Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T00:37:20.764Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hubris and Humility Effect and the Domain-Masculine Intelligence Type in Two Countries: Colombia and the UK

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2016

Josephine Storek
Affiliation:
University College London (UK)
Adrian Furnham*
Affiliation:
University College London (UK)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Adrian Furnham. University College London. Psychology. WC1E 6BT. London (UK). E-mail: a.furnham@ucl.ac.uk

Abstract

Spanish-speaking Colombian (n = 50) and English-speaking British (N = 52) adults completed a self-assessed intelligence measure that yielded a score on domain-masculine intelligence (DMIQ), a composite of mathematical/logical and spatial intelligences. They also completed a Sex Role inventory in order to establish their masculinity and femininity. Males in both countries gave significantly higher self-estimates (Colombia: Males 110.36, Females 100.75, d = .94; England: Males 114.37, Females 105.75, d = .86; both p < .01) than females but sex role was note related to DMIQ. However there was a positive relationship between masculinity and DMIQ (r = .45, r = .39, p < .01), but only for males. Cultural issues in self-assessed intelligence and limitations, particularly sample size of this exploratory study are considered.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bem, S. L. (1981). Bem Sex Role Inventory: Professional manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. P., & Josephs, R. A. (1999). A burden of proof: Stereotype relevance and gender differences in math performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 246257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.246 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dar-Nimrod, I. (2007). Math ability in women- nature versus nurture. Nanotoday, 2, 56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1748-0132(07)70102-0 Google Scholar
Francis, L. J., & Wilcox, C. (1998). The relationship between Eysenck’s personality dimensions and Bem’s masculinity and femininity scales revisited. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 683687. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00085-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freund, P. A., & Kasten, N. (2012). How smart do you think you are? A meta-analysis on the validity of self-estimates of cognitive ability. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 296321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026556 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A. (2000). Parents’ estimates of their own and their children’s multiple intelligences. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 583594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151000165869 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A. (2001). Self-estimates of intelligence: Culture and gender difference in self and other estimates of both general (g) and multiple intelligences. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 13811405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00232-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A., & Bunclark, K. (2006). Sex differences in parents’ estimations of their own and their children’s intelligence. Intelligence, 34, 114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.05.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2005). Estimating one’s own and one’s relatives’ multiple intelligence: A study from Argentina. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 8, 1220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600004911 Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Clark, K., & Bailey, K. (1999). Sex differences in estimates of multiple intelligences. European Journal of Personality, 13, 247259.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A., & Gasson, L. (1998). Sex differences in parental estimates of their children’s intelligence. Sex Roles, 38, 151162.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., & Shagabutdinova, K. (2012). Sex differences in estimating multiple intelligences in self and others. International Journal of Psychology, 47, 448459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.658054 Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Shahidi, S., & Baluch, B. (2002). Sex and culture differences in perceptions of estimated multiple intelligence for self and family a British-Iranian comparison. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 270285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033003004 Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (2003). Culture’s consequences, comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. (2nd Ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Self estimates of general, crystallized, and fluid intelligences in an ethnocally diverse population. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 118122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.10.001 Google Scholar
Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007). Implicit stereotypes and women’s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes influence women’s susceptibility to stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 825832. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.08.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mengelkamp, C., & Jager, R. (2007). Self-estimates of attention performance. Psychology Science, 49, 223237.Google Scholar
Neto, F., Mullet, E., & Furnham, A. (2016). Self-estimated correlates of lay view about reversal multiple intelligences. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 35, 380396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0276236615599532 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez, L. F., González, C., & Beltrán, J. A. (2010). Parental estimates of their own and their relatives’ intelligence. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 669676. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrides, K. V., Furnham, A., & Martin, G. N. (2004). Estimates of emotional and psychometric intelligence: Evidence for gender-based stereotypes. The Journal of Social Psychology, 144, 149162. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.144.2.149-162 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rammstedt, B., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2002a). Gender differences in self-estimated intelligence and their relation to gender-role orientation. European Journal of Personality, 16, 369382. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.454 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rammstedt, B., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2002b). Self-estimated intelligence. Gender differences, relationship to psychometric intelligence and moderating effects of level of education. European Psychologist, 7, 275284.Google Scholar
Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2015). The effect of priming gender roles on women’s implicit gender beliefs and career aspirations. Social Psychology, 41, 192202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000027 Google Scholar
Rydell, R. J., Rydell, M. T., & Boucher, K. L. (2010). The effect of negative performance stereotypes on learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 883896. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021139 Google Scholar
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797811. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stieger, S., Kastner, C. K., Voracek, M., von Stumm, S., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Independent effects of personality and sex on self-estimated intelligence: Evidence from Austria. Psychological Reports, 107, 553563. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/04.07.09.PR0.107.5.553-563 Google Scholar
Storek, J., & Furnham, A. (2012). Gender and gender role differences in domain-masculine intelligence and beliefs about intelligence: A study with Mensa UK members. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 890895. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.039 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storek, J., & Furnham, A. (2013). Gender, ‘g’, and fixed versus growth intelligence mindsets as predictors of self-estimated domain masculine intelligence (DMIQ). Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 9398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.03.007 Google Scholar
Storek, J., & Furnham, A. (2014). Gender and task confidence as predictors of the domain-masculine intelligence type (DMIQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 4349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2010). Self-assessed intelligence: Inter-ethnic, rural–urban, and sex differences in Malaysia. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(1), 5155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.11.002 Google Scholar
Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2011). Gender differences in self-estimates of general, mathematical, spatial, and verbal intelligence: Four meta-analyses. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 493504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.001 Google Scholar
Szymanowicz, A., & Furnham, A. (2013). Gender and gender role differences in self- and other-estimates of multiple intelligences. Journal of Social Psychology, 153, 399423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2012.754397 Google Scholar
von Stumm, S., Gale, C. R., Batty, G. D., & Deary, I. J. (2009). Childhood intelligence, locus of control and behavior disturbance as determinants of intergenerational social mobility: British Cohort Study 1970. Intelligence, 37, 329340. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2009.04.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar