Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T11:27:42.363Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural engagement and cognitive reserve: museum attendance and dementia incidence over a 10-year period

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 July 2018

Daisy Fancourt*
Affiliation:
Wellcome Research Fellow, Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, UK
Andrew Steptoe
Affiliation:
Professor, Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, UK
Dorina Cadar
Affiliation:
Research Associate, Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, UK
*
Correspondence: Daisy Fancourt, Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, 1–19 Torrington Place, London WC1E 7HB, UK. Email: d.fancourt@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Theories of cognitive reserve, disuse syndrome and stress have suggested that activities that are mentally engaging, enjoyable and socially interactive could be protective against the development of dementia. Using data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, this study shows that for adults aged 50 and older visiting museums every few months or more was associated with a lower incidence rate of dementia over a 10-year follow-up period compared with less-frequent visiting. This association was independent of demographics, socioeconomic status, health-related variables including sensory impairment, depression, vascular conditions and other forms of community engagement. Visiting museums may be a promising psychosocial activity to support the prevention of dementia.

Declaration of interest

None.

Type
Short reports
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2018

Neuropsychological and psychiatric studies have presented several theories on how dementia can be prevented or delayed. Theories of cognitive reserve suggest that complex and stimulating experiences can enhance neuronal structure and brain function, protecting against neurodegeneration.Reference Wu, Teale, Matthews, Brayne, Woods and Clare1 ‘Disuse syndrome’ proposes that a lack of cognitive stimulation in everyday life leads to faster deterioration in cognitive function.Reference Hultsch, Hertzog, Small and Dixon2 Similarly, studies on arousal and hedonic tone have shown that cognitive flexibility can be improved through positive affect,Reference Ashby, Isen and Turken3 demonstrating a bidirectional relationship between well-being and cognitive function.Reference Allerhand, Gale and Deary4 Moreover, social engagement has been found to support resilience, which has a neuroprotective effect.Reference Rafnsson, Orrell, d'Orsi, Hogervorst and Steptoe5 Consequently, activities that are mentally engaging, enjoyable, stress-reducing and socially interactive could be protective against the development of dementia, and in light of this, there has been a call for the identification of more affordable multimodal public health interventions to decrease the risk of dementia for individuals.Reference Hankey6

A multimodal activity that combines a number of protective factors (including intellectual stimulation, light physical activity, positive affect, relaxation and social engagement through interaction with staff, fellow visitors or friends) is visiting museums, art galleries and exhibitions (hereafter referred to as ‘museums’).Reference Wilson, Bennett, Bienias, Aggarwal, Mendes De Leon and Morris7Reference Camic and Chatterjee9 Previous studies have incorporated ‘visiting museums’ within definitions of leisure activities in dementia research,Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell10 or looked at the broader impact of attendance on people with dementia, finding benefits for cognitive performance, well-being, quality of life, self-esteem and social support.Reference Camic, Hulbert and Kimmel11 However, this is the first study (to the authors’ knowledge) to explore whether visiting museums among adults aged 50 and older is associated with a lower incidence rate of dementia over a 10-year period. The study has ethical approval from the National Research Ethics Service.

Method

Participants

We analysed data from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA): a large, nationally representative, multidisciplinary cohort study of the English population aged 50 and older.Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo12 We included participants who were core members in the study, who were free of dementia at baseline (wave 2) and who provided data across all variables of interest at 10-year follow-up (wave 7) (n = 3911).

Measures

Cultural engagement

Visiting museums, art galleries and exhibitions was measured using a self-report scale asking about frequency of engagement (‘never’, ‘less than once a year’, ‘about once or twice a year’, ‘every few months’, ‘about once a month’ or ‘twice a month or more’). We collapsed the final three categories together to provide an overall four-point scale.

Dementia

Dementia occurrence was determined at each wave by using an algorithm based on a combination of self- or informant-reported physician diagnosis of dementia or Alzheimer's disease, or a score above the threshold of 3.38 on the 16-question Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE). Memory complaints captured by IQCODE have previously been validated as a predictor of dementia.Reference Jorm, Masaki, Davis, Hardman, Nelson and Markesbery13

Covariates

Based on previous analyses, we identified a number of potentially confounding variables including demographic covariates (gender, age, marital status, educational attainment, employment, wealth and previous occupational classification), health-related covariates (eyesight, hearing, depression and existing cardiovascular health conditions) and community engagement (including membership of social clubs, arts or music groups, charities, church groups, volunteer networks, political or union groups, neighbourhood groups, environmental groups or sports clubs).

Statistical analyses

Incidence rates (IR) of dementia were computed per 1000 person-years in relation to museum visits frequency. We used Poisson regression analyses to calculate the incidence rate ratio (IRR) of dementia incidence and 95% CIs. Model 1 was unadjusted, model 2 adjusted for demographics, model 3 additionally adjusted for health-related factors, and model 4 was additionally adjusted for other indicators of community engagement. All analyses were weighted using baseline cross-sectional weights derived from ELSA to ensure the sample was representative of the English population. We applied three types of sensitivity analyses. To explore in more detail how age might affect the relationship observed, sensitivity analyses first included age as an interaction term, and then split participants into those above (51.6%) and below (48.4%) the age of 65. To confirm that analyses were not biased by the inclusion of participants already experiencing preclinical symptoms of dementia, we excluded all participants who developed dementia in the 2 years following baseline and re-ran analyses. Finally, in order to account for non-response on cultural participation (15.4%), we imputed missing data on cultural participation using chained equations, which included all health-related variables in the prediction model to generate 100 imputed data-sets (each had a final n = 4607). The missing-at-random assumption was strengthened by the fact that some of the same variables used to predict cultural engagement are also known to predict non-response in ELSA (including age, education and wealth).Reference Steptoe, Breeze, Banks and Nazroo12 Sensitivity analyses are shown in supplementary Tables 1–3 (available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2018.129). Analyses were carried out using Stata SE Version 14.1.

Results

Our sample included 3911 adults (45% male, 55% female; mean age 63.8 years, s.d. = 8.3), of whom 246 (6.3%) developed dementia during the 10-year study period. A total of 32.7% reported never attending an art gallery or museum, 26.8% reported attending less than once a year, 21.6% reported attending once or twice a year, and 18.9% reported attending every few months or more. The overall incidence rate was 5.42 (95% CI 4.78–6.17) per 1000 person-years. As anticipated, there was an above-average incidence rate for those who never visited (IR = 9.47, 95% CI 8.02–11.25), a slightly below-average rate for those who visited rarely (less than once a year: IR = 3.96, 95% CI 3.03–5.29; once or twice a year: IR = 3.73, 95% CI 2.70–5.30) and the lowest rate for those visited every few months or more (IR = 2.15, 95% CI 1.41–3.48).

The multivariable analyses are summarised in Table 1. Attending museums at any level of frequency was associated with a lower dementia incidence in unadjusted analyses. However, demographic variables accounted for much of this association, such that after this adjustment, the association only held for museum attendance every few months or more (IRR = 0.47, 95% CI 0.29–0.75). Further accounting for health-related variables and community engagement did not account for much more of the remaining association (IRR = 0.50, 95% CI 0.31–0.81 and IRR = 0.51, 95% CI 0.32–0.83, respectively).

Table 1 Associations between visiting art galleries and museums and dementia incidence

Results in bold are significant. Model 1: unadjusted. Model 2: adjusted for gender, age, marital status, educational attainment, employment, wealth and occupational classification. Model 3: additionally adjusted for eyesight, hearing, depression and existing cardiovascular health conditions. Model 4: additionally adjusted for community engagement.

Sensitivity analyses showed that age was a partial moderating factor (for every few months or more, IRR = 1.06, 95% CI 1.02–1.12) but examinations of the data for those above and below 65 showed similar patterns of association (age 50–64: IRR = 0.12, 95% CI 0.02–0.83; age ≥65: IRR = 0.64, 95% CI 0.39–1.03). Results were unaffected by omitting participants who might have been experiencing preclinical symptoms and developed dementia in the 2 years of follow-up (for every few months or more, IRR = 0.49, 95% CI 0.29–0.82) or by using multiple imputation to account for the 15.4% who were missing baseline data on cultural participation (n = 4607; for every few months or more, IRR = 0.58, 95% CI 0.37–0.92). In order to isolate visiting museums from other types of cognitive activities, we also ran correlational analyses with activities such as going to the theatre, concerts or opera and reading newspapers. Correlations were low, suggesting visiting museums is distinct from other cognitive activities, and including these further variables in the model did not affect the significance of results (but did reduce sample size because of missing data; data available from the authors on request).

Discussion

This study showed for the first time that among people who visit museums every few months or more, there is a lower incidence rate of dementia over a 10-year follow-up period. Although much of the association was explained by demographic and socioeconomic variables, it is notable that the relationship for more frequent engagement was maintained even when controlling for these confounders. The inclusion of further confounders such as health-related variables and other forms of community engagement had very little effect on results, suggesting that the association between visiting museums and dementia onset is independent of factors such as sensory impairment, depression and vascular conditions and separate from multiple further types of community social engagement. The combined neural and sensory stimulation and cognitive engagement provided by museums, make attendance a potential cultural intervention for increasing or maintaining cognitive reserve.Reference Stern14 Visiting museums is also a light physical activity so could reduce the negative effects of sedentary behaviours. Further, visiting museums can be seen as a specific type of social engagement: visiting can reduce perceived isolation by encouraging people to leave their homes, it is an activity that is frequently a focal point for meeting family/friends, and even if people attend alone, there is casual social contact with museum staff and/or other visitors. These findings therefore build on a previous study which examined social engagement as a determinant for cognitive reserve, demonstrating associations with better global cognition in early Parkinson's disease and lower risk of dementia incidence.Reference Hindle, Hurt, Burn, Brown, Samuel and Wilson15

This study has a number of strengths: through the extensive biennial monitoring of an extensive, nationally representative cohort study, we were able to measure 10-year dementia incidence. We considered non-response and attrition through the use of inverse probability weighting and multiple imputation. The comprehensive inclusion of covariates in ELSA meant that we could control for identified confounding variables. However, this study is observational rather than experimental and it is still possible that unidentified confounding might have affected these findings, or that selected variables might incompletely control for the constructs they represent. In particular, it is recognised that cultural engagement has a social gradient. However, our analyses included variables on education, wealth and socioeconomic classification, with results maintained independent of these factors, suggesting that going to museums is more than just a proxy for broader social factors already known to be protective against dementia. Further, our ascertainment of dementia incidence may have been underestimated because of recognised diagnostic challenges to date. Finally, it is recognised that subtle cognitive and behavioural changes may precede dementia diagnoses by more than a decade. Although our sensitivity analyses excluding participants who developed dementia in the 2 years following baseline showed no substantial changes to results, as more data become available it will be relevant to test associations between cultural engagement and dementia across longer timespans.

Further studies may wish to extend these findings by exploring more the cognitive mechanisms underlying the results reported here, considering the relationship between museum visits and incidence rates for more specific types of dementia as well as focusing in particular on subgroups at high risk, and exploring potential protective effects of other types of multimodal cultural activities. In conclusion, this novel analysis demonstrates that cultural participation through museum attendance could provide opportunities for interventions in older adults as a way of supporting engaged lifestyles to prevent dementia. Given museums number around 40 000 in Europe, the USA and Canada and reach diverse geographical populations and demographic groups, their potential could be explored further as sites for public health interventions.Reference Camic and Chatterjee9

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available online at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2018.129.

Funding

This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust Fellowship 205407/Z/16/Z and the National Institute on Aging (NIA Grant 528182). The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is funded by the NIA (Grant RO1AG7644) and by a consortium of UK government departments coordinated by the Economic and Social Research Council. The data are linked to the UK Data Archive and are freely available (https://discover.ukdataservice.ac.uk/catalogue). The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIA or any other funding bodies mentioned above.

References

1Wu, Y-T, Teale, J, Matthews, FE, Brayne, C, Woods, B, Clare, L. Lifestyle factors, cognitive reserve, and cognitive function: results from the Cognitive Function and Ageing Study Wales, a population-based cohort. Lancet 2016; 388: S114.Google Scholar
2Hultsch, DF, Hertzog, C, Small, BJ, Dixon, RA. Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging 1999; 14: 245–63.Google Scholar
3Ashby, FG, Isen, AM, Turken, AU. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol Rev 1999; 106: 529–50.Google Scholar
4Allerhand, M, Gale, CR, Deary, IJ. The dynamic relationship between cognitive function and positive well-being in older people: a prospective study using the English Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychol Aging 2014; 29: 306–18.Google Scholar
5Rafnsson, SB, Orrell, M, d'Orsi, E, Hogervorst, E, Steptoe, A. Loneliness, social integration, and incident dementia over 6 years: prospective findings from the english longitudinal study of ageing. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2017; June 27 (https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/advance-article/doi/10.1093/geronb/gbx087/3896175).Google Scholar
6Hankey, GJ. Public health interventions for decreasing dementia risk. JAMA Neurol 2018; 75: 1112.Google Scholar
7Wilson, RS, Bennett, DA, Bienias, JL, Aggarwal, NT, Mendes De Leon, CF, Morris, MC, et al. Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology 2002; 59: 1910–4.Google Scholar
8Wang, H-X, Karp, A, Winblad, B, Fratiglioni, L. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. Am J Epidemiol 2002; 155: 1081–7.Google Scholar
9Camic, PM, Chatterjee, HJ. Museums and art galleries as partners for public health interventions. Perspect Public Health 2013; 133: 6671.Google Scholar
10Yates, LA, Ziser, S, Spector, A, Orrell, M. Cognitive leisure activities and future risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr 2016; 28: 1791–806.Google Scholar
11Camic, PM, Hulbert, S, Kimmel, J. Museum object handling: a health-promoting community-based activity for dementia care. J Health Psychol 2017; Jan 1 (Epub ahead of print).Google Scholar
12Steptoe, A, Breeze, E, Banks, J, Nazroo, J. Cohort profile: the english longitudinal study of ageing. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42: 1640–8.Google Scholar
13Jorm, AF, Masaki, KH, Davis, DG, Hardman, J, Nelson, J, Markesbery, WR, et al. Memory complaints in nondemented men predict future pathologic diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2004; 63: 1960–1.Google Scholar
14Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol 2012; 11: 1006–12.Google Scholar
15Hindle, JV, Hurt, CS, Burn, DJ, Brown, RG, Samuel, M, Wilson, KC, et al. The effects of cognitive reserve and lifestyle on cognition and dementia in Parkinson's disease – a longitudinal cohort study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2016; 31: 1323.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Associations between visiting art galleries and museums and dementia incidence

Supplementary material: File

Fancourt et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Fancourt et al. supplementary material(File)
File 32.7 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.