Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T06:35:34.752Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association of parental substance misuse with offspring substance misuse and criminality: a genetically informed register-based study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2020

Antti Latvala*
Affiliation:
Institute of Criminology and Legal Policy, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Ralf Kuja-Halkola
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Brian M. D'Onofrio
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
Nitya Jayaram-Lindström
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Center for Psychiatry Research, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Henrik Larsson
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden School of Medical Sciences, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden
Paul Lichtenstein
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
*
Author for correspondence: Antti Latvala, E-mail: antti.latvala@helsinki.fi
Get access
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Genetically informed studies have provided mixed findings as to what extent parental substance misuse is associated with offspring substance misuse and antisocial behavior due to shared environmental and genetic factors.

Methods

We linked data from nationwide registries for a cohort of 2 476 198 offspring born in Sweden 1958–1995 and their parents. Substance misuse was defined as International Classification of Diseases diagnoses of alcohol/drug use disorders or alcohol/drug-related criminal convictions. Quantitative genetic offspring-of-siblings analyses in offspring of monozygotic and dizygotic twin, full-sibling, and half-sibling parents were conducted.

Results

Both maternal and paternal substance misuse were robustly associated with offspring substance misuse [maternal adjusted hazard ratio (aHR) = 1.83 (95% confidence interval (CI) 1.80–1.87); paternal aHR = 1.96 (1.94–1.98)] and criminal convictions [maternal aHR = 1.56 (1.54–1.58); paternal aHR = 1.66 (1.64–1.67)]. Additive genetic effects explained 42% (95% CI 25–56%) and 46% (36–55%) of the variance in maternal and paternal substance misuse, respectively, and between 36 and 44% of the variance in substance misuse and criminality in offspring. The associations between parental substance misuse and offspring outcomes were mostly due to additive genetic effects, which explained 54–85% of the parent-offspring covariance. However, both nuclear and extended family environmental factors also contributed to the associations, especially with offspring substance misuse.

Conclusions

Our findings from a large offspring-of-siblings study indicate that shared genetic influences mostly explain the associations between parental substance misuse and both offspring substance misuse and criminality, but we also found evidence for the contribution of environmental factors shared by members of nuclear and extended families.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Introduction

Alcohol and drug use disorders tend to run in families (Mellentin et al. Reference Mellentin, Brink, Andersen, Erlangsen, Stenager, Bjerregaard and Christiansen2016; Merikangas et al. Reference Merikangas, Stolar, Stevens, Goulet, Preisig, Fenton and Rounsaville1998). Offspring of parents with substance use disorders (SUDs) have an elevated risk of problematic substance use as compared to offspring of parents without SUDs (Lieb et al. Reference Lieb, Merikangas, Hofler, Pfister, Isensee and Wittchen2002; Walden, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Walden, Iacono and McGue2007). In addition, offspring of parents with SUDs are at increased risk for antisocial behavior (Hussong et al. Reference Hussong, Wirth, Edwards, Curran, Chassin and Zucker2007).

Several mechanisms have been proposed to mediate these risks. For example, individuals with a family history of SUDs have been found to differ from those without a family history in amygdalar, hippocampal, basal ganglia, and cerebellar volumes, as well as in brain responses related to inhibitory control (Cservenka, Reference Cservenka2016). Parental SUDs are also associated with offspring's lower general cognitive ability (Khemiri et al. Reference Khemiri, Larsson, Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Lichtenstein, Jayaram-Lindstrom and Latvala2020), poorer performance in specific cognitive domains such as working memory and executive functioning, higher impulsivity and risk-taking, and altered emotional reactivity (Cservenka, Reference Cservenka2016; Weinberg, Reference Weinberg1997). Further, a family history of alcohol use disorders is associated with a reduced subjective response to alcohol, leading to higher levels of consumption (Quinn & Fromme, Reference Quinn and Fromme2011).

Social learning effects of parental modeling could also contribute to the parent-offspring resemblance in substance use (Petraitis, Flay, & Miller, Reference Petraitis, Flay and Miller1995). Alcohol-related cognitions (i.e. alcohol-related knowledge, norms, and expectancies) are thought to be important for the development of alcohol use, but associations with parental alcohol use have been inconsistent (Voogt et al. Reference Voogt, Beusink, Kleinjan, Otten, Engels, Smit and Kuntsche2017). Environmental effects could also be indirect: children of parents with SUDs are at risk for various adverse exposures such as parental divorce early on in life, and substance use problems are likely to interfere with parenting and increase the likelihood of maltreatment or neglect (Hafekost et al. Reference Hafekost, Lawrence, O'Leary, Bower, O'Donnell, Semmens and Zubrick2017; Smith, Wilson, & Comm Subst Use, Reference Smith, Wilson and Comm Subst Use2016; Windle & Windle, Reference Windle and Windle2018). These factors might, in turn, increase the risk for substance use problems and antisocial behavior in the offspring (Sheridan, Reference Sheridan1995). Finally, affiliation with substance-using and antisocial peers could mediate the associations between parental SUDs and offspring substance use and antisocial behaviors (Bountress, Chassin, & Lemery-Chalfant, Reference Bountress, Chassin and Lemery-Chalfant2017; Henry, Fulco, Agbeke, & Ratcliff, Reference Henry, Fulco, Agbeke and Ratcliff2018; Mahedy et al. Reference Mahedy, MacArthur, Hammerton, Edwards, Kendler, Macleod and Heron2018).

Importantly, besides the question of the specific mediating mechanisms, the broader question remains as to what extent the intergenerational associations are due to genetic and environmental influences. While genetic differences explain 40–50% of individual differences in the risk of SUDs within generations (Dick, Prescott, & McGue, Reference Dick, Prescott and McGue2010; Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Benyamin, de Leeuw, Sullivan, van Bochoven, Visscher and Posthuma2015; Verhulst, Neale, & Kendler, Reference Verhulst, Neale and Kendler2015), the relative importance of genetic and environmental factors for intergenerational transmission of substance misuse is less clear. Specifically, while non-genetic factors shared by siblings typically explain 10–20% of the variance in SUD risk (Dick et al. Reference Dick, Prescott and McGue2010; Polderman et al. Reference Polderman, Benyamin, de Leeuw, Sullivan, van Bochoven, Visscher and Posthuma2015; Verhulst et al. Reference Verhulst, Neale and Kendler2015), it is not well known whether exposure to parental SUDs increases the risk for offspring SUDs beyond the contribution of genetic risk transmitted from parents to offspring. Besides a direct effect of the exposure, intergenerational associations may reflect shared genetic effects on the exposure and the outcome, formed by the parental and offspring phenotypes (Plomin, Defries, & Loehlin, Reference Plomin, Defries and Loehlin1977). This has been stressed in the context of parental SUDs, calling for genetically informed research designs (McGue, Reference McGue1997).

Effects of the rearing environment can be separated from genetic factors in adoption studies, which compare the resemblance of offspring with their biological and adoptive parents. Early adoption studies highlighted genetic effects on alcohol and drug abuse as well as antisocial behavior (Cadoret, Cain, & Grove, Reference Cadoret, Cain and Grove1980; Cadoret, Yates, Troughton, Woodworth, & Stewart, Reference Cadoret, Yates, Troughton, Woodworth and Stewart1995; Crowe, Reference Crowe1974; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Schulsinger, Moller, Hermansen, Winokur and Guze1974; Moffitt, Reference Moffitt1987). More recent analyses of families with biological and adoptive relatives as well as families with stepparents and biological parents who did not live with their offspring have suggested that both genetic and environmental factors contribute to the parent-offspring transmission of alcohol and drug misuse as well as antisocial behavior (Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist, & Sundquist, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015c; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Lonn, Morris, Sundquist, Langstrom and Sundquist2014, Reference Kendler, Ji, Edwards, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015a).

Adoption studies may, however, suffer from selection biases and other methodological limitations, which makes it necessary to utilize other designs to replicate findings. The Children-of-Twins (CoT) design compares offspring of monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin parents who are discordant for the exposure (D'Onofrio, Lahey, Turkheimer, & Lichtenstein, Reference D'Onofrio, Lahey, Turkheimer and Lichtenstein2013; McAdams et al. Reference McAdams, Neiderhiser, Rijsdijk, Narusyte, Lichtenstein and Eley2014, Reference McAdams, Hannigan, Eilertsen, Gjerde, Ystrom and Rijsdijk2018). Based on this contrast, genetic and environmental associations between parent and offspring phenotypes can be distinguished. A review of CoT studies found that previous studies of parental substance misuse have been conducted in relatively small samples derived from only two cohorts of twin parents (McAdams et al. Reference McAdams, Neiderhiser, Rijsdijk, Narusyte, Lichtenstein and Eley2014). Findings have been mixed, with some studies reporting no support for an environmental effect of parental substance misuse (Slutske et al. Reference Slutske, D'Onofrio, Turkheimer, Emery, Harden, Heath and Martin2008) while others have been unable to reliably separate genetic v. environmental effects (Duncan et al. Reference Duncan, Scherrer, Fu, Bucholz, Heath, True and Jacob2006, Reference Duncan, Sartor, Scherrer, Grant, Heath, Nelson and Bucholz2008). Similarly, CoT studies of parental substance misuse and offspring antisocial behavior have been inconclusive (Haber et al. Reference Haber, Bucholz, Jacob, Grant, Scherrer, Sartor and Heath2010; Waldron, Martin, & Heath, Reference Waldron, Martin and Heath2009).

The CoT design has recently been expanded into a more general offspring-of-siblings design where offspring of parents who are full or half-siblings are studied (Chang et al. Reference Chang, Lichtenstein, D'Onofrio, Almqvist, Kuja-Halkola, Sjolander and Larsson2014; Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, Reference Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Larsson and Lichtenstein2014; Latvala, Kuja-Halkola, Langstrom, & Lichtenstein, Reference Latvala, Kuja-Halkola, Langstrom and Lichtenstein2015; McAdams et al. Reference McAdams, Hannigan, Eilertsen, Gjerde, Ystrom and Rijsdijk2018). Importantly, this design can utilize much larger sample sizes compared to CoT studies and is expected to result in improved statistical power and representativeness.

The focus of the present study was to investigate genetic and environmental contributions to the associations between parental substance misuse and offspring outcomes. Specifically, we aimed first to describe individual-level associations of maternal and paternal substance misuse with substance misuse and criminality in the offspring, and then to estimate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors on the parent-offspring associations. We conducted quantitative genetic offspring-of-siblings analyses in a large Swedish nationwide register-based dataset and included criminality as an outcome in addition to substance misuse to also study the intergenerational association between parental substance misuse and offspring antisocial behavior.

Methods

Study population

We conducted a population-based cohort study by linking several Swedish longitudinal registers, using the unique personal identity number given to all Swedish citizens at birth and to immigrants upon arrival to Sweden as key. Data on substance misuse and criminality were available for the period 1973–2013. To maximize the sample size but minimize missing information due to left and right censoring, we included in the study population offspring born in Sweden to parents who were born between 1943 and 1983. We identified the sample from the Total Population Register (Ludvigsson et al. Reference Ludvigsson, Almqvist, Bonamy, Ljung, Michaelsson, Neovius and Ye2016), which provides links to biological parents for each individual born since 1932. Requiring the offspring to have a register-based follow-up at least until age 18 and non-missing information on both parents resulted in data for 2 476 198 individuals (51% men) in the offspring generation, who were born between 1958 and 1995.

The parent generation was further linked to their parents (i.e. grandparents of the offspring generation) in order to identify full siblings and maternal/paternal half-siblings in the parental generation. The Swedish Twin Registry was used to identify MZ and DZ twin parents. Thus, we could identify cousins in the offspring generation with different types of relationships. From these cousins, we extracted information for two different types of analyses: within-family and quantitative genetic analyses (see below). For the within-family analyses, we used 337 672 paternal and 341 680 maternal full cousins randomly selecting one offspring per nuclear family. We used two different samples also for the quantitative genetic analyses. We identified 744 679 extended families related through the father and then we randomly selected up to two full or half-siblings per nuclear family, resulting in 938 347 parents and 1 619 271 offspring included in the analysis (online Supplementary Tables S1 and S2). Similarly, 759 067 maternally related extended families including 958 773 parents and 1 662 574 offspring were studied (online Supplementary Tables S3 and S4). The samples used in the different analyses are summarized in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. Samples used for individual-level, within-family, and quantitative genetic analyses. All Ns refer to the offspring generation. The samples used in within-family analyses are subsamples of those used in the quantitative genetic analyses.

Measures

Substance misuse

We used an omnibus measure of substance misuse combining alcohol and drug-related medical diagnoses and criminal convictions. Diagnoses came from the National Patient Register (NPR) which contains details of all individual episodes of hospitalization in Sweden since 1973 and of outpatient treatments since 2001, and convictions were available from the National Crime Register for individuals aged 15 (age of criminal responsibility) and older since 1973. This measure captures a wide range of alcohol and drug-related events but does not include all individuals with substance misuse, as many of them are not registered in any medical or governmental registry. Similar measures have been used in earlier register-based studies of alcohol and drug misuse (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015c; Latvala, Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, Reference Latvala, Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Larsson and Lichtenstein2016). Substance misuse events were defined as having an International Classification of Diseases (ICD) diagnosis of mental and behavioral disorders due to alcohol or drugs (excluding tobacco) in the NPR (ICD-8 codes 291, 303, 304; ICD-9 codes 291, 303–305; ICD-10 codes F10-F16, F18, F19) or a court conviction of alcohol- or drug-related crime defined as (1) violations of the Narcotic Drugs Act which include possession of substances for personal use, supply, manufacture, and consumption, and (2) convictions of driving under the influence of alcohol and/or illicit substances. Substance misuse was defined identically for parents and offspring.

Criminality

Criminality in the offspring was defined as any conviction of violent crimes (i.e. murder, manslaughter, assault, kidnapping, illegal restraint, illegal coercion or threats, robbery, threats or violence against an officer, arson, gross violation of a person's integrity, and harassment), sexual crimes (i.e. rape, sexual coercion, child molestation, sexual intercourse with a child, child pornography offenses, pimping, and sexual harassment), property crimes (e.g. theft, larceny, burglary, embezzlement), and traffic crimes (e.g. crimes against road safety, and unlawful driving of a vehicle), but excluded substance-related convictions which were included in the definition of substance misuse.

Covariates

As parental covariates, we included both parents' birth year (as continuous variables), immigrant status, educational level, and lifetime history of any psychiatric disorders other than alcohol and drug use disorders in the NPR [ICD-8 codes 290–315 (excluding 291, 303, 304); ICD-9 codes 290–319 (excluding 291, 303–305); ICD-10 codes F00-F09, F20-F99]. Immigrant status was a dichotomous variable denoting whether the parent was born outside of Sweden. Educational level was defined as the highest attained education, available for the years 1990–2013 in the Longitudinal Integration Database for Health Insurance and Labour Market Studies (LISA). As offspring covariates, we included birth year (continuous variable) and sex. Finally, we used dates of emigration from Sweden and death, available from the Migration Register and the Cause of Death Register, respectively, to correctly define follow-up time for offspring outcomes.

Statistical methods

Individual-level analysis

Following descriptive analyses, we fitted Cox proportional hazard regression models to estimate the relative hazard of substance misuse and criminality registrations during the follow-up among individuals with parental substance misuse as compared to those without parental substance misuse. We conducted separate analyses for maternal and paternal substance misuse as exposure and for the two outcomes, following the participants from birth until the occurrence of the first substance misuse registration or criminal conviction, respectively. Those who had no registrations during the study period contributed person-time at risk until the end of follow-up (31 December 2013), emigration, or death, whichever occurred first. In the first model, we adjusted for parent's birth year, and offspring birth year and sex. The second model added the co-parent's birth year and substance misuse as well as immigrant status, educational level, and psychiatric disorders for both parents. Graphical inspection of the Schoenfeld residuals did not reveal violations of the proportional hazards assumption. The models were fitted with adjustment of standard errors for the non-independence of siblings in the offspring generation using a cluster-robust sandwich estimator.

Within-family analysis

We conducted stratified Cox regression analyses within clusters of paternal and maternal full cousins. These models use information from cousins who are discordant for parental substance misuse and by design rule out all factors that are constant within clusters, including unmeasured genetic and shared environmental factors (Allison, Reference Allison2009). Full cousin offspring of fathers who were brothers and mothers who were sisters were included, and the estimates were compared with results from the individual-level models, taking reduced hazard ratios (HRs) as evidence for familial (i.e. genetic and environmental factors shared between full cousins) influences contributing to the parent-offspring associations.

Quantitative genetic modeling

To estimate the contributions of genetic and environmental factors to the intergenerational associations, we conducted quantitative genetic structural equation models in extended family pedigree data. We used an extension of the classical twin model, which decomposes variance into additive genetic influences (A), shared environmental (C) and non-shared environmental (E) influences. In contrast to the standard twin model, our model estimates intergenerational bivariate associations between parental substance misuse and the offspring outcomes. The model decomposes the shared environmental variance into environmental influences shared by all members of an extended family (i.e. fathers/mothers who are full siblings, MZ twins, or maternal half-siblings, and their offspring) (T) and into environmental influences only shared by members of a nuclear family (i.e. father/mother and his/her offspring) (N) (Kuja-Halkola et al. Reference Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Larsson and Lichtenstein2014). The model partitions the association between parental substance misuse and offspring outcomes into the A, T and N components and estimates the proportion of the correlation explained by each factor (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. Quantitative genetic model for the association between parental and offspring substance misuse (SM), showing additive genetic (A), extended family environmental (T), nuclear family environmental (N), and individual-specific environmental (E) variance components and their correlations (r) in the parent (P) and offspring (O) generations. The figure shows a pair of sibling parents (Parent1, Parent2), two siblings in the offspring generation in nuclear family 1 (Offspring11, Offspring12), and, for simplicity, one sibling in the offspring generation in nuclear family 2 (Offspring21). g denotes the genetic relatedness between the sibling parents and gets the value 1, 0.5, and 0.25 in parents who are MZ twins, DZ twins or full siblings, and half-siblings, respectively. go denotes the genetic relatedness between offspring within nuclear families, either 0.5 or 0.25 for full siblings and half-siblings, respectively. Please note that the T and N components in the parent generation correspond to the shared and non-shared environmental components, respectively, in the classical twin model. While, in the offspring generation, the N and E components correspond to the shared and non-shared environmental components, respectively, and the T component corresponds to an environmental effect shared between cousins.

The model has been described in detail elsewhere (Kuja-Halkola et al. Reference Kuja-Halkola, D'Onofrio, Larsson and Lichtenstein2014; Latvala et al. Reference Latvala, Kuja-Halkola, Langstrom and Lichtenstein2015), but a comparison with the classical twin design is illustrative. The parent generation part of the model includes the variance components A, T, and N, which are analogous to the A, C, and E components of the twin design. The nuclear family component N is non-shared between sibling parents, and thus contains all individual-specific influences, including measurement error, in the parent generation. In the offspring generation, the variance is decomposed into the A, T, N, and E components. Here the A and E components correspond perfectly with the same components of the classical twin design. The difference is in the shared environmental variance, which is captured in the C component in twin studies but divided into the nuclear family environmental component N and the extended family environmental component T in the current model.

We estimated a liability-threshold model with binary outcome variables indicating substance misuse and criminal convictions during follow-up. The liability-threshold model assumes each individual to have an unobserved normally distributed liability for the outcome, and those with observed events are assumed to have a liability exceeding the threshold (Posthuma et al. Reference Posthuma, Beem, de Geus, van Baal, von Hjelmborg, Lachine and Boomsma2003). The quantitative genetic models were adjusted for parental and offspring birth year and for offspring sex. The OpenMx package (Neale et al. Reference Neale, Hunter, Pritikin, Zahery, Brick, Kirkpatrick and Boker2016) in the software R (R Core Team, 2013) was used for modeling.

Sensitivity analyses

To test the robustness of our findings, we conducted the following sensitivity analyses. First, while the main individual-level and within-family analyses included lifetime measures of exposures and outcomes, to rule out reverse associations between exposures and outcomes we repeated the Cox regression models defining parental substance misuse as a time-varying covariate and studied associations with offspring outcomes (i) during the lifetime, and (ii) by age 18. Second, to rule out the potential effect of left censoring in the exposure (i.e. unknown parental substance misuse status before the start of the registers in 1973), we repeated the Cox models including only offspring whose parents were born 1958 or later and thus had information about substance misuse since age 15. Third, we investigated the evidence for differential associations by offspring sex by conducting the individual-level analyses separately for sons and daughters. Finally, we sought to test possible carry-over effects in within-family analyses stratified by whether the earlier-born or the later-born sibling parent had substance misuse.

Results

Descriptive data of the study population are presented in Table 1. There were 366 641 (14.8%) individuals with paternal and 113 190 (5.6%) individuals with maternal substance misuse in the offspring generation, while 188 352 (7.6%) individuals had substance misuse and 309 221 (12.5%) had criminal convictions. Person-years at risk and incidence rates for substance misuse and criminal convictions are given in online Supplementary Table S5. The median length of follow-up was 27.3 (interquartile range: 21.7–35.2) years for substance misuse and 26.2 (20.8–34.1) years for criminal convictions.

Table 1. Characteristics of offspring (N = 2 476 198) by parental substance misuse

Individual-level and within-family analyses

Table 2 shows HRs for substance misuse and criminal convictions associated with paternal and maternal substance misuse. In minimally adjusted models, both paternal and maternal substance misuse more than doubled the risk for offspring substance misuse [paternal: HR = 2.51 (95% confidence interval (CI) 2.49–2.54), maternal: HR = 2.81 (2.77–2.86)] and criminality [paternal: HR = 2.07 (2.05–2.09), maternal: HR = 2.20 (2.17–2.23)]. In fully adjusted models, the associations were reduced but both paternal and maternal substance misuse were still robustly associated with the offspring outcomes (HRs between 1.56 and 1.96).

Table 2. Cox proportional hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals) for substance misuse and criminal convictions associated with paternal and maternal substance misuse

a Stratified Cox regression in pairs of paternal/maternal full cousins.

b Adjusted for parental and offspring birth years and offspring sex.

c Further adjusted for birth year and substance misuse of the co-parent, and immigrant status, educational level and psychiatric disorders of both parents.

Associations were further reduced in stratified Cox regression models within pairs of full cousins (Table 2), suggesting the contribution of familial factors. In fully adjusted models, HRs for the associations between parental substance misuse and offspring outcomes within extended families ranged from 1.41 to 1.57.

Quantitative genetic models

Online Supplementary Tables S6–S9 provide unadjusted and adjusted correlations between family members. We report estimates from the full offspring-of-siblings models in Table 3. Additive genetic influences explained 46% of the variance in paternal and 42% in maternal substance misuse and between 36 and 44% of the variance in offspring substance misuse and criminality. Environmental factors shared between parent and offspring members of extended families (T) and offspring members of nuclear families (N) made only small contributions within generations, explaining 2–5% of the variance. Additive genetic factors (A) explained most of the parent-offspring covariance between parental substance misuse and the offspring outcomes. The proportion of covariance explained by A effects ranged from 54% (95% CI 25–84%), between maternal substance misuse and offspring substance misuse, to 85% (72–97%), between paternal substance misuse and offspring criminality, and the corresponding genetic correlations were between 0.70 and 0.98. With the exception of the association between maternal substance misuse and offspring criminality, also extended and nuclear family environmental components significantly contributed to the association between parental substance misuse and offspring outcomes. These contributions were most notable for the association between maternal and offspring substance misuse [T = 17% (2–31%), N = 29% (12–45%)].

Table 3. Offspring-of-siblings model estimates (95% confidence intervals) for the proportions of variance and covariance of parental substance misuse and offspring outcomes due to additive genetic (A), extended family environmental (T), nuclear family environmental (N), and individual-specific environmental (E) factors

All models were adjusted for parental and offspring birth years and offspring sex. Confidence intervals are Wald-type hence boundaries can span outside parameter space (e.g. correlation upper boundary above 1); we have truncated such boundaries in the table.

a Please note that the T and N components in the parent generation correspond to the shared (C) and non-shared environmental (E) components, respectively, in the classical twin model. While, in the offspring generation, the N and E components correspond to the shared and non-shared environmental components, respectively, and the T component corresponds to an environmental effect shared between cousins.

Sensitivity analyses

Analyses with parental substance misuse as a time-varying covariate found similar associations with offspring substance misuse and criminality as the main analysis with lifetime exposure variables when adjusted for covariates (online Supplementary Table S10). Estimates were also similar in analyses restricted to the offspring of parents born since 1958 (online Supplementary Table S11). Further, individual-level associations between paternal substance misuse and offspring outcomes were similar for sons and daughters, whereas the associations between maternal substance misuse and offspring outcomes were larger in daughters than in sons (online Supplementary Table S12). Finally, we found no systematic differences between the associations of earlier-born v. later-born sibling parent's substance misuse with the offspring outcomes (online Supplementary Table S13).

Discussion

Using nationwide register data from 2.5 million individuals with follow-up time up to over 35 years, we found parental substance misuse to be robustly associated with substance misuse and criminal convictions in offspring. The associations were similar for paternal and maternal substance misuse, and they were not fully explained by childhood socioeconomic status or parental psychiatric comorbidity. Within-family analyses in full cousins suggested the associations were not independent of familial factors shared between sibling parents. To better understand the sources of this familial resemblance, we conducted quantitative genetic models in more than 700 000 maternal and paternal extended families and found genetic factors to explain most of the covariance of parental substance misuse with offspring substance misuse and criminality.

Genetic factors explained 54–67% of the association between parental and offspring substance misuse. This finding concurs with previous evidence from adoption studies highlighting the genetic contribution to the parent-offspring resemblance in alcohol and drug use problems (Cadoret et al. Reference Cadoret, Cain and Grove1980, Reference Cadoret, Yates, Troughton, Woodworth and Stewart1995; Goodwin et al. Reference Goodwin, Schulsinger, Moller, Hermansen, Winokur and Guze1974; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Ji, Edwards, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015a, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015c). Interestingly, our results suggested a possibly stronger genetic contribution to the association between parental substance misuse and offspring criminality, with genetic factors explaining approximately 85% of the intergenerational association. This genetic contribution is consistent with evidence from twin studies of shared genetic influences on substance misuse and antisocial behavior (Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, Reference Kendler, Prescott, Myers and Neale2003; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Lonn, Maes, Lichtenstein, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Hicks, Patrick, Carlson, Iacono and McGue2002) and from adoption studies of parental and offspring externalizing behavior (Hicks, Foster, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Hicks, Foster, Iacono and McGue2013).

Besides genetic influences, we found evidence for the contribution of both nuclear and extended family environmental factors to the associations of parental substance misuse with offspring substance misuse and criminality. The nuclear family environmental factor explained 19–29% of the intergenerational resemblance in substance misuse. In other words, our findings suggest that parents and offspring, as well as siblings living together, resemble each other in their risk for substance misuse even when the contribution of genetic influences is accounted for. While our study cannot establish causality, a causal effect of parental substance misuse on the offspring would be detected as a contribution of the nuclear family environmental component in the model. Such an effect might be mediated through factors such as social learning, problems in parenting, exposure to adverse rearing environment, or effects on offspring psychological development (Cservenka, Reference Cservenka2016; Hafekost et al. Reference Hafekost, Lawrence, O'Leary, Bower, O'Donnell, Semmens and Zubrick2017; Smith et al. Reference Smith, Wilson and Comm Subst Use2016; Weinberg, Reference Weinberg1997; Windle & Windle, Reference Windle and Windle2018). Importantly, our findings from an offspring-of-siblings design replicate earlier adoption studies, which have also found support for parent-offspring associations independent of genetic factors (Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Lonn, Morris, Sundquist, Langstrom and Sundquist2014, Reference Kendler, Ji, Edwards, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015a, Reference Kendler, Ohlsson, Sundquist and Sundquist2015c). However, the contribution of nuclear environmental factors for the association between parental substance misuse and offspring criminal behavior was somewhat less clear. It should also be noted that the nuclear environmental component also includes possible confounding factors shared by parents and offspring.

The extended family environmental component also contributed, although to a lesser degree, to the intergenerational associations, suggesting that the risks in parents and offspring within extended families correlate even when shared genetic effects and environmental influences within nuclear families are accounted for. While more difficult to interpret, this could reflect broader socioeconomic or cultural factors operating within extended families. Taken together, our findings on the nuclear and extended family environmental contributions are compatible with twin studies finding that environmental factors shared between co-twins partly explain the risk for SUDs and criminal behavior (Dick et al. Reference Dick, Prescott and McGue2010; Kendler et al. Reference Kendler, Maes, Lonn, Morris, Lichtenstein, Sundquist and Sundquist2015b, Reference Kendler, Lonn, Maes, Lichtenstein, Sundquist and Sundquist2016; Verhulst et al. Reference Verhulst, Neale and Kendler2015).

Our results extend the earlier findings from a CoT study reporting shared genetic factors influencing the parent-offspring resemblance in alcohol use disorder (Slutske et al. Reference Slutske, D'Onofrio, Turkheimer, Emery, Harden, Heath and Martin2008), as well as other CoT studies which have had limited success in differentiating genetic influences from direct effects of parental substance misuse (Duncan et al. Reference Duncan, Scherrer, Fu, Bucholz, Heath, True and Jacob2006, Reference Duncan, Sartor, Scherrer, Grant, Heath, Nelson and Bucholz2008; Haber et al. Reference Haber, Bucholz, Jacob, Grant, Scherrer, Sartor and Heath2010; Waldron et al. Reference Waldron, Martin and Heath2009). Taken together, our results and findings from other designs clearly indicate passive gene-environment correlation (Plomin et al. Reference Plomin, Defries and Loehlin1977) as a key mechanism giving rise to the intergenerational transmission of substance use problems and antisocial behavior whereas evidence for an environmental effect of exposure to parental substance misuse on offspring outcomes is less consistent and may depend on the outcome. Passive gene-environment correlation refers to the situation where parents provide their children with both the genetic risk and the environmental exposures associated with the risk of substance misuse and antisocial behavior. Our findings regarding the environmental contributions to the parent-offspring resemblance in substance misuse are similar to those from recent genetically informative studies of parental depressive and anxiety disorders, which have found evidence for both genetic and environmental effects of parental, especially maternal, internalizing disorders on offspring internalizing disorders (Eley et al. Reference Eley, McAdams, Rijsdijk, Lichtenstein, Narusyte, Reiss and Neiderhiser2015; Hannigan et al. Reference Hannigan, Eilertsen, Gjerde, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Eley, Rijsdijk and McAdams2018; Tully, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Tully, Iacono and McGue2008). Taken together, existing evidence indicates that genetic effects should feature centrally in theoretical models of intergenerational transmission of psychopathology and that they should not be disregarded when developing and evaluating intervention efforts to reduce the intergenerational burden of mental health problems. On the other hand, these findings also further underline the importance of accounting for genetic risks in studies of environmental effects on child development and demonstrate that such environmental factors can indeed be separated from genetic effects.

Our findings should be considered in the context of several limitations. First, register-based data only include a subset of all parents and offspring with substance misuse. In addition to diagnosed disorders, we included alcohol and drug-related criminal convictions to widen the definition of substance misuse, but our results are best interpreted as pertaining to more severe substance use problems in parents and offspring. Complementing this perspective, evidence from an adoption study suggests genetic influences also contribute to the parent-offspring similarity in patterns of non-disordered alcohol use (McGue, Malone, Keyes, & Iacono, Reference McGue, Malone, Keyes and Iacono2014). Second, our main analyses included lifetime registrations of parental exposure and offspring outcomes, raising the issue of timing of the possible environmental effects as well as reverse causality. Lifetime data were used because register-based dates of diagnoses or criminal convictions related to alcohol or drug use disorders do not correspond well with the true onset of substance use problems (McGorry, Purcell, Goldstone, & Amminger, Reference McGorry, Purcell, Goldstone and Amminger2011). However, when using parental substance misuse registrations as time-varying covariates predating offspring outcomes we found similar associations as the main analyses both in the individual-level and within-family analyses. This suggests that our findings with lifetime measures are unlikely to be significantly biased. Relatedly, we also found similar estimates when using data from parental birth cohorts with register-based coverage of substance misuse starting no later than at age 15, suggesting that the inclusion of earlier cohorts with left-censored data did not distort our findings. Fourth, the main Cox regression analyses and quantitative genetic models were conducted pooling data from sons and daughters, which might have concealed sex-specific effects. However, sensitivity analyses did not suggest notable sex-specificity of the associations between parental substance misuse and the offspring outcomes; thus, we aimed to maximize statistical power in the intergenerational models. Fifth, cohort effects could have affected our results. We adjusted all analyses for parental and offspring birth years, which had an effect on the intergenerational associations and reduced the possible bias due to cohort differences. Finally, the offspring-of-siblings design makes several assumptions, the validity of which needs to be critically assessed (D'Onofrio et al. Reference D'Onofrio, Lahey, Turkheimer and Lichtenstein2013). For example, they assume that there are no carry-over effects from uncles/aunts to their nephews/nieces. We aimed to test this by comparing the within-family associations in extended families where the older v. the younger sibling parent had substance misuse but found no systematic differences in the associations. Further, we conducted both fixed effects within-family analyses and quantitative genetic modeling which are based on partly different assumptions, supporting the validity of our findings.

In conclusion, in a large population-based family study we found consistent support for genetic influences contributing to the associations of parental substance misuse with offspring substance misuse and criminality. Independent of genetic influences, environmental factors shared within nuclear and extended families also partly explained the intergenerational associations.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002135.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the Swedish Twin Registry for access to data. This research was supported by funding from the Academy of Finland (grants no. 277209 and 308698 to AL), and the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare (project 2012–1678), and the Swedish Research Council (2016-01989) to PL. The Swedish Twin Registry is managed by Karolinska Institutet and receives funding through the Swedish Research Council under the grant no 2017-00641.

Conflicts of interest

HL has served as a speaker for Evolan Pharma and Shire and has received research grants from the Shire; all outside the submitted work. Other authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

*

These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Allison, P. (2009). Fixed effects regression models, Vol. 160. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bountress, K., Chassin, L., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2017). Parent and peer influences on emerging adult substance use disorder: A genetically informed study. Development and Psychopathology, 29(1), 121142. doi:10.1017/s095457941500125x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cadoret, R. J., Cain, C. A., & Grove, W. M. (1980). Development of alcoholism in adoptees raised apart from alcoholic biologic relatives. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37(5), 561563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stewart, M. A. (1995). Adoption study demonstrating 2 genetic pathways to drug-abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52(1), 4252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, Z., Lichtenstein, P., D'Onofrio, B. M., Almqvist, C., Kuja-Halkola, R., Sjolander, A., & Larsson, H. (2014). Maternal age at childbirth and risk for ADHD in offspring: A population-based cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(6), 18151824. doi:10.1093/ije/dyu204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowe, R. R. (1974). Adoption study of antisocial personality. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31(6), 785791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cservenka, A. (2016). Neurobiological phenotypes associated with a family history of alcoholism. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 158, 821. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.10.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Prescott, C., & McGue, M. (2010). The genetics of substance Use and substance Use disorders. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
D'Onofrio, B. M., Lahey, B. B., Turkheimer, E., & Lichtenstein, P. (2013). Critical need for family-based, quasi-experimental designs in integrating genetic and social science research. American Journal of Public Health, 103, 4655. doi:10.2105/ajph.2013.301252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, A. E., Sartor, C. E., Scherrer, J. F., Grant, J. D., Heath, A. C., Nelson, E. C., … Bucholz, K. K. (2008). The association between cannabis abuse and dependence and childhood physical and sexual abuse: Evidence from an offspring of twins design. Addiction, 103(6), 990997. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02210.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, A. E., Scherrer, J., Fu, Q., Bucholz, K. K., Heath, A. C., True, W. R., … Jacob, T. (2006). Exposure to paternal alcoholism does not predict development of alcohol-use disorders in offspring: Evidence from an offspring-of-twins study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67(5), 649656. doi:10.15288/jsa.2006.67.649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eley, T. C., McAdams, T. A., Rijsdijk, F. V., Lichtenstein, P., Narusyte, J., Reiss, D., … Neiderhiser, J. M. (2015). The intergenerational transmission of anxiety: A children-of-twins study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(7), 630637. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14070818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, D. W., Schulsinger, F., Moller, N., Hermansen, L., Winokur, G., & Guze, S. B. (1974). Drinking problems in adopted and nonadopted sons of alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 31(2), 164169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haber, J. R., Bucholz, K. K., Jacob, T., Grant, J. D., Scherrer, J. F., Sartor, C. E., … Heath, A. (2010). Effect of paternal alcohol and drug dependence on offspring conduct disorder: Gene-environment interplay. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71(5), 652663. doi:10.15288/jsad.2010.71.652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafekost, K., Lawrence, D., O'Leary, C., Bower, C., O'Donnell, M., Semmens, J., & Zubrick, S. R. (2017). Maternal alcohol use disorder and subsequent child protection contact: A record-linkage population cohort study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 72, 206-214. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.08.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannigan, L. J., Eilertsen, E. M., Gjerde, L. C., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Eley, T. C., Rijsdijk, F. V., … McAdams, T. A. (2018). Maternal prenatal depressive symptoms and risk for early-life psychopathology in offspring: Genetic analyses in the Norwegian mother and child birth cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry, 5(10), 808815. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(18)30225-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, K. L., Fulco, C. J., Agbeke, D. V., & Ratcliff, A. M. (2018). Intergenerational continuity in substance abuse: Does offspring's friendship network make a difference? Journal of Adolescent Health, 63(2), 205212. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.02.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hicks, B. M., Foster, K. T., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2013). Genetic and environmental influences on the familial transmission of externalizing disorders in adoptive and twin offspring. Jama Psychiatry, 70(10), 10761083. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hussong, A. M., Wirth, R. J., Edwards, M. C., Curran, P. J., Chassin, L. A., & Zucker, R. A. (2007). Externalizing symptoms among children of alcoholic parents: Entry points for an antisocial pathway to alcoholism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 529542. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.116.3.529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Ji, J. G., Edwards, A. C., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2015a). An extended Swedish national adoption study of alcohol use disorder. Jama Psychiatry, 72(3), 211218. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Lonn, S. L., Maes, H. H., Lichtenstein, P., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2016). A Swedish population-based multivariate twin study of externalizing disorders. Behavior Genetics, 46(2), 183192. doi:10.1007/s10519-015-9741-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Lonn, S. L., Morris, N. A., Sundquist, J., Langstrom, N., & Sundquist, K. (2014). A Swedish national adoption study of criminality. Psychological Medicine, 44(9), 19131925. doi:10.1017/s0033291713002638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Maes, H. H., Lonn, S. L., Morris, N. A., Lichtenstein, P., Sundquist, J., & Sundquist, K. (2015b). A Swedish national twin study of criminal behavior and its violent, white-collar and property subtypes. Psychological Medicine, 45(11), 22532262. doi:10.1017/s0033291714002098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Ohlsson, H., Sundquist, K., & Sundquist, J. (2015c). The causes of parent-offspring transmission of drug abuse: A Swedish population-based study. Psychological Medicine, 45(1), 8795. doi:10.1017/s0033291714001093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(9), 929937. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khemiri, L., Larsson, H., Kuja-Halkola, R., D'Onofrio, B. M., Lichtenstein, P., Jayaram-Lindstrom, N., & Latvala, A. (2020). Association of parental substance use disorder with offspring cognition: A population family-based study. Addiction, 115(2), 326336. doi:10.1111/add.14813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Carlson, S. R., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2002). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(3), 411424. doi:10.1037//0021-843x.111.3.411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuja-Halkola, R., D'Onofrio, B. M., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2014). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and adverse outcomes in offspring: Genetic and environmental sources of covariance. Behavior Genetics, 44(5), 456467. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9668-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latvala, A., Kuja-Halkola, R., D'Onofrio, B. M., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2016). Cognitive ability and risk for substance misuse in men: Genetic and environmental correlations in a longitudinal nation-wide family study. Addiction, 111(10), 18141822. doi:10.1111/add.13440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latvala, A., Kuja-Halkola, R., Langstrom, N., & Lichtenstein, P. (2015). Paternal antisocial behavior and sons’ cognitive ability: A population-based quasiexperimental study. Psychological Science, 26(1), 7888. doi:10.1177/0956797614555726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieb, R., Merikangas, K. R., Hofler, M., Pfister, H., Isensee, B., & Wittchen, H. U. (2002). Parental alcohol use disorders and alcohol use and disorders in offspring: A community study. Psychological Medicine, 32(1), 6378. doi:10.1017/s0033291701004883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ludvigsson, J. F., Almqvist, C., Bonamy, A. K. E., Ljung, R., Michaelsson, K., Neovius, M., … Ye, W. M. (2016). Registers of the Swedish total population and their use in medical research. European Journal of Epidemiology, 31(2), 125136. doi:10.1007/s10654-016-0117-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahedy, L., MacArthur, G. J., Hammerton, G., Edwards, A. C., Kendler, K. S., Macleod, J., … Heron, J. (2018). The effect of parental drinking on alcohol use in young adults: The mediating role of parental monitoring and peer deviance. Addiction, 113(11), 20412050. doi:10.1111/add.14280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAdams, T. A., Hannigan, L. J., Eilertsen, E. M., Gjerde, L. C., Ystrom, E., & Rijsdijk, F. V. (2018). Revisiting the children-of-twins design: Improving existing models for the exploration of intergenerational associations. Behavior Genetics, 48(5), 397412. doi:10.1007/s10519-018-9912-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAdams, T. A., Neiderhiser, J. M., Rijsdijk, F. V., Narusyte, J., Lichtenstein, P., & Eley, T. C. (2014). Accounting for genetic and environmental confounds in associations between parent and child characteristics: A systematic review of children-of-twins studies. Psychological Bulletin, 140(4), 11381173. doi:10.1037/a0036416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGorry, P. D., Purcell, R., Goldstone, S., & Amminger, G. P. (2011). Age of onset and timing of treatment for mental and substance use disorders: Implications for preventive intervention strategies and models of care. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 24(4), 301306. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283477a09.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGue, M. (1997). A behavioral-genetic perspective on children of alcoholics. Alcohol Health & Research World, 21(3), 210217.Google ScholarPubMed
McGue, M., Malone, S., Keyes, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2014). Parent-offspring similarity for drinking: A longitudinal adoption study. Behavior Genetics, 44(6), 620628. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9672-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellentin, A. I., Brink, M., Andersen, L., Erlangsen, A., Stenager, E., Bjerregaard, L. B., & Christiansen, E. (2016). The risk of offspring developing substance use disorders when exposed to one versus two parent(s) with alcohol use disorder: A nationwide, register-based cohort study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 80, 5258. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.06.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merikangas, K. R., Stolar, M., Stevens, D. E., Goulet, J., Preisig, M. A., Fenton, B., … Rounsaville, B. J. (1998). Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(11), 973979. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.11.973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, T. E. (1987). Parental mental disorder and offspring criminal behavior – an adoption study. Psychiatry-Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 50(4), 346360. doi:10.1080/00332747.1987.11024366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, M. C., Hunter, M. D., Pritikin, J. N., Zahery, M., Brick, T. R., Kirkpatrick, R. M., … Boker, S. M. (2016). Openmx 2.0: Extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika, 81(2), 535549. doi:10.1007/s11336-014-9435-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petraitis, J., Flay, B. R., & Miller, T. Q. (1995). Reviewing theories of adolescent substance use – organizing pieces in the puzzle. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 6786. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., Defries, J. C., & Loehlin, J. C. (1977). Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in analysis of human-behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84(2), 309322. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.2.309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polderman, T. J. C., Benyamin, B., de Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., & Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702–+. doi:10.1038/ng.3285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posthuma, D., Beem, A. L., de Geus, E. J. C., van Baal, G. C. M., von Hjelmborg, J. B., Lachine, I., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Theory and practice in quantitative genetics. Twin Research, 6(5), 361376. doi:10.1375/twin.6.5.361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinn, P. D., & Fromme, K. (2011). Subjective response to alcohol challenge: A quantitative review. Alcoholism-Clinical and Experimental Research, 35(10), 17591770. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01521.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Sheridan, M. J. (1995). A proposed intergenerational model of substance-abuse, family functioning, and abuse neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19(5), 519530. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(95)00012-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slutske, W. S., D'Onofrio, B. M., Turkheimer, E., Emery, R. E., Harden, K. P., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (2008). Searching for an environmental effect of parental alcoholism on offspring alcohol use disorder: A genetically informed study of children of alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(3), 534551. doi:10.1037/a0012907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, V. C., Wilson, C. R., & Comm Subst Use, P. (2016). Families affected by parental substance use. Pediatrics, 138(2), 13. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-1575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tully, E. C., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2008). An adoption study of parental depression as an environmental liability for adolescent depression and childhood disruptive disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(9), 11481154. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07091438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhulst, B., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2015). The heritability of alcohol use disorders: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Medicine, 45(5), 10611072. doi:10.1017/s0033291714002165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voogt, C., Beusink, M., Kleinjan, M., Otten, R., Engels, R., Smit, K., & Kuntsche, E. (2017). Alcohol-related cognitions in children (aged 2–10) and how they are shaped by parental alcohol use: A systematic review. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 177, 277290. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.04.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walden, B., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2007). Trajectories of change in adolescent substance use and symptomatology: Impact of paternal and maternal substance use disorders. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(1), 3543. doi:10.1037/0893-164x.21.1.35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waldron, M., Martin, N. G., & Heath, A. C. (2009). Parental alcoholism and offspring behavior problems: Findings in Australian children of twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 12(5), 433440. doi:10.1375/twin.12.5.433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, N. Z. (1997). Cognitive and behavioral deficits associated with parental alcohol use. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(9), 11771186. doi:10.1097/00004583-199709000-00009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Windle, M., & Windle, R. C. (2018). Parental divorce and family history of alcohol disorder: Associations with young adults’ alcohol problems, marijuana use, and interpersonal relations. Alcoholism-Clinical and Experimental Research, 42(6), 10841095. doi:10.1111/acer.13638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Samples used for individual-level, within-family, and quantitative genetic analyses. All Ns refer to the offspring generation. The samples used in within-family analyses are subsamples of those used in the quantitative genetic analyses.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Quantitative genetic model for the association between parental and offspring substance misuse (SM), showing additive genetic (A), extended family environmental (T), nuclear family environmental (N), and individual-specific environmental (E) variance components and their correlations (r) in the parent (P) and offspring (O) generations. The figure shows a pair of sibling parents (Parent1, Parent2), two siblings in the offspring generation in nuclear family 1 (Offspring11, Offspring12), and, for simplicity, one sibling in the offspring generation in nuclear family 2 (Offspring21). g denotes the genetic relatedness between the sibling parents and gets the value 1, 0.5, and 0.25 in parents who are MZ twins, DZ twins or full siblings, and half-siblings, respectively. go denotes the genetic relatedness between offspring within nuclear families, either 0.5 or 0.25 for full siblings and half-siblings, respectively. Please note that the T and N components in the parent generation correspond to the shared and non-shared environmental components, respectively, in the classical twin model. While, in the offspring generation, the N and E components correspond to the shared and non-shared environmental components, respectively, and the T component corresponds to an environmental effect shared between cousins.

Figure 2

Table 1. Characteristics of offspring (N = 2 476 198) by parental substance misuse

Figure 3

Table 2. Cox proportional hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals) for substance misuse and criminal convictions associated with paternal and maternal substance misuse

Figure 4

Table 3. Offspring-of-siblings model estimates (95% confidence intervals) for the proportions of variance and covariance of parental substance misuse and offspring outcomes due to additive genetic (A), extended family environmental (T), nuclear family environmental (N), and individual-specific environmental (E) factors

Supplementary material: File

Latvala et al. Supplementary Materials

Latvala et al. Supplementary Materials

Download Latvala et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 42.6 KB