Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T15:43:21.462Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Caloric stimulation in neglect: Evaluation of response as a function of neglect type

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 February 2004

JOHN C. ADAIR
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of New Mexico and Albuquerque VA Medical Center, Albuquerque, New Mexico
DUK L. NA
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Sung Kyun Kwan University College of Medicine and Samsung Medical Center, Seoul, Korea
RONALD L. SCHWARTZ
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, 3 Hattiesburg Clinic, Hattiesburg, Mississippi
KENNETH M. HEILMAN
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, 3 Hattiesburg Clinic, Hattiesburg, Mississippi

Abstract

Contralesional neglect may be induced by either unawareness of contralesional stimuli (attentional neglect, AN) or failure to act in contralesional space (intentional neglect, IN). We examined whether contralesional cold caloric stimulation differentially affects AN versus IN. Patients with left-sided neglect (n = 16) from right-hemisphere lesions performed target cancellation and line bisection tasks. Using a video-based apparatus that reverses the right and left side of stimuli, patients with abnormal cancellation performance were divided into those with AN and those with IN. The 5 subjects with normal cancellation performance but rightward bisection bias were also separated into 2 neglect groups. Subjects performed cancellation or bisection tasks before and immediately after irrigation of the left auditory canal with ice water. Caloric stimulation induced brisk rightward nystagmus in all subjects. Subjects with AN cancelled more left-sided targets after stimulation than those with IN (p = .02). Whereas caloric stimulation significantly shifted bisection error leftward for both IN and AN groups (p < .0001), AN patients exhibited a greater magnitude of shift than the IN patients. While the basis for differential performance remains undefined, the data indicate that caloric stimulation influences AN more than IN. (JINS, 2003, 9, 983–988.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2003 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Albert, M.D. (1973). A simple test of neglect. Neurology, 23, 658664.Google Scholar
Bisiach, E. & Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14, 129133.Google Scholar
Bisiach, E., Geminiani, G., Berti, A., & Rusconi, M.L. (1990). Perceptual and premotor factors of unilateral neglect. Neurology, 40, 12781281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisiach, E., Rusconi, M.L., & Vallar, G. (1991). Remission of somatoparaphrenic delusion through vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 29, 10291031.Google Scholar
Bottini, G., Karnath, H.O., Vallar, G., Sterzi, R., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J., & Paulesu, E. (2001). Cerebral representations for egocentric space. Functional-anatomical evidence from caloric vestibular and neck vibration. Brain, 124, 11821196.Google Scholar
Brandt, T., Dieterich, M., & Danek, A. (1994). Vestibular cortex lesions affect the perception of verticality. Annals of Neurology, 35, 403412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, T., Bartenstein, P., Janek, A., & Deiterich, M. (1998). Reciprocal inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction. Visual motion stimulation deactivates parieto-insular vestibular cortex. Brain, 121, 17491758.Google Scholar
Cappa, S., Sterzi, R., Vallar, G., & Bisiach, E. (1987). Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 25, 775782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coslett, H.B., Bowers, D., Fitzpatrick, E., Haws, B., & Heilman, K.M. (1990). Directional hypokinesia and hemispatial inattention in neglect. Brain, 113, 475486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friberg, L., Olsen, T.S., Roland, P.E., Paulson, O.B., & Lassen, N.A. (1985). Focal increase of blood flow in the cerebral cortex of man during vestibular stimulation. Brain, 108, 609623.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grusser, O.J., Pause, M., & Schreiter, U. (1990). Vestibular neurons in the parieto-insular cortex of monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): Visual and neck receptor responses. Journal of Physiology (London), 430, 559583.Google Scholar
Heilman, K.M., Watson, R.T., & Valenstein, E. (1993). Neglect and related disorders. In K.M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 279336). New York: Oxford University Press.
Karnath, H.O. (1994). Subjective body orientation in neglect and the interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioception and vestibular stimulation. Brain, 117, 10011012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladavas, E. (1994). The role of visual attention in neglect: A dissociation between perceptual and directional motor neglect. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4, 155159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladavas, E., Menghini, G., & Umilta, C. (1994). A rehabilitation study of hemispatial neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 7595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesulam, M.M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121, 10131052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, A.D. & Harvey, M. (1994). Toward a taxonomy of spatial neglect. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4, 177181.Google Scholar
Na, D.L., Adair, J.C., Williamson, D.J.G., Schwartz, R.L., Haws, B., & Heilman, K.M. (1998). Dissociation of sensory-attentional from motor-intentional spatial neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 64, 331338.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. & Jasper, H. (1954). Epilepsy and the functional anatomy of the human brain. Boston: Little Brown.
Robertson, I.H. (1993). The relationship between lateralized and non-lateralized attention in unilateral neglect. In I.H. Robertson & J.C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 257275). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Robertson, I.H., Halligan, P.W., & Marshall, J.C. (1993). Prospects for the rehabilitation of unilateral neglect. In I.H. Robertson & J.C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 279292). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Rode, G., Charles, N., Perenin, M.T., Vighetto, A., Trillet, M., & Aimard, G. (1992). Partial remission of hemiplegia and somatoparaphrenia through vestibular stimulation in a case of unilateral neglect. Cortex, 29, 203208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubens, A.B. (1985). Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect. Neurology, 35, 10191024.Google Scholar
Schenkenberg, T., Bradford, D.C., & Ajax, E.T. (1980). Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment. Neurology, 30, 509517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storrie-Baker, H.J., Segalowitz, S.J., Black, S.E., McLean, J.A.G., & Sullivan, N. (1997). Improvement of hemispatial neglect with cold-water calorics: An electrophysiological test of the arousal hypothesis of neglect. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 394402.Google Scholar
Tegner, R. & Levander, M. (1991). Through a looking glass: A new technique to demonstrate directional hypokinesia in unilateral neglect. Brain, 114, 19431951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallar, G., Bottini, G., Rusconi, M.L., & Sterzi, R. (1993). Exploring somatosensory neglect by vestibular stimulation. Brain, 116, 7186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallar, G., Sterzi, R., Bottini, G., Cappa, S., & Rusconi, M.L. (1990). Temporary remission of left hemianesthesia after vestibular stimulation. A sensory neglect phenomenon. Cortex, 26, 123131.Google Scholar