Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T22:01:54.439Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are late-onset eating disorders in the elderly really the more common variant? Concerns around publication bias

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2010

James Main
Affiliation:
Redwood Ward, St Charles Hospital, London, U.K. Email: jamesmain@nhs.net
Leena Reddy
Affiliation:
Central and North West London NHS Foundation Trust, North Westminster CMHT, Latimer House, London, U.K.
Maja Lazarevic
Affiliation:
Central and North West London NHS Foundation Trust, North Westminster CMHT, Latimer House, London, U.K.
Paul J. Whelan
Affiliation:
Central and North West London NHS Foundation Trust, North Westminster CMHT, Latimer House, London, U.K.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Lapid et al. (2010) provide a fascinating insight into the much over-looked problem of eating disorders (EDs) in older adults and highlight the fact that an ED is often not considered in our differential when assessing elderly patients.

Type
Letters
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2010

Lapid et al. (Reference Lapid, Prom, Burton, McAlpine, Sutar and Rummans2010) provide a fascinating insight into the much over-looked problem of eating disorders (EDs) in older adults and highlight the fact that an ED is often not considered in our differential when assessing elderly patients.

However, we have some concerns over the suggestion that the majority of EDs in older adults are new-onset (69% of cases in the paper by Lapid et al.) rather than a recurrence or continuation from earlier in life. This seems a surprising finding as intuitively we feel that cases of EDs in old age are more likely to be in the context of pre-existing undetected symptoms or a recurrence rather than de novo. We briefly describe a case of early onset anorexia nervosa (AN) carried forward into old age, which we think is more representative of the true presentation of EDs in the elderly. The patient provided written informed consent to publication.

Mrs D. is a 75-year-old patient with a diagnosis of AN, restrictive type, with onset in early life. This was in the context of ongoing family relationship difficulties since her childhood years and both obsessive and perfectionist personality traits. She did well with psychological therapy and was discharged. The patient re-presented in 2007 with depressive symptoms and a BMI of 14.8, against the background of an ongoing difficult relationship with her daughter.

She would accept only energy drinks and small amounts of liquidized meals. No other physical health issues were identified. She is currently receiving input from a family therapist and a community dietician in addition to medical and psychiatric input from primary care and community psychiatry.

If the findings of Lapid et al. (Reference Lapid, Prom, Burton, McAlpine, Sutar and Rummans2010) are representative of the true picture, one potential explanation could be that smaller numbers of pre-existing/ recurrent EDs in old age are seen due to the high mortality rate in AN. However, this would not account for the similarly higher proportion of apparently new-onset cases they find of Bulimia Nervosa which does not carry a similar risk of mortality (though numbers are small, n = 5). That is to say, one would expect a higher prevalence of pre-existing/ recurrent BN than de novo cases, contrary to their finding.

We suggest that, instead, the study by Lapid et al. (Reference Lapid, Prom, Burton, McAlpine, Sutar and Rummans2010) is subject to both powerful publication bias and further biases within each case study. New-onset ED in an older age patient is surprising and represents a more desirable subject for publication than a known pre-existing diagnosis in an aging patient. Hence the collection of case studies will tend to over-represent apparent “new-onset” EDs.

A second problem is with the diagnosis of “new-onset” EDs in patients at the end of their life without robust data regarding their adolescence. There will be problems with recall bias and with the reliability and validity of diagnoses and clinical information from early life in case studies of older adults ranging from 1936 to 2008 when operational criteria for the diagnosis of EDs first emerged in 1980 (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association, 1980). Diagnostic practice will vary greatly over a patient's life time and more so at times when there are no clear criteria.

It is known that many potential cases of ED never present to services or receive treatment. Similarly, it is known that many patients move between diagnoses of AN, BN and atypical EDs (Fairburn and Harrison, Reference Fairburn and Harrison2003). Apparent new-onset AN could then be either an exacerbation of a pre-existing subclinical or unrecognized syndrome or conversion from atypical ED. Hence it is difficult to be confident in stating the absence of ED symptomatology in an older adult's past history.

The study by Lapid et al. (Reference Lapid, Prom, Burton, McAlpine, Sutar and Rummans2010) raises further questions of etiology. It has been observed that EDs run in families and that AN is highly heritable with twin concordances of MZ = 55% versus DZ = 5% (Fairburn and Harrison, Reference Fairburn and Harrison2003). It is difficult to understand why patients with a genetic predisposition to the compulsive and impulsive behaviors seen in EDs would develop a clinically significant syndrome later in life rather than in adolescence or early adulthood. If the genetic predisposition has always been there and is etiologically significant, why would a clinical ED not emerge earlier in life? It would be interesting to see family studies in patients with apparent new-onset ED in later life and whether they reflect the patterns of depression, obsessive and perfectionist personality traits and substance misuse seen in relatives of a proband with early onset ED.

Lapid et al. (Reference Lapid, Prom, Burton, McAlpine, Sutar and Rummans2010) suggest that AN could be “ushered in” by loss of appetite due to other medical conditions in this population. We would caution that whilst the symptom of anorexia is common in older adults due to both psychiatric and general medical conditions, this needs to be carefully distinguished from AN, the disorder. They suggest greater control over concerned relatives at a time when failing health threatens autonomy could be an explanation for new onset ED. However we need to be careful when delineating significant clinical syndromes from, for example, hunger strike expressed as concern over body weight. The latter would constitute a very different clinical entity to refusal to eat due to a fear of weight gain. Given the very different psychological and environmental stressors in later life and our observation regarding heritability, it may prove useful to regard new-onset EDs in older adults as a further subcategory of ED with potentially unique predisposing and precipitating factors.

Certainly there is room for further study on the actual prevalence of continued, recurrent and new-onset EDs in the older adult population. In addition, assessment of the characteristics of ED in this population in terms of family studies and response to treatment could prove very illuminating.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Fairburn, C. G. and Harrison, P. J. (2003). Eating disorders. The Lancet, 361, 407–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lapid, M. I., Prom, M. C., Burton, C., McAlpine, D. E., Sutar, B. and Rummans, T. A. (2010). Eating disorders in the elderly. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 523536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed