Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Modeling Intelligibility of Written Germanic Languages: Do We Need to Distinguish Between Orthographic Stem and Affix Variation?

  • Wilbert Heeringa (a1), Femke Swarte (a1), Anja Schüppert (a1) and Charlotte Gooskens (a1)

Abstract

We measured orthographic differences between five Germanic languages. First, we tested the hypothesis that orthographic stem variation among languages does not correlate with orthographic variation in inflectional affixes. We found this hypothesis true when considering the aggregated stem and affix distances between the languages. We also correlated the stem and affix distances within the cognate pairs in each language pair. We found low correlations, the lowest of them being not significant. Second, we tested the hypothesis that orthographic stem variation among languages is larger than orthographic variation in inflectional affixes. This hypothesis was also found to be true. Orthographic distance is likely to be a potential predictor of written intelligibility, but our results suggest that when modeling written intelligibility, a distinction needs to be made between stem and affix distances.*

  • View HTML
    • Send article to Kindle

      To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

      Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

      Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

      Modeling Intelligibility of Written Germanic Languages: Do We Need to Distinguish Between Orthographic Stem and Affix Variation?
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Dropbox

      To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

      Modeling Intelligibility of Written Germanic Languages: Do We Need to Distinguish Between Orthographic Stem and Affix Variation?
      Available formats
      ×

      Send article to Google Drive

      To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

      Modeling Intelligibility of Written Germanic Languages: Do We Need to Distinguish Between Orthographic Stem and Affix Variation?
      Available formats
      ×

Copyright

Corresponding author

University of Groningen, Faculty of Arts, Applied Linguistics, P.O. Box 716, 9700AS Groningen, The Netherlands, [w.j.heeringa@rug.nl], [f.h.e.swarte@rug.nl], [a.schueppert@rug.nl], [c.s.gooskens@rug.nl]

References

Hide All
Baroni, Marco, Johannes, Matiasek, & Harald, Trost. 2002. Unsupervised discovery of morphologically related words based on orthographic and semantic similarity. Proceedings of the Workshop on Morphological and Phonological Learning of ACL/SIGPHON-2002, ed. by Maxwell, Mike, 4857. East Stroudsburg, PA: ACL.
Bauer, Laurie. 2003. Introducing linguistic morphology. 2nd edn. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Baugh, Albert C., & Thomas, Cable. 1978. History of the English language. 3rd edn. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
Bezooijen, Renée van, & Charlotte, Gooskens. 2005. How easy is it for speakers of Dutch to understand spoken and written Frisian and Afrikaans, and why? Linguistics in the Netherlands 22, ed. by Doetjes, Jenny & van de Weijer, Jeroen, 1324. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Bonnet, Eric, & Yves Van de, Peer. 2002. zt: a software tool for simple and partial Mantel tests. Journal of Statistical Software 7. 112. Available at http://www.jstatsoft.org/.
Bourassa, Derrick C., & Rebecca, Treiman. 2008. Morphological constancy in spelling: A comparison of children with dyslexia and typically developing children. Dyslexia 14. 155169.
Wiebke, Brockhaus, 1995. Final devoicing in the phonology of German. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.
Campbell, Lyle. 2013. Historical linguistics. An introduction. 3rd edn. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cronbach, Lee J. 1951. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 16. 297334.
Daan, Jo, & Dirk, Peter Blok. 1969. Van randstad tot landrand (Bijdragen en Mededelingen der Dialectencommissie van de Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen te Amsterdam 37). Amsterdam: N.V. Noord-Hollandsche uitgevers maatschappij.
De Schutter, Georges, Boudewijn van den, Berg, Goeman, Ton, & Thera de, Jong. 2005. Morphological atlas of the Dutch dialects, vol. I. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
Doetjes, Gerke, & Charlotte, Gooskens. 2009. Skriftsprogets rolle i den dansk-svenske talesprogsforståelse. Språk och Stil 19. 105123.
Drewnowski, Adam, & Alice, F. Healy. 1980. Missing -ing in reading: Letter detection errors on word endings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19. 247262.
Fernanda, Ferreira, & Henderson, John M.. 1991. Recovery from misanalyses of garden-path sentences. Journal of Memory and Language 31. 725745.
Fowler, Carol A., Sherley, E. Napps, & Laurie, B. Feldman. 1985. Relations among regular and irregular morphologically related words in the lexicon as revealed by repetition priming. Memory and Cognition 13. 241255.
Frazier, Lynn, & Keith, Rayner. 1982. Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology 14. 178210.
Goebl, Hans, with assistance of Wolf-Dieter Rase & Hilmar Pudlatz. 1982. Dialektometrie: Prinzipien und Methoden des Einsatzes der numerischen Taxonomie im Bereich der Dialektgeographie (Philosophisch-Historische Klasse Denkschriften 157). Vienna: Verlag der österreichischen Akademie der Wissenschaften.
Goebl, Hans, with assistance of Siegfried Selberherr, Wolf-Dieter Rase & Hilmar Pudlatz. 1984. Dialektometrische Studien. Anhand italoromanischer, rätoromanischer und galloromanischer Sprachmaterialien aus AIS und ALF (Beihefte zur Zeitschrift für Romanische Philologie 191, 192, 193). Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.
Goebl, Hans. 1993. Probleme und Methoden der Dialektometrie: Geolinguistik in globaler Perspektive. Proceedings of the International Congress of Dialectologists 1, ed. by Viereck, Wolfgang, 3781. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner Verlag.
Goeman, Ton, van Oostendorp, Marc van Reenen, Pieter, Oele, Koornwinder, van den Berg, Boudewijn, & van Reenen, Anke . 2009. Morphological atlas of the Dutch dialects, vol. II. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
Hawkins, John A., & Anne, Cutler. 1988. Psycholinguistic factors in morphological asymmetry. Explaining language universals, ed. by Hawkins, John A., 280317. Oxford: Blackwell.
Heeringa, Wilbert. 2004. Measuring dialect pronunciation differences using Levenshtein distance. Groningen, The Netherlands: University of Groningen dissertation.
Heeringa, Wilbert, Martijn, WielingBoudewijn van den, BergNerbonne, John 2009. A quantitative examination of variation in Dutch Low Saxon morphology. Low Saxon dialects across borders/Niedersächsiche dialekte über grenzen hinweg (ZDL-Beiheft 138), ed. by Lenz, Alexandra N., Gooskens, Charlotte, & Reker, Siemon, 195216. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner Verlag.
Heeringa, Wilbert, & Frans, Hinskens. 2014. Convergence between dialect varieties and dialect groups in the Dutch language area. Aggregating dialectology, typology, and register analysis: Linguistic variation in text and speech. (Linguae et Litterae: Publications of the School of Language and Literature, Freiburg Institute for Advanced Studies 28), ed. by Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt & Wälchli, Bernhard, 2652 and 452–453. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter.
Hilton, Nanna HaugCharlotte, Gooskens, & Anja, Schüppert. 2013. The influence of non-native morphosyntax on the intelligibility of a closely related language. Lingua 137. 118.
Inoue, Fumio. 1996. Computational dialectology (2). Area and Culture Studies 53. 115134.
Jain, Anil K., & Dubes, Richard C.. 1988. Algorithms for clustering data. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Jarvella, Robert J., & Meijers, Guust. 1983. Recognizing morphemes in spoken words: Some evidence for a stem-organized mental lexicon. The process of language understanding, ed. by Jarvella, Robert J. & d'Arcais, Giovanni B. FloresChichester: John Wiley.
Joseph, Holly S. S. L., & Simon, P. Liversedge. 2013. Children's and adults’ online processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences during reading. PLoS ONE 8. 113.
Kruskal, Joseph B. 1999. An overview of sequence comparison. Time warps, string edits, and macromolecules. The theory and practice of sequence comparison, ed. by Sankoff, David & Kruskal, Joseph B., 1–44. 2nd edn. Stanford, CA: CSLI.
Vladimir, Levenshtein, 1966. Binary codes capable of correcting deletions, insertions, and reversals. Cybernetics and Control Theory 10. 707710.
Nathan, Mantel, 1967. The detection of disease clustering and a generalized regression approach. Cancer Research 27. 209220.
Matthews, Peter H. 1991. Morphology. 2nd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hugo van der, Molen, & Morton, John. 1979. Remembering plurals: Unit of coding and form of coding during serial recall. Cognition 7. 3547.
Nunnally, Jum C. 1978. Psychometric theory. 2nd edn. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Lee, Osterhout, & Holcomb, Phillip J.. 1992. Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language 31. 785–780.
Jean, Séguy, 1971. La relation entre la distance spatiale et la distance lexicale. Revue de Linguistique Romane 35. 335357.
Séguy, Jean. 1973. La dialectométrie dans l'Atlas linguistique de la Gascogne. Revue de Linguistique Romane 37. 124.
Smith, Philip T., & Sterling, Christopher M.. 1982. Factors affecting the perceived morphological structure of written words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 21. 704721.
Sokal, Robert R., & Rohlf, F. James. 1962. The comparison of dendrograms by objective methods. Taxon 11. 3340.
Stanners, Robert F., Neiser, James J., Hernon, William P., & Hall, Roger. 1979. Memory representation for morphologically related words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18. 399412.
Venezky, Richard L. 2004. In search of the perfect orthography. Written Language and Literacy 7. 139163.
Zhiyong, Zhang, & Yuan, Ke-Hai. 2013. Robust Cronbach's alpha with missing and non-normal data. Software package description in R.
Nicholas, Zulu, Botha, Gerrit, & Barnard, Etienne. 2008. Orthographic measures of language distances between the official South African languages. Literator 29. 185204.

Related content

Powered by UNSILO

Modeling Intelligibility of Written Germanic Languages: Do We Need to Distinguish Between Orthographic Stem and Affix Variation?

  • Wilbert Heeringa (a1), Femke Swarte (a1), Anja Schüppert (a1) and Charlotte Gooskens (a1)

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.