Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-8mjnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T13:57:24.591Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How well do children aged 5–7 years recall food eaten at school lunch?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 October 2007

Janet M Warren*
Affiliation:
Nutrition and Food Science Group, School of Biological and Molecular Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 OBP, UK
C Jeya K Henry
Affiliation:
Nutrition and Food Science Group, School of Biological and Molecular Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 OBP, UK
M Barbara E Livingstone
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre Diet and Health, University of Ulster, Cromore Road, Coleraine, Co. Londonderry, BT52 1SA, UK
Helen J Lightowler
Affiliation:
Nutrition and Food Science Group, School of Biological and Molecular Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 OBP, UK
Suzanne M Bradshaw
Affiliation:
Nutrition and Food Science Group, School of Biological and Molecular Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 OBP, UK
Sylvia Perwaiz
Affiliation:
Nutrition and Food Science Group, School of Biological and Molecular Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane Campus, Headington, Oxford OX3 OBP, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Email jmwarren@brookes.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.
Objective:

This study aimed to determine the accuracy with which children aged 5 to 7 years were able to report the food eaten at a school lunch.

Subjects/setting:

Two hundred and three children (103 boys, 100 girls) aged 5–7 years were recruited from three primary schools in Oxford.

Design:

Trained investigators made observational records of the school dinner and packed lunch intakes of four or five children per session. Children were interviewed within two hours of finishing the lunchtime meal and asked to provide a free recall of their meal. When the child had completed the recall, non-directive prompts were used to assess if the child was able to remember anything else. Foods recalled were classified as matches (recalled food agreed with observation), omissions (failed to report a food observed) or phantoms (recalled food was not observed).

Results:

The percentage of accurate recall was significantly higher (P<0.01) in children eating packed lunch (mean 70±29%) than in children consuming school dinners (mean 58±27%). This difference may have been due to increased familiarity of foods in packed lunches. Leftovers were not readily reported in this age group. Prompts and cues enhanced recall by all children.

Conclusions:

This study indicated that there was a wide range in the ability of children aged 5–7 years to recall intake from a packed lunch and/or school dinner. This dietary assessment method is unlikely to be suitable at an individual level. Investigators using dietary recall to estimate food intake in children aged 5–7 years need to be aware of the limitations of this method.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © CABI Publishing 2003

References

1Dietz, WH. Prevention: it all starts with the children. Appetite 1998; 31(3): 413–6.Google Scholar
2Hill, JO, Trowbridge, FL. Childhood obesity: future directions and research priorities. Pediatrics 1998; 101(3): 570–4.Google Scholar
3Schmitz, MKH, Jeffery, RW. Public health interventions for the prevention and treatment of obesity. Med. Clin. N. Am. 2000; 84(2): 491512, viii.Google Scholar
4Bingham, S, Nelson, M, Paul, A, Haraldsdottir, J, Loken, EB, van Staveren, WA. Methods for data collection at an individual level. In: Cameron, ME, van Staveren, WA, eds. Manual on Methodology for Food Consumption Studies, 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 1988; 53106.Google Scholar
5Medlin, C, Skinner, JD. Individual dietary intake methodology: a 50-year review of progress. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1988; 88(10): 1250–7.Google Scholar
6Thompson, FE, Byers, T. Dietary assessment resource manual. J. Nutr. 1994; 124(11 Suppl.): 2245S–317S.Google Scholar
7Haraldsdottir, J, Hermansen, B. Repeated 24-H recalls with young schoolchildren – a feasible alternative to dietary history from parents. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 1995; 49(10): 729–39.Google Scholar
8Rockett, HR, Colditz, GA. Assessing diets of children and adolescents. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1997; 65(4 Suppl.): 1116S–22S.Google Scholar
9Livingstone, MB, Robson, PJ. Measurement of dietary intake in children. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2000; 59(2): 279–93.Google Scholar
10Baranowski, T, Domel, SB. A cognitive model of children's reporting of food-intake. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1994; 59(1): S212–7.Google Scholar
11Domel, SB, Thompson, WO, Baranowski, T, Smith, AF. How children remember what they have eaten. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1994; 94(11): 1267–72.Google Scholar
12Baxter, SD, Thompson, WO, Davis, HC, Litaker, MS. Children's dietary recalls: the salience of entree and liking for foods on accuracy and order of reporting. Nutrition 1999; 15(1112): 848–53.Google Scholar
13Meredith, A, Matthews, A, Zickerfoose, M, Weagley, E, Wayave, M, Brown, EG. How well do school children recall what they have eaten? J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1951; 27: 749–51.Google Scholar
14Emmons, L, Hayes, M. Accuracy of 24-hour recalls in young children. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1973; 62: 409–15.Google Scholar
15McPherson, RS, Hoelscher, DM, Alexander, M, Scanlon, KS, Serdula, MK. Dietary assessment methods among school-aged children: validity and reliability. Prev. Med. 2000; 31(2): S11–33.Google Scholar
16Baranowski, T, Dworkin, R, Henske, JC, Clearman, DR, Dunn, JK, Nader, PR. The accuracy of children's self-reports of diet: Family Health Project. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1986; 86(10): 1381–5.Google Scholar
17Basch, CE, Shea, S, Arliss, R, Contento, IR, Rips, J, Gutin, B, et al. Validation of mothers reports of dietary-intake by 4-year-old to 7-year-old children. Am. J. Public Health 1990; 80(11): 1314–7.Google Scholar
18Eck, LH, Klesges, RC, Hanson, CL. Recall of a child's intake from one meal: are parents accurate? J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1989; 89(6): 784–9.Google Scholar
19Klesges, RC, Klesges, LM, Brown, G, Frank, GC. Validation of the 24-hour dietary recall in preschool-children. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1987; 87(10): 1383–5.Google Scholar
20Johnson, RK, Driscoll, P, Goran, MI. Comparison of multiple-pass 24-hour recall estimates of energy intake with total energy expenditure determined by the doubly labelled water method in young children. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1996; 96(11): 1140–4.Google Scholar
21Bollella, MC, Spark, A, Boccia, LA, Nicklas, TA, Pittman, BP, Williams, CL. Nutrient intake of Head Start children: home vs. school. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 1999; 18(2): 108–14.Google Scholar
22Birch, LL. Development of food preferences. Ann. Rev. Nutr. 1999; 19: 4162.Google Scholar
23Van Horn, LV, Gernhofer, N, Moag-Stahlberg, A, Farris, R, Hartmuller, G, Lasser, VI, et al. Dietary assessment in children using electronic methods: telephones and tape recorders. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1990; 90(3): 412–6.Google Scholar
24Samuelson, G. An epidemiological study of child health and nutrition in a northern Swedish county II. Methodological study of the recall technique. Nutr. Metab. 1970; 12(6): 321–40.Google Scholar