape, including the way we parent. Human behavioural ecologists have explored the change in parenting in relation to subsistence, and this should be something that is central to consider in the bioarchaeological models of care.

One such factor is the relationship between subsistence and fertility and the implications that can have on care (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020). Studies of modern foragers, such as !Kung from southern Africa, space their births up to four years (Howell, Reference Howell2010). In societies that cultivate, on the other hand, interbirth intervals are much shorter (Bentley et al., Reference Bentley, Goldberg and Jasienska1993). Intensive farmers tend to take this to a further extreme (Kramer & Boone, Reference Kramer and Boone2002). Lancaster and Lancaster (Reference Lancaster, Lancaster, Altmann, Rossi and Sherrod1987) argued that the transition from ‘low-density’ societies (foragers and extensive farmers) to ‘high-density’ ones (intensive agriculturalists and others) was accompanied by a change in parental considerations within different resource settings, the former requiring the rearing of ‘fit, healthy’ children and the latter requiring that, as well as the provision of resources that are unequally distributed within the community. Within this constrained resource environment, parental values are modified, which can lead to cultural regulation of the number of children reared, infanticide, child neglect and social systems of dowry (Lancaster & Lancaster, Reference Lancaster, Lancaster, Altmann, Rossi and Sherrod1987).

A further change in society associated with the Neolithic transition was the emergence of property and its inter-generational transition in the form of marriage payments and inheritance (Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Stieglitz, Hooper, Gomes and Kaplan2012; Shennan, Reference Shennan2011). Concerns over inheritance can have important ramifications for parental care of infants and young children (Kushnick, Reference Kushnick2010). Inheritance patterns with the development of agriculture and child care may be used to understand parenting behaviour in past societies. Recent bioarchaeological research on Chinese Bronze Age Eastern Zhou dynasty infant feeding patterns using chemical analyses of diet indicates gender preference in food allocation (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Dong, Pechenkina, Fan and Halcrow2020). The findings suggest that feeding children was a significant aspect of socialisation and cultural gendering of individuals (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Dong, Pechenkina, Fan and Halcrow2020), and that this finding may be related to the social system of patrilineal inheritance in historic and modern-period China.

4.6. Technology and childcare

Technologies related to childcare, so important for our way of parenting today, were probably used to some degree by humans in populations studied by bioarchaeologists and palaeoanthropologists. Kushnick (Reference Kushnick, Weekes-Shackelford and Shackelford2020) examines how the three major evolutionary approaches to human behaviour – human behavioural ecology, evolutionary psychology and cultural evolutionary approaches – can inform our understanding of the relationship between parenting behaviour and technology. Behavioural ecology approaches emphasise how parents behave in ways that balance the fitness costs and benefits of childcare and competing activities. The introduction of water-lifting devices, for instance, which raise water to the ground surface for usage, has a relatively deep history (Yannopoulos et al., Reference Yannopoulos, Lyberatos, Theodossiou, Li, Valipour, Tamburrino and Angelakis2015) and may signal something about how children were raised. The introduction of water pumps may reduce maternal energy expenditure and affect birthrate and child-rearing behaviours and health as a consequence (e.g. Gibson & Mace, Reference Gibson and Mace2006). Similarly, baby-carrying devices might arise as the result of a woman's need to free her hands for subsistence work. Kushnick (Reference Kushnick, Weekes-Shackelford and Shackelford2020) showed that, in an initial analysis of 77 societies from the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample, baby-carrying technology was present in around 75% of societies where women had high levels of responsibility for subsistence work, but only 50% where women had lower levels of responsibility in this domain. Sturdier varieties of carrying devices, such as cradleboards, may leave archaeological traces (Mattori et al., Reference Mattori, Armentrout, Larsen and Hutchinson1987), while cloth-based ones, such as slings, may not. Some use of these devices may leave physical traces on the body such as cranial modification from cradle boarding that has been seen in the bioarchaeological record (Pomeroy et al., Reference Pomeroy, Stock, Zakrzewski and Lahr2013).

4.7. Maternal health and altriciality from an evolutionary perspective

From an evolutionary perspective, it is interesting that during human pregnancy women can be subject to significant medical complications (Haig, Reference Haig2019). For instance, postpartum haemorrhage is the leading cause of maternal mortality today (Abrams & Rutherford, Reference Yannopoulos, Lyberatos, Theodossiou, Li, Valipour, Tamburrino and Angelakis2011). Haig (Reference Haig2019) asks why placentas are less reliable organs than hearts or kidneys and why maternal hearts and kidneys are more subject to catastrophic failures during pregnancy than at other times. Here he argues that pregnancy involves an interaction between genetically distinct individuals whose cooperation is countered by evolutionary conflicts of interest. Abrams and Rutherford (Reference Abrams and Rutherford2011) explain the occurrence of postpartum haemorrhage from an evolutionary biology perspective where humans have developed vulnerability to this condition from changes in placental invasiveness and vascular remodelling to counteract gravitational effects of bipedalism. They cite a cross-cultural study of indigenous birth practices where traditional childbirth attendants’ actions (Lefèber & Voorhoeve, Reference Lefèber and Voorhoeve1998) mimic World Health Organization recommendations on cord traction and uterine massage to induce placental birth (Abrams & Rutherford, Reference Abrams and Rutherford2011). Abrams and Rutherford (Reference Abrams and Rutherford2011) argue that the cross-cultural occurrence and variability in this practice illustrates the deep evolutionary past of placental vulnerability (Abrams & Rutherford, Reference Abrams and Rutherford2011). It is important to consider the evolutionary perspective on maternal health care and childbirth in bioarchaeological studies. As is shown today, more than one-third of all women who give birth do so without skilled birth assistants, which can lead to poor health outcomes for mother and baby (World Health Organization et al., 2015). Similarly, in resource-poor settings in the past, this probably had detrimental implications for maternal and child health.

The human birthing process with larger neonate skulls and changes in the configuration of the pelvis with bipedalism would have necessitated the development of help during birth (Trevathan, Reference Trevathan1987). Various hypotheses have been put forth for the truncation of the hominin gestational period, leading to an extremely altricial infant. The traditional view was that there is an ‘obstetric dilemma’ (OD), where there is a trade-off between a large skull and a constrained pelvic outlet (Washburn, Reference Washburn1960). However, more recent work explores a metabolic hypothesis which examines human maternal energy expenditures, where this is two times greater near the end of pregnancy than pre-pregnancy and a maximum threshold is met (Dunsworth et al., Reference Dunsworth, Warrener, Deacon, Ellison and Pontzer2012). Wells et al. (Reference Wells, Desilva and Stock2012) have argued that health factors related to ecological and cultural changes, especially with agricultural development, resulted in increases in infectious disease and poorer nutrition and therefore poorer growth outcomes, including the pelvic cavity. However, others have highlighted the modern biomedical approach to pregnancy and birth as leading to the perceived OD. Stone (Reference Stone2016) presents a comprehensive review of cephalopelvic relationships from evolutionary biology, palaeoanthropology, bioarchaeology, medical anthropology and biomedical approaches. She argues that the OD theory shifts the focus away from physiological and cultural components that have evolved in concert with bipedalism that ensure safe birth for the baby and maternal outcome (Stone, Reference Stone2016). She argues that the medical risk for women today, and we would argue in some historic periods, is tied to biomedical management of birth and the pregnant woman's body, and structural inequalities (Martin, Reference Martin2001) that women may face today, including poverty, lack of education and poor nutrition.

4.8. What can human evolution studies draw from bioarchaeology?

By drawing on bioarchaeological frameworks of care, palaeoanthropologists may gain a deeper appreciation of the health and social repercussions of care practices and the social and cultural environments on mothers, babies and others in the past. Tilley's model of care has been applied to Neanderthal individuals (Spikins et al., Reference Spikins, Needham, Tilley and Hitchens2018), but has not been considered broadly in the context of care for infants and children in palaeoanthropology. As mentioned above, our model to investigate infant and child care in the past considers variables including maternal and infant health (palaeopathology) and mortality, infant feeding practices, fertility, family and social structure, and population size, and supplies a useful framework in this regard (Halcrow, Reference Halcrow, Gowland and Halcrow2020).

Fitting with the nature of palaeoanthropological finds of single or small numbers of individuals, there has been some shift of focus in bioarchaeology and palaeopathology from cemetery population-level evidence for health and disease to detailed contextualised osteobiographies through a Bioarchaeology of Individuals approach to complement population-level analysis (Hosek & Robb, Reference Hosek and Robb2019; Stodder & Palkovich, Reference Stodder and Palkovich2012). Social bioarcheological approaches examine health and care through a contextualised approach that considers aspects of identity (age, sex, class, ethnicity) (Gowland & Thompson, Reference Gowland and Thompson2013), and some of these factors of identity may be applicable to the palaeoanthropological context.

Palaeoanthropology has some focus on palaeopathology (e.g. Trinkaus, Reference Trinkaus2018). However, there is scope for further work in this area to look at the evolution of disease within populations. It is important to look at children, in particular, as they carry a large disease burden within populations (Lewis, Reference Lewis2007, Reference Lewis2017; Halcrow & Tayles, Reference Halcrow and Tayles2008). Bioarchaeology has a main interest in major transitional events in the natural and cultural environments, such as climate change, the agricultural transition and how this affected past populations (Larsen, Reference Larsen2015; Robbins Schug, Reference Robbins Schug2020). Similarly, it is important to consider climate and social factors such as population demographics in the understanding of pathology and mortality in individuals within the palaeoanthropological context.

Some palaeoanthropological studies in the Upper Palaeolithic already explore aspects of mortuary behaviour to ascertain information on social responses to infant loss, familial relationships and social age (e.g. Einwögerer et al., Reference Einwögerer, Friesinger, Händel, Neugebauer-Maresch, Simon and Teschler-Nicola2006; Humphrey et al., Reference Humphrey, Freyne, van de Loosdrecht, Hogue, Turner, Barton and Bouzouggar2019; Nowell, Reference Nowell2020). Recently, there has been an increased research interest on grief and emotion from the archaeological context, and part of this research is starting to consider community members’ responses to infant and foetal death (e.g. Cannon & Cook, Reference Cannon and Cook2015; Le Roy & Murphy, Reference Le Roy, Murphy, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Murphy, Reference Murphy2011). The purported marginalisation of foetuses along with infants in the archaeological record, including location and simplified mortuary treatment, has led some scholars to interpret that they were of little concern beyond immediate family members (Cannon & Cook, Reference Cannon and Cook2015). Consideriation of evidence for intense grief after miscarriage and infant death starts to challenge the notion that their loss was of little consequence (Murphy, Reference Murphy2011).

Evolution anthropology (as well as bioarchaeology) could draw on a multitude of other research on infant care, breastfeeding, sleep, and infant and child socialisation research from social anthropology (including medical anthropology) and social archaeological approaches (Baxter, Reference Baxter2005; Gottlieb, Reference Yannopoulos, Lyberatos, Theodossiou, Li, Valipour, Tamburrino and Angelakis2004; Han, Reference Yannopoulos, Lyberatos, Theodossiou, Li, Valipour, Tamburrino and Angelakis2020; Palmquist, Reference Palmquist, Gowland and Halcrow2020; Tomori et al., Reference Yannopoulos, Lyberatos, Theodossiou, Li, Valipour, Tamburrino and Angelakis2018).

5. Conclusion

The investigation of care for infants in the past from multiple lenses offers an opportunity to begin to pry the doors open between evolutionary anthropology and bioarchaeology within Lancy's (Reference Lancy2012) Anthropology of Childhood house. Social aspects of infant and maternal care, alloparenting and infant health are central to evolutionary and bioarchaeological reconstructions of the human life course. Through an investigation of the human evolutionary past of the development of child care, and human biological and social development, we can contribute to a more holistic understanding of child care networks in the archaeological context. Similarly, the use of socially nuanced bioarchaeological theoretical frameworks of infant and child care within evolutionary anthropology may extend the understanding of health care and aspects of social structure for the deep past.

Acknowledgements

We thank the organisers of the Wenner Gren Foundation funded Symposium on Children and Innovation for the opportunity to contribute a version of the paper we presented. We received constructive feedback by Melanie Miller on a draft of this paper. Robbie McPhee assisted with some of the graphics.

Author contributions

Conceptulization and design of paper: SH, RW, GK, AN. Initial draft: SH. Editing and redrafting: SH, RW, GK and AN.

Financial support

No specific funding was linked to the research for this paper. We acknowledge the Wenner Gren Foundation for the funding of the symposium that stimulated and contributed to the development of this paper.

References

Abrams, E.T., & Rutherford, J. N. (2011). Framing Postpartum Hemorrhage as a Consequence of Human Placental Biology: An Evolutionary and Comparative Perspective. American Anthropologist, 113(3), 417430. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1433.2011.01351.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agarwal, S. C., & Glencross, B. A. (2011). Social bioarchaeology. Blackwell. DOI: 10.1002/9781444390537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2008). On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives, 2(2), 5965. DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00044.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alemseged, Z., Spoor, F., Kimbel, W. H., Bobe, R., Geraads, D., Reed, D., … Wynn, J. G. (2006). A juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature, 443(7109), 296301. DOI: 10.1038/nature05047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, J., & Samuels, A. (1992). Cost of maternal care: Infant-carrying in baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29(6), 391398. DOI: 10.1007/BF00170168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, D. J., & Osmond, C. (1986). Infant mortality, childhood nutrition, and ischaemic heart disease in England and Wales. Lancet, 10(1), 10771081. DOI: 10.1016/s0140-6736(86)91340-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baxter, J. (2005). The archaeology of childhood: Children, gender, and material culture. AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Bentley, G. R., Goldberg, T., & Jasienska, G. (1993). The fertility of agricultural and non-agricultural traditional societies. Population Studies, 47(2), 269281. DOI: 10.1080/0032472031000147006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blake, K. (2017). The Biology of the fetal period: Interpreting life from fetal skeletal remains. In Han, S., Betsinger, T. K. & Scott, A. B. (Eds.), The anthropology of the fetus: Biology, culture, and society (pp. 3458). Berghahn Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boas, F. (1912). Instability of human types. In Spiller, G. (Eds.), Papers on interracial problems (pp. 99103). Ginn.Google Scholar
Bogin, B., & Smith, B. H. (2012). Evolution of the human life cycle. In Stinson, S., Bogin, B., & O'Rourke, D. (Eds.), Human biology: An evolutionary and biocultural perspective (Vol. 8, 2nd ed., pp. 515586). Wiley.Google Scholar
Bogin, B., & Smith, H. (1996). Evolution of the human life cycle. American Journal of Human Biology, 8(6), 703716. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(1996)8:6<703::AID-AJHB2>3.0.CO;2-U3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogin, B., Varea, C., Hermanussen, M., & Scheffler, C. (2018). Human life course biology: A centennial perspective of scholarship on the human pattern of physical growth and its place in human biocultural evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 165(4), 834854. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.23357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolter, D. R., Elliott, M. C., Hawks, J., & Berger, L. R. (2020). Immature remains and the first partial skeleton of a juvenile Homo naledi, a late Middle Pleistocene hominin from South Africa. PLoS ONE, 15(4), e0230440. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0230440CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (1992). Reproductive decisions. In Smith, E. A. & Winterhalder, B. (Eds.), Evolutionary ecology and human behavior (pp. 339374). Aldine de Grutyer.Google Scholar
Bourke, C. D., Berkley, J. A., & Prendergast, A. J. (2016). Immune dysfunction as a cause and consequence of malnutrition. Trends in Immunology, 37(6), 386398. DOI: 10.1016/j.it.2016.04.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brabin, B.J., Romagosa, C., Abdelgalil, S., Menéndez, C., Verhoeff, F.H., McGready, R., … Ordi, J. (2004). The sick placenta – The role of malaria. Placenta, 25(5), 359378. DOI: 10.1016/j.placenta.2003.10.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandone, A. C., Stout, W., & Moty, K. (2020). Triadic interactions support infants’ emerging understanding of intentional actions. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12880. DOI: 10.1111/desc.12880CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buikstra, Jane E. (1977). Biocultural dimensions of archeological study: A regional perspective. In Blakey, R. L. (Ed.). Biocultural adaptation in prehistoric America. Proceedings of the Southern Anthropological Society 11 (pp. 6784). University of Georgia Press.Google Scholar
Cancian, F. M., & Oliker, S. J. (1999). Caring and gender. Sage.Google Scholar
Cannon, A., & Cook, K. (2015). Infant death and the archaeology of grief. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 25(2), 399416. DOI: 10.1017/S0959774315000049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cascio, C. J., Moore, D., & McGlone, F. (2019). Social touch and human development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 35, 511. DOI: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cofran, Z., & DeSilva, J. M. (2015). A neonatal perspective on Homo erectus brain growth. Journal of Human Evolution, 81, 4147. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.02.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, S., Hadley, D., & Shepherd, G. (2018). The Oxford handbook of the archaeology of childhood. Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199670697.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, H. (2005). Children and childhood in Western society since 1500. Pearson Education. DOI: 10.4324/9781003033165Google Scholar
Dávid-Barrett, T., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Bipedality and hair loss in human evolution revisited: The impact of altitude and activity scheduling. Journal of Human Evolution, 94, 7282. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2016.02.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeSilva, J. M. (2011). A shift toward birthing relatively large infants early in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(3), 10221027. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1003865108CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunsworth, H. M., Warrener, A. G., Deacon, T., Ellison, P. T., & Pontzer, H. (2012). Metabolic hypothesis for human altriciality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 109(38), 1521215216. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1205282109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einwögerer, T., Friesinger, H., Händel, M., Neugebauer-Maresch, C., Simon, U., & Teschler-Nicola, M. (2006). Upper Palaeolithic infant burials. Nature, 444(7117), 285. DOI: 10.1038/444285aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R. (2007). Parent–infant synchrony. Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Current Directions of Psychological Science, 16(6), 340345. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foley, R. (1995). Evolution and adaptive significance of hominid maternal behaviour. In Pryce, C. R., Martin, R. D. & Skuse, D. (Eds.), Motherhood in humans and nonhuman primates. 3rd Schultz-Biegert Symposium, Kartause Ittingen (pp. 2736). Karger.Google Scholar
Geber, J. (2005). Victims of Ireland's Great Famine: The bioarchaeology of mass burials at Kilkenny Union Workhouse. University Press of Florida. DOI: 10.5744/florida/9780813061177.001.0001Google Scholar
Gibson, MA, & Mace, R. (2006). An energy-saving development initiative increases birth rate and childhood malnutrition in rural Ethiopia. PLOS Medicine, 3(4), e87. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, A. (2004). The afterlife is where we come from: The culture of infancy in West Africa. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gowland, R. (2020). Ruptured: Reproductive loss, bodily boundaries, time and the life course in archaeology. In Gowland, R., & Halcrow, S. E. (Eds.), The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes (pp. 257274). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gowland, R. L. (2015). Entangled lives: Implications of the developmental origins of health and disease hypothesis for bioarchaeology and the life course. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 158(4): 530540. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.22820CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gowland, R. L., & Halcrow, S. (2020). The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes. Springer. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-27393-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gowland, R. L., & Knüsel, C. (2006). Social archaeology of funerary remains. Oxbow Books.Google Scholar
Gowland, R. L., & Thompson, T. J. U. (2013). Human identity and identification. Cambridge University Press. DOI: 10.1017/CBO9781139029988CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M., Stieglitz, J., Hooper, P. L., Gomes, C., & Kaplan, H. (2012). From the womb to the tomb: The role of transfers in shaping the evolved human life history. Experimental Gerontology, 47(10), 807813. DOI: 10.1016/j.exger.2012.05.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haig, D. (2019). Cooperation and conflict in human pregnancy. Current Biology, 29(11), R455R458.DOI:10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halcrow, S. E. (2020). Infants in the bioarchaeological past: Who cares? In Gowland, R. & Halcrow, S. E. (Eds.), The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes (pp. 1938). Springer. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-27393-4_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halcrow, S. E., & Tayles, N. (2008). The bioarchaeological investigation of childhood and social age: Problems and prospects. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 15, 190215. DOI: 10.1007/s10816-008-9052-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halcrow, S. E., & Tayles, N. (2010). The archaeological infant in biological and social context: A response to Mike Lally and Traci Ardren 2008. Little artefacts: Rethinking the constitution of the archaeological infant. Childhood in the Past, 3(1), 123130. DOI: 10.1179/cip.2010.3.1.123Google Scholar
Halcrow, S. E., Tayles, N., & Elliott, G. E. (2017). The bioarchaeology of fetuses. In Han, S., Betsinger, T. K., & Scott, A. B. (Eds.), The anthropology of the fetus: Biology, culture, and society (pp. 83111). Berghahn Books. DOI: 10.2307/j.ctvw04h7z.10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halcrow, S. E., & Ward, S. M. (2017). Bioarchaeology of childhood. In Montgomery, H. (Ed.), Oxford bibliographies: Childhood studies. Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/obo/9780199791231-0178Google Scholar
Halcrow, S. E., & Ward, S. M. (2018). Children in bioarchaeology and forensic anthropology. In Smith, C. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of global archaeology (2nd ed.). Springer. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-51726-1_143-2Google Scholar
Han, S. (2020). Mothering Tongues: Anthropological Perspectives on Language and the Mother-Infant Nexus. In Gowland, R., & Halcrow, S. E. (Eds.), The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes (pp. 145157). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harpending, H. C., Draper, P., & Pennington, R. (1990). Cultural evolution, parental care, and mortality. In Armelagos, G. J. & Swedlund, A. C. (Eds.), Disease in populations in transition: Anthropological and epidemiological perspectives (pp. 251265). Bergin & Garvey.Google Scholar
Herries, A. I. R., Martin, J. M., Leece, A. B., Adams, J. W., Boschian, G., Joannes-Boyau, R., … Menter, C. (2020). Contemporaneity of Australopithecus, Paranthropus, and early Homo erectus in South Africa. Science, 368 (6486), eaaw7293. DOI: 10.1126/science.aaw7293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertenstein, M. J. (2002). Touch: Its communicative functions in infancy. Human Development, 45(2), 7094. DOI: 10.1159/000048154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, K. (1993). Life history theory and evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 2(3), 7888. DOI: 10.1002/evan.1360020303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodson, C., & Gowland, R. L. (2020). Like mother, like child: Investigating perinatal and maternal health stress in Post-medieval London. In Gowland, R., & Halcrow, S. E. (Eds.), The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes (pp. 3964). Springer. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-27393-4_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Högberg, A., & Gärdenfors, P. (2015). Children, teaching and the evolution of humankind. Childhood in the Past, 8(2), 113121. DOI: 10.1179/1758571615Z.00000000033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosek, L., & Robb, J. (2019). Osteobiography: A platform for bioarchaeological research. Bioarchaeology International, 3(1): 115. DOI: 10.5744/bi.2019.1005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosfield, R. (2020). The earliest Europeans: A year in the life: Seasonal survival strategies in the Lower Palaeolithic. Oxbow. DOI: 10.2307/j.ctv138wsvqGoogle Scholar
Howell, N. (2010). Life histories of the Dobe !Kung. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, L., Freyne, A., van de Loosdrecht, M., Hogue, J. T., Turner, E., Barton, N., & Bouzouggar, A. (2019). Infant funerary behavior and kinship in Pleistocene hunter–gatherers from Morocco. Journal of Human Evolution, 135, 102637-102637. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2019.07.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglis, R., & Halcrow, S. E. (2018). The bioarchaeology of childhood: Theoretical development in the field. In Beauchesne, P., & Agarwal, S. C. (Eds.), Children and childhood in bioarchaeology (pp. 3360). University of Florida Press.Google Scholar
Johnston, F. (1962). Growth of the long bones of infants and young children at Indian Knoll. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 20(3), 249254. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.1330200309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamp, K. A. (2001). Where have all the children gone?: The archaeology of childhood. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 8, 134. DOI: 10.1023/A:1009562531188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konner, M. (2005). Hunter-gatherer infancy and childhood: The !Kung and others. In Hewlett, B. S. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Hunter–gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 1964). Routledge.Google Scholar
Konner, M. (2010). The evolution of childhood: Relationships, emotion, mind. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kramer, K. L., & Boone, J. L. (2002). Why intensive agriculturalists have higher fertility: A household energy budget approach. Current Anthropology, 43(3), 511517. DOI: 10.1086/340239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kushnick, G. (2009). Parental supply and offspring demand amongst Karo Batak mothers and children. Journal of Biosocial Science, 41(2), 183193. 10.1017/S0021932008002988CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushnick, G. (2010). Resource competition and reproduction in Karo Batak villages. Human Nature, 21(1), 6281. DOI: 10.1007/s12110-010-9082-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kushnick, G. (2020). The cradle of humankind: Evolutionary approaches to technology and parenting. In Weekes-Shackelford, V. A. & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology and parenting. Oxford University Press (SocArxiv Preprint. DOI: 10.31235/osf.io/k23md), in press.Google Scholar
Lancaster, J. B., & Lancaster, C. S. (1987). The watershed: Change in parental-investment and family-formation strategies in the course of human evolution. In Altmann, J., Rossi, A. S., & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Parenting across the lifespan: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 187205). Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2012). Why Anthropology of Childhood? A brief history of an emerging discipline. AnthropoChildren, 1. DOI: 10.1093/OBO/9780199791231-0002Google Scholar
Larsen, C. (2015). Bioarchaeology: Interpreting behaviour through the human skeleton (2nd ed.). Cambridge Series on Biological and Evolutionary Anthropology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lefèber, Y., & Voorhoeve, H. W. A. (1998). Indigenous customs in childbirth and child care. Van Gorcum.Google Scholar
Le Roy, M., & Murphy, E. (2020). Archaeothanatology as a tool for interpreting death during pregnancy: A proposed methodology using examples from medieval Ireland. In Gowland, R. & Halcrow, S. E. (Eds.), The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes (pp. 221234). Springer.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. E. (2007). The bioarchaeology of childhood: Perspectives from biological and forensic anthropology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. E. (2017). Paleopathology of children: Identification of pathological conditions in the human skeletal remains of non-adults. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, A. F., & Slade, A. (2000). Parenting toddlers: Developmental and clinical considerations. In Osofsky, J. D. & Fitzgerald, H. E. (Eds.), WAIMH handbook of infant mental health: Parenting and child care (Vol. 3, pp. 2565). John Wiley.Google Scholar
Lillehammer, G. (1989). A child is born. The child's world in an archaeological perspective. Norwegian Archaeological Review, 22(2), 89105. DOI: 10.1080/00293652.1989.9965496CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lillehammer, G. (2015). 25 Years with the ‘child’ and the archaeology of childhood. Childhood in the Past, 8(2), 7886. DOI: 10.1179/1758571615Z.00000000030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mace, R., & Sear, R. (2005). Are humans communal breeders? In Voland, E., Chasiotis, A., & Schiefenhoevel, W. (Eds.), Grandmotherhood – the evolutionary significance of the second half of female life (pp. 143159). Rutgers.Google Scholar
Martin, E. (2001). Women in the body: A cultural analysis of reproduction. Penguin Random House.Google Scholar
Mattori, E. M., Armentrout, L. L., Larsen, C. S., & Hutchinson, D. L. (1987). An ethnohistoric infant burial from Western Nevada. Contributions to the Study of Cultural Resources, Technical Report No. 16. Nevada Bureau of Land Management.Google Scholar
Mays, S., Gowland, R., Halcrow, S. E., & Murphy, E. (2017). Child bioarchaeology: Perspectives on the past 10 years. Childhood in the Past, 10(1), 3856. DOI: 10.1080/17585716.2017.1301066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDade, T. W. (2003). Life history theory and the immune system: Steps toward a human ecological immunology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 122(S32), 100125. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.10398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, M. J., Dong, Y., Pechenkina, K., Fan, W., & Halcrow, S. E. (2020). Raising girls and boys in early China: Stable isotope data reveal sex differences in weaning and childhood diets during the eastern Zhou era. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 17(2), 567585. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.24033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montagu, A. (1986). Touching: The significance of the human skin. Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Murphy, E. M. (2011). Children's burial grounds in Ireland (Cilliní) and parental emotions toward infant death. International Journal of Historical Archaeology, 15, 409428. DOI: 10.1007/s10761-011-0148-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D., Gibson, M. A., Lawson, D. A., & Sear, R. (2013), Human behavioral ecology: Current research and future prospects. Behavioral Ecology, 24(5), 10311040. DOI: 10.1093/beheco/ars222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. (2020). Reconsidering the personhood of infants in the Gravettian. Journal of Anthropological Research, 76(2), 232250. DOI: 10.1086/708395CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. (2016). Play and the evolution of cultural capacity in Neandertals. In Haidle, M.., Conard, N. & Bolus, M. (Eds.), The nature of culture. Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology series (pp. 8797). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A., & Kurki, H. (2020). Moving beyond the obstetrical dilemma hypothesis: Birth, weaning and infant care in the Plio-Pleistocene. In Gowland, R., & Halcrow, S. E. (Eds.), The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes (pp. 173190). Springer. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-27393-4_10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmquist, A. (2020). Cooperative lactation and the mother–infant nexus. In Gowland, R., & Halcrow, S. E. (Eds.), The mother–infant nexus in anthropology: Small beginnings, significant outcomes (pp. 125–124). Springer. DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-27393-4_7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, G. A., Royle, N. J., & Hartley, I. R. (2002). Intrafamilial conflict and parental investment: A synthesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 357(1419), 295307. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0950CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piantadosi, S. T., & Kidd, C. (2016). Extraordinary intelligence and the care of infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 68746879. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1506752113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomeroy, E., Stock, J. T., Zakrzewski, S. R., & Lahr, M. M. (2013). A metric study of three types of artificial cranial modification from North-Central Peru. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology, 20(3), 317334. DOI: 10.1002/oa.1044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powell, L., Southwell-Wright, W., & Gowland, R. (2016). Care in the past: Archaeological and interdisciplinary perspectives. Oxbow Books.Google Scholar
Quinlan, R. J. (2007). Human parental effort and environmental risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1606), 121125. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2006.3690CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinlan, R. J. (2010). Extrinsic mortality effects on reproductive strategies in a Caribbean community. Human Nature, 21(2), 124139. DOI: 10.1007/s12110-010-9085-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins Schug, G. (Ed.) (2020). The Routledge handbook of the bioarchaeology of climate and environmental change. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sear, R. (2015) Beyond the nuclear family: an evolutionary perspective on parenting. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 98103. DOI: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shennan, S. (2011). Property and wealth inequality as cultural niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1566), 918926. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, S. W., Quade, J., Levin, N. E., Butler, R., Dupont-Nivet, G., Everett, M., … Semaw, S. (2008). A female Homo erectus pelvis from Gona, Ethiopia. Science, 322(5094), 10891092. DOI: 10.1126/science.1163592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, P. K. (2010). Children and play. Wiley-Blackwell. DOI:10.1002/9781444311006Google Scholar
Sofaer, J. (2006). The body as material culture: A theoretical osteoarchaeology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spikins, P., Needham, A., Tilley, L., & Hitchens, G. (2018). Calculated or caring? Neanderthal healthcare in social context. World Archaeology: The Archaeology of Medicine and Healthcare, 50(3), 384403. DOI: 10.1080/00438243.2018.1433060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stodder, A. L. W., & Palkovich, A. M. (2012). The bioarchaeology of individuals. University Press of Florida. DOI: 10.5744/florida/9780813038070.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, P. K. (2016). Biocultural perspectives on maternal mortality and obstetrical death from the past to the present. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 159(61), S150S171. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.22906CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tilley, L. (2015). Theory and practice in the bioarchaeology of care. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilley, L., & Nystrom, K. (2019). A ‘cold case’ of care: Looking at old data from a new perspective in mummy research. International Journal of Paleopathology, 25, 7281. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpp.2018.08.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tilley, L., & Oxenham, M. (2011). Survival against the odds: Modeling the social implications of care provision to seriously disabled individuals. International Journal of Paleopathology, 1(1), 3542. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpp.2011.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tilley, L., & Schrenk, A. A. (2017). New developments in the bioarchaeology of care: Further case studies and expanded theory. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobias, P. V. (1984). The child from Taung. Science, 84(5), 99101.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121125. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomori, C., Palmquist, A. E. L., & Quinn, E. A. (2018). Breastfeeding: New anthropological approaches (1st ed.). Routledge.Google Scholar
Tomori, C., Palmquist, A. E. L., & Quinn, E. A. (2017). Breastfeeding: New anthropological approaches (1st ed.). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevathan, W. A. (1987). Human birth: An evolutionary perspective. Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Trinkaus, E. (2018). An abundance of developmental anomalies and abnormalities in Pleistocene people. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(47), 1194111946; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1814989115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Washburn, S. L. (1960). Tools and human evolution. Scientific American, 203, 6375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, J. C., Desilva, J. M., & Stock, J. T. (2012). The obstetric dilemma: An ancient game of Russian roulette, or a variable dilemma sensitive to ecology? American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 149(S55), 4071. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.22160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: Human behavioural ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 5172. DOI: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(2000)9:2<51::AID-EVAN1>3.0.CO;2-73.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization, UNICEF, UNFPA, World Bank Group and the United Nations Population Division (2015). Trends in maternal mortality: 1990 to 2015. Estimates by WHO, UNICEF, UNFPA, World Bank Group and the United Nations Population Division. WHO reference number: WHO /RHR/15.23. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/reproductivehealth/publications/monitoring/maternal-mortality-2015/en/Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2019). Children: Reducing mortality. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/children-reducing-mortality#_ftnref1Google Scholar
Yannopoulos, S. I., Lyberatos, G., Theodossiou, N., Li, W., Valipour, M., Tamburrino, A., … Angelakis, A. (2015). Evolution of water-lifting devices (pumps) over the centuries worldwide. Water, 9, 50315060. DOI: 10.3390/w7095031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Schematic of relationship between bioarchaeology and evolutionary anthropology.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Bioarchaeological model of infant and childcare in the past.