Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T16:38:05.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A survey of manual preference, skill and strength in undergraduates

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 June 2014

Caomhghin S Breathnach
Affiliation:
Department of Human Anatomy and Physiology, University College Dublin, Earlsfort Terrace, Dublin 2, Ireland

Abstract

Objective: To examine hand preference, hand skill and hand strength in university undergraduates in order to determine the most reliable index of handedness.

Method: The Edinburgh Inventory, a group test of handedness by marking dots in circles, and a standard dynamometer were used with 248 (148 female, 100 male) subjects.

Results: No statistically significant correlations were found between hand skill and strength, or between hand preference and strength. A significant correlation between skill and preference was found in the male right-handed group of 93 students.

Conclusion: It is not possible to demonstrate whether preference or skill is the better index of handedness; a standardised method for assessment of hemispheric dominance for handedness is required; in the meantime the criterion used in a specific study should be explicitly stated in any citation of that study.

Type
Original Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1.Oldfield, RC. The assessment of analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971; 9: 97113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Annett, M. Five tests of hand skill. Cortex 1992; 28: 583600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Tapley, SM, Bryden, MP. A group test for the assessment of performance between the hands. Neuropsychologia 1985; 23: 215–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.McManus, IC. The inheritance of left-handedness. In: Bock, GR, Marsh, J, editors. Biological asymmetry and handedness. CIBA Foundation Symposium 162. Chichester: Wiley, 1991: 257–67.Google Scholar
5.Davis, A, Annett, M. Handedness as a function of twinning, age and sex. Cortex 1994; 30: 105–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Bryden, MP. Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia 1977; 15:617–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Provins, KA, Cunliffe, P. The reliability of some motor performance tests of handedness. Neuropsychologia 1972; 10: 199206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Porac, S, Coren, C. Lateral preferences and human behaviour. New York: Springer, 1981.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Steenhuis, RE, Bryden, MP. Different dimensions of hand preference that relate to skilled and unskilled activities. Cortex 1989; 25: 289304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Todor, JI, Doane, T. Handedness classification: preference versus proficiency. Percept Motor Skills 1977; 45: 1041–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Bryden, MP, Singh, M, Steenhuis, RE, Clarkson, KL. A behavioral measure of hand preference as opposed to hand skill. Neuropsychologia 1994; 32: 991–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Provins, KA. Motor skills, handedness and behaviour. Amer J Psychol 1967; 19: 137–50.Google Scholar
13.Provins, KA, Cunliffe, P. The relationship between EEG activity and handedness. Cortex 1972; 8: 136–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Annett, M. The growth of manual preference and speed. Brit J Psychol 1970; 61: 545–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Woo, TL, Pearson, K. Dextrality and sinistrality of hand and eye. Biometrika. 1927; 19: 165–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16.Nathan, PW, Smith, MC, Deacon, P. The corticospinal tracts in man. Brain 1990; 113: 303–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Schmeid, A, Vedel, J-P, Pagni, S. Human spinal lateralisation assessed from motoneurone synchronisation dependent on handedness and motor unit type. J Physiol 1994; 480: 369–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18.Tan, U. Spinal motor lateralisation assessed by recovery curve of H reflex from wrist flexors in right and left handed normal subjects. International J Neuroscience 1989; 64: 1347–51.Google Scholar
19.Yakovlev, PJ, Rakic, P. Patterns of decussation of bulbar pyramids and distribution of pyramidal tracts on two sides of the spinal cord. Transactions of the American Neurological Association 1966; 91: 366–7.Google Scholar
20.White, LE, Lucas, G, Richards, A, Purves, D. Cerebral asymmetry and handedness. Nature 1994; 368: 197–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Nativ, A, Pratt, H, Allard, F, Frank, JS. Brain correlates of hand dominance: the association between peripheral and cerebral asymmetries revisited. Cortex 1992; 28: 435–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Boecker, H, Kleinschmidt, A, Requardt, M, Hanicke, W, Merboldt, KD, Frahm, J. Functional cooperativity of human cortical motor areas during self-paced simple finger movements: a high-resolution MRI study. Brain 1994; 117: 1231–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Kim, SG, Ashe, J, Hendrick, K, Ellermann, JM, Merkle, H, Ugurbil, K, Georgopoulos, AP. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. Science 1993; 261: 615–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Kawashima, R, Roland, PE, O'Sullivan, BT. Activity in the human primary motor cortex related to ipsilateral hand movements. Brain Research 1994; 663: 251–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Gandevia, SC, Burke, D. Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements? Behav Brain Sci 1993; 15:614–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26.Jeannerod, M. The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav Brain Sci 1994; 17: 187245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar