Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T07:13:47.136Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Sequential Analysis of Private and Social Speech in Children's Dyadic Communication

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 April 2014

Dolors Girbau*
Affiliation:
Jaume I University
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Dolors Girbau, Departamento de Psicología Básica, Clínica y Psicobiología.Universidad Jaume I. Campus Borriol. 12080 Castellón de la Plana (Spain). E-mail: girbau@psb.uji.es

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to perform a sequential analysis of private and social speech in children's dyadic communication. To investigate the communication patterns, a category system was applied to the communication of 64 paired third (M = 8 years and 8 months) and fifth (M = 10 years and 8 months) graders, while playing with a Lego-set (construction material). The results revealed that: (a) at both grades, when one child addresses the other child about the task, it is highly probable that the latter will address the first child immediately afterwards and will adapt to task-related semantic content; (b) at both grades, children's private speech about the task stops them from communicating a task-related production to their partner immediately afterwards; (c) at third grade, task-relevant private speech favors the prolongation of the break in interpersonal communication and the use of inner speech by both children; and (d) at fifth grade, children are more able to distinguish private speech from social speech than at third grade.

El objetivo de este estudio fue realizar un análisis secuencial del habla privada y social en la comunicación diádica infantil. Para investigar los patrones comunicativos, se aplicó un sistema de categorías a la comunicación de 64 sujetos de tercer (M = 8 años y 8 meses) y quinto (M = 10 años y 8 meses) curso, emparejados mientras jugaban con el juego Lego (un material de construcción). Los resultados permitieron concluir lo siguiente: (a) en ambos cursos, cuando un sujeto se dirige al otro haciendo alguna alusión a la tarea, es altamente probable que este último se dirija al primer interlocutor inmediatamente después y, a la vez, se adapte al citado contenido semántico relacionado con la tarea; (b) en ambos cursos, el habla privada sobre la tarea les inhibe de comunicarse con el compañero sobre la tarea inmediatamente después; (c) en el tercer curso, el habla privada relacionada con la tarea favorece la prolongación de la ruptura de la comunicación interpersonal y el uso de habla interna por parte de ambos sujetos; y (d) los alumnos de quinto curso son más capaces de distinguir el habla privada del habla social que los de tercero.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bakeman, R., & Quera, V. (1995a). Analyzing interaction: Sequential analysis with SDIS and GSEQ. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Quera, V. (1995b). Log-linear approaches to lag-sequential analysis when consecutive codes may and cannot repeat. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 272284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berk, L.E., & Spuhl, S.T. (1995). Maternal interaction, private speech and task performance in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 145169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bricker, P.D., Garlock, C.M., Krauss, R.M., & McMahon, L.E. (1977). The role of audible and visible back-channel responses in interpersonal communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 295300.Google Scholar
Cattell, R.B., & Cattell, A.K.S. (1973). Measuring intelligence with The Culture Fair Tests (4th ed.). Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. [Spanish version: Tests de factor «g», Escalas 2 y 3. (1986). Madrid: TEA.]Google Scholar
Diaz, R.M. (1986). Issues in the empirical study of private speech: A response to Frawley and Lantolf's commentary. Developmental Psychology, 22, 709711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz, R.M. (1992). Methodological concerns in the study of private speech. In Diaz, R.M. & Berk, L.E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 5581). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Diaz, R.M., & Berk, L.E. (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Flavell, J.H. (1966). Le langage privé. Bulletin de Psychologie, 19, 698701. [Originally presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Speech and Hearing Association, San Francisco, November 1964.]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleiss, J.L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Fuson, K.C. (1979). The development of self-regulating aspects of speech: A review. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech (pp. 135217). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Girbau, D. (1996). Private and social speech in communication: Terminology and distinctive traits. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 507513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girbau, D. (1999). El estudio de la comunicación mediante la metodología observacional. In Anguera, M.T. (Ed.), Observación de la conducta interactiva en contextos naturales: Aplicaciones (pp. 243268). Barcelona: Edicions de la Universitat de Barcelona.Google Scholar
Girbau, D. (2002). Psicología de la comunicación. Barcelona: Ariel.Google Scholar
Girbau, D. (in press). Private and social speech in children's dyadic communication in naturalistic context. Anuario de Psicología, 33.Google Scholar
Haberman, (1978). Analysis of qualitative data (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Yaeger, J., & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: Four studies and a review of theories. Child Development, 39, 691736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R.M. (1987). The role of the listener: Addressee influences on message formulation. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 6, 8198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R.M., & Bricker, P.D. (1967). Effects of transmission delay and access delay on the efficiency of verbal communication. Journal of the Acoustical Society, 41, 286–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manning, B.H., White, C.S., & Daugherty, M. (1994). Young children's private speech as a precursor to metacognitive strategy use during task engagement. Discourse Processes, 17, 191211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meichenbaum, D., & Goodman, S. (1979). Clinical use of private speech and critical questions about its study in natural settings. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech (pp. 325360). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1968). Le langage et la pensée chez l'enfant. Études sur la logique de l'enfant [The language and thought of the child] (7th ed.). Neuchâtel, Switzerland: Delachaux et Niestlé. (Original work published 1923.)Google Scholar
Quera, V., & Bakeman, R. (2000). Quantification strategies in behavioral observation research. In Thompson, T., Felce, D., & Symons, F. (Eds.), Behavioral observation: Technology and applications in developmental disabilities (pp. 297315). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Sackett, G. P. (1979). The lag sequential analysis of contingency and cyclicity in behavioral interaction research. In Osofsky, J.D. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (1st ed., pp. 623649). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sackett, G.P. (1987). Analysis of sequential social interaction data: Some issues, recent developments, and a causal inference model. In Osofsky, J.D. (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 855878). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. Tokyo: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L.S. (1987). The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky: Vol. 1. Problems of General Psychology. Including the Volume Thinking and Speech. (Rieber, R.W. & Carton, A.S., Eds.; N. Minick, Trans.). New York: Plenum Press. (Original work published 1934.)Google Scholar
Ward, N., & Tsukahara, W. (2000). Prosodic features which cue back-channel responses in English and Japanese. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 11771207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar