Skip to main content
    • Aa
    • Aa
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 64
  • Cited by
    This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Beyderman, Irina and Young, Michael A. 2016. Rumination and overgeneral autobiographical memory as mediators of the relationship between attachment and depression. Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 98, p. 37.

    Cook, Lorna and Watkins, Edward 2016. Guided, internet-based, rumination-focused cognitive behavioural therapy (i-RFCBT) versus a no-intervention control to prevent depression in high-ruminating young adults, along with an adjunct assessment of the feasibility of unguided i-RFCBT, in the REducing Stress and Preventing Depression trial (RESPOND): study protocol for a phase III randomised controlled trial. Trials, Vol. 17, Issue. 1,

    De Haan-Rietdijk, Silvia Gottman, John M. Bergeman, Cindy S. and Hamaker, Ellen L. 2016. Get Over It! A Multilevel Threshold Autoregressive Model for State-Dependent Affect Regulation. Psychometrika, Vol. 81, Issue. 1, p. 217.

    Hasegawa, Akira Nishimura, Haruki Mastuda, Yuko Kunisato, Yoshihiko Morimoto, Hiroshi and Adachi, Masaki 2016. Is Trait Rumination Associated with the Ability to Generate Effective Problem Solving Strategies? Utilizing Two Versions of the Means-Ends Problem-Solving Test. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, Vol. 34, Issue. 1, p. 14.

    Joormann, Jutta and Stanton, Colin 2016. Examining emotion regulation in depression: A review and future directions. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

    Julien, Camille L. Rimes, Katharine A. and Brown, Richard G. 2016. Rumination and behavioural factors in Parkinson's disease depression. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, Vol. 82, p. 48.

    Lehmann, Mick Seifritz, Erich Henning, Anke Walter, Martin Böker, Heinz Scheidegger, Milan and Grimm, Simone 2016. Differential effects of rumination and distraction on ketamine induced modulation of resting state functional connectivity and reactivity of regions within the default-mode network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, p. nsw034.

    LeMoult, Joelle Yoon, K. Lira and Joormann, Jutta 2016. Rumination and Cognitive Distraction in Major Depressive Disorder: an Examination of Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, Vol. 38, Issue. 1, p. 20.

    Riihimäki, K. Vuorilehto, M. Jylhä, P. and Isometsä, E. 2016. Response style and severity and chronicity of depressive disorders in primary health care. European Psychiatry, Vol. 33, p. 1.

    Schiller, Crystal Edler Johnson, Sarah L. Abate, Anna C. Schmidt, Peter J. and Rubinow, David R. 2016. Comprehensive Physiology.

    Selby, Edward A. Kranzler, Amy Panza, Emily and Fehling, Kara B. 2016. Bidirectional-Compounding Effects of Rumination and Negative Emotion in Predicting Impulsive Behavior: Implications for Emotional Cascades. Journal of Personality, Vol. 84, Issue. 2, p. 139.

    Varga, Somogy 2016. Evolutionary Thinking in Medicine.

    Aldao, Amelia and Christensen, Kara 2015. Linking the Expanded Process Model of Emotion Regulation to Psychopathology by Focusing on Behavioral Outcomes of Regulation. Psychological Inquiry, Vol. 26, Issue. 1, p. 27.

    Hasegawa, Akira Hattori, Yosuke Nishimura, Haruki and Tanno, Yoshihiko 2015. PROSPECTIVE ASSOCIATIONS OF DEPRESSIVE RUMINATION AND SOCIAL PROBLEM SOLVING WITH DEPRESSION: A 6-MONTH LONGITUDINAL STUDY1,2. Psychological Reports, Vol. 116, Issue. 3, p. 870.

    Kovacs, Maria Yaroslavsky, Ilya Rottenberg, Jonathan George, Charles J. Baji, Ildikó Benák, István Dochnal, Roberta Halas, Kitti Kiss, Enikő Vetró, Ágnes and Kapornai, Krisztina 2015. Mood repair via attention refocusing or recall of positive autobiographical memories by adolescents with pediatric-onset major depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 56, Issue. 10, p. 1108.

    O'Mahen, Heather A. Boyd, Alex and Gashe, Caroline 2015. Rumination decreases parental problem-solving effectiveness in dysphoric postnatal mothers. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Vol. 47, p. 18.

    Roley, Michelle E. Claycomb, Meredith A. Contractor, Ateka A. Dranger, Paula Armour, Cherie and Elhai, Jon D. 2015. The relationship between rumination, PTSD, and depression symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, Vol. 180, p. 116.

    Dieler, A. C. Herrmann, M. J. and Fallgatter, A. J. 2014. Voluntary suppression of thoughts is influenced by anxious and ruminative tendencies in healthy volunteers. Memory, Vol. 22, Issue. 3, p. 184.

    Hasegawa, Akira Koda, Munenaga Hattori, Yosuke Kondo, Tsuyoshi and Kawaguchi, Jun 2014. DEPRESSIVE RUMINATION AND PAST DEPRESSION IN JAPANESE UNIVERSITY STUDENTS: COMPARISON OF BROODING AND REFLECTION1,2. Psychological Reports, Vol. 114, Issue. 3, p. 653.

    Hasegawa, Akira Koda, Munenaga Hattori, Yosuke Kondo, Tsuyoshi and Kawaguchi, Jun 2013. LONGITUDINAL PREDICTIONS OF THE BROODING AND REFLECTION SUBSCALES OF THE JAPANESE RUMINATIVE RESPONSES SCALE FOR DEPRESSION1. Psychological Reports, Vol. 113, Issue. 2, p. 566.


Rumination, mood and social problem-solving in major depression

  • C. DONALDSON (a1) and D. LAM (a1)
  • DOI:
  • Published online: 01 October 2004

Background. Ruminating when depressed is thought to lower mood and impair problem-solving, while distraction is thought to alleviate mood and assist problem-solving. The present study investigates each of these proposals using both naturally occurring and experimentally induced rumination and distraction in a sample of patients with major depression.

Method. Thirty-six patients with major depression and 36 control participants were randomly allocated to either a rumination or distraction induction condition. Levels of trait rumination and distraction were measured at baseline, mood and problem-solving were measured before and after the inductions.

Results. In terms of trait measures, depressed patients with higher levels of trait rumination reported poorer mood and gave less effective problem solutions than those who were less ruminative. Trait distraction was not associated with mood or problem-solving. In terms of induced responses, depressed patients who were made to ruminate experienced a deterioration in their mood and gave poorer problem solutions. For those receiving the distraction induction, mood improved in all patients and problem-solving improved in patients who were not naturally ruminating at a high level. Neither induction had an impact on mood or problem-solving in control participants.

Conclusions. Treatment for depression associated with adverse life events may need to target rumination as well as problem-solving deficits if interventions are to be effective. The differential effects of self-applied versus experimentally induced distraction require further investigation. Future research will need to consider that high levels of trait rumination may interfere with the impact of experimental inductions.

Corresponding author
Dr C. Donaldson, Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Psychological Medicine
  • ISSN: 0033-2917
  • EISSN: 1469-8978
  • URL: /core/journals/psychological-medicine
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *