Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T16:10:57.725Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Public mental health: An interdisciplinary subject?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Sarah L. Stewart-Brown*
Affiliation:
Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK. Email: sarah.stewart-brown@warwick.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

The Chief Medical Officer's report for 2013 was the first of its kind to highlight the public's mental rather than physical health and thus represents a very important landmark for public health in the UK. Written primarily from the perspective of psychiatrists, the report has created confusion in public health circles by failing to adequately address the public health perspective. David Foreman's editorial in this issue, calling as it does for more training in public health for psychiatrists, is therefore very welcome and timely.

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2015 

Publication, in the autumn of last year, of the Chief Medical Officer's (CMO's) Report for 2013 Public Mental Health Priorities: Investing in the Evidence Reference Davies1 makes David Foreman's editorial Reference Foreman2 in this issue very timely. He calls for psychiatric training to encompass more of the knowledge and skills of public health so that psychiatrists can better collaborate with public health colleagues and enable public mental health interventions to be effectively delivered. Such a training, he suggests, would need to encompass greater understanding of health intelligence and informatics, health economics and prevention science. He lists knowledge of mental health impact assessment and logical framework matrix (or logic modelling) as approaches that would enable psychiatrists to engage more easily. Training in evaluation and generation of evidence, he suggests, needs to extend beyond the classic randomised controlled trial (RCT) approach and allow for delivery in non-healthcare settings, detection of small effect sizes, use of proxy outcomes and successful implementation – now a science in its own right.

Psychiatrists' perspectives on public mental health

The CMO endorses the value of this new area of practice and the biopsychosocial model that underpins it, and recommends in Chapter 1 that commissioners in local authorities, health and well-being boards and clinical commissioning groups should commission for mental health promotion, mental illness prevention and treatment and rehabilitation. Reference Davies1 This very comprehensive 300 page report is co-authored by a large number of UK authorities in psychiatry, but interestingly and relevant from the point of view of Foreman's editorial, the lack of contribution from experts in public health (with one exception) is notable. So this is a report written mainly by psychiatrists for public health practitioners and policy makers. The wealth of information, succinctly summarised will be of great benefit in bringing public health professionals, as a whole, up to speed on the psychiatrist's perspective on public mental health. And this is to be welcomed because many of those practising in public health have ignored mental health for much too long.

However, there is no doubt that a public health perspective would have enhanced the report and the lack of it has resulted in some unbalanced recommendations. Chapter 3 on neuroscience and mental illness, for example, provides a very readable update on the advances in basic science that have contributed to our understanding of the mechanisms underpinning psychiatric disease. This chapter describes the potential for neuroimaging to detect patients at high risk of developing psychotic disorders, an attractive suggestion on the face of it, but at the same time one that would call for consideration of the principles of effective screening programmes. 3 To date screening has proved difficult to apply successfully in mental health and this is to be expected from the nature of psychiatric disease. Because symptoms of the latter are continuously distributed in the population, leaving no obvious cut-point between illness and health, it is unlikely that screening programmes could ever meet the essential criteria for effectiveness. There is no reason to suppose that improved technology could get around this fundamental issue.

Interventions to prevent psychopathology

Chapter 5, a fascinating read on the rapidly evolving science of developmental psychopathology, describes epigenetic studies showing the profound interaction of nature and nurture and even the intergenerational transmission of these epigenetic phenomena, all things that would have been considered impossible for most of the 20th century. It covers the central role of environmental factors in establishing the sensitivity of the stress response and thus life course resilience, and shows how these may operate through methylation of glucocorticoid receptor genes. And it puts the ‘attachment’ relationship – the parent (or carer's) sensitivity to the infant and child's experience – as central to these environmental influences. The discovery of epigenetic phenomena has had an important impact on thinking in public health because that which was previously thought to be genetic is now known to be amenable to intervention. The relevance of this chapter to public mental health therefore lies in the fact that it is possible to intervene to prevent psychopathology. But this chapter, and indeed the rest of the report, is remarkably devoid of information on the large body of research on programmes to support healthy parent–infant relationships and parenting more generally, and thus prevent psychopathology. 4 This research epitomises all the issues and challenges that arise in trying to apply the medical model RCT approach to complex intervention evaluation, but it has still managed to demonstrate a sufficient number of programmes to be effective for us to know this can be done. Public health professionals would have discussed this evidence base and offered information on what needs to be done to enable these interventions and programmes to be rolled out effectively at local level.

Promoting mental well-being

Chapter 2 contains the most controversial aspects of the CMO's report and concludes with the recommendation that commissioners in local authorities, health and well-being boards and clinical commissioning groups should not ‘commission well-being interventions as there is insufficient evidence to support this’. Reference Davies1 Public health professionals would have pointed out that the report only attempts to review and discuss evidence relating to mental health outcomes. As much of the justification for well-being interventions rests in the prevention of unhealthy lifestyles and physical diseases this recommendation cannot be justified.

The argument which is presented, relates only to the potential for well-being initiatives to have an impact on mental illness prevalence and rests on three suppositions that do not stand up under public health scrutiny.

The first supposition that the definition of mental well-being is still debated especially its relationship to psychiatric illness and its measurement is not secure. New concepts need to be debated and researched before opinions converge. This healthy process is well documented in the literature Reference Deci and Ryan5 as is also the coming together of the understanding that mental well-being is characterised by variation in affect – the hedonic perspective, and functioning – the eudaimonic perspective (‘feeling good and functioning well’). In this way it relates to mental illness that is characterised by problems in both aspects of living (feeling bad and functioning poorly). Public health research and development is very often based on proxy measures, and decision-making depends on a consistent story arising from studies using different methodologies and measures. The fact that different measures are giving similar answers strengthens rather than reduces the quality of evidence.

The second supposition is that the central hypothesis of the Foresight report 6 – namely that well-being interventions can prevent mental illness by shifting the curve – is unproved. Geoffrey Rose proposed that for health problems assessed by measures that are continuously distributed in the population, the population mean was closely related to the proportion of the population with scores indicating illness. Reference Rose and Day7 He showed that where this was true, a small shift in the population mean could achieve a large impact on the prevalence of the health problem, potentially greater than that achieved by treatment of those with the problem. This argument has been used to lobby for reduced salt levels in processed foods and increased taxation on alcohol. The veracity of the proposal has been demonstrated with regard to common mental disorders in both adult and child populations. Reference Veerman, Dowrick, Ayuso-Mateos, Dunn and Barendregt8,Reference Goodman and Goodman9 It is simply a matter of maths that if a measure is continuously distributed, and if you shift the mean without changing the shape or variance of the distribution, you will have an impact on the prevalence of the health problem. Exactly how much will depend on the shape of the distribution, which may vary with the measure employed, and how much it is possible to shift the curve. The only other question that needs to be answered then is ‘is it possible to change the population mean?’ The answer to this question can be found in studies of interventions in normal or universal populations and it is ‘yes’. These studies include the evidence that underpins National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) guidelines on the promotion of mental health in primary school children; Reference Weare and Nind10,Reference Weare and Nind11 studies of some parenting programmes; 4,Reference Stewart-Brown and Schrader-McMillan12,Reference Prinz, Sanders, Shapiro, Whitaker and Lutzker13 and interventions like mindfulness that have proved valuable in general population as well as clinical groups. Reference de Vibe, Bjørndal, Tipton, Hammerstrøm and Kowalski14 The latter include doctors, Reference Martine Asuero, Moix Queraltó, Pujol-Ribera, Berenguera, Rodriguez-Blanco and Epstein15 something perhaps of interest with regard to Chapter 10 of the CMO's report, which addresses the mental health of the medical profession.

Finally, the report suggests that ‘evaluations of well-being interventions are not sound’. This is undoubtedly true, as it is true for every subject in the public health and medical field, for some studies of well-being interventions. But is it curious to critique in detail one evaluation and ignore the trials and evaluations of mental health promotion interventions that are sound. Even allowing for all the issues that make RCTs so very challenging in this area, a significant positive RCT evidence base relating to key mental health promotion interventions (in parenting; 4,Reference Stewart-Brown and Schrader-McMillan12,Reference Prinz, Sanders, Shapiro, Whitaker and Lutzker13, schools; Reference Veerman, Dowrick, Ayuso-Mateos, Dunn and Barendregt8Reference Weare and Nind10 communities Reference Cattan, White, Bond and Learmouth16 and workplaces Reference Martine Asuero, Moix Queraltó, Pujol-Ribera, Berenguera, Rodriguez-Blanco and Epstein15 ) has accumulated and been extensively reviewed.

Conclusions

There is so much of value to public mental health in this report, Reference Davies1 that I thoroughly recommend colleagues in both public health and psychiatry find the time to read it. I also recommend that next year the CMO produces a report that does justice to the public health end of public mental health and the ever expanding evidence base for commissioning for mental well-being. But if that is not to be, enhancing public health training in psychiatry and mental health training in public health will hopefully create the mutual respect between the two disciplines on which this important interdisciplinary field can thrive.

References

1 Davies, S. Annual Report of the Chief Medical Officer 2013, Public Mental Health Priorities: Investing in the Evidence. Department of Health, 2014.Google Scholar
2 Foreman, DM. Psychiatry in public mental health: easy to say, but harder to achieve. Br J Psychiatry 2015; 207; 189–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 UK National Screening Committee. Programme Screening Criteria. UK Screening Portal, no date (http://www.screening.nhs.uk/criteria).Google Scholar
4 Department for Education. Finding a Parenting Programme. Database of Evidence Based Parenting Programmes. Department for Education, no date (http://www.education.gov.uk/commissioning-toolkit/Programme/CommissionersSearch).Google Scholar
5 Deci, EL, Ryan, RM. Hedonia, eudaimonia and well-being: an introduction. J Happiness Stud 2008; 9: 111 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6 The Government Office for Science. The Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project. Final Project Report. Government Office for Science, 2008.Google Scholar
7 Rose, G, Day, S. The population mean predicts the number of deviant individuals. BMJ 1990; 301: 1031–4.Google ScholarPubMed
8 Veerman, JL, Dowrick, C, Ayuso-Mateos, JL, Dunn, G, Barendregt, JJ. Population prevalence of depression and mean Beck Depression Inventory score. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 516–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Goodman, A, Goodman, R. Population mean scores predict child mental disorder rates: validation SDQ prevalence estimators in Britain. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52: 100–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Weare, K, Nind, M. Mental health promotion and problem prevention in schools: what does the evidence say? Health Promot Int 2011; 26 (suppl 1): i2969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Weare, K, Nind, M. Mental health promotion and problem prevention in schools: what does the evidence say? Health Promot Int 2011; 26 (suppl 1): i2969 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Stewart-Brown, SL, Schrader-McMillan, A. Parenting for mental health: what does the evidence say we need to do? Report of Workpackage 2 of the DataRev project. Health Promot Int 2011; 26 (suppl 1): i1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13 Prinz, RJ, Sanders, MR, Shapiro, CJ, Whitaker, DJ, Lutzker, JR. Population-based prevention of child maltreatment: The U.S. Triple P system population trial. Prev Sci 2009; 10: 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 de Vibe, M, Bjørndal, A, Tipton, E, Hammerstrøm, K, Kowalski, K. Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) for improving health, quality of life, and social functioning in adults. Campbell Syst Rev 2012: 3: 1127.Google Scholar
15 Martine Asuero, A, Moix Queraltó, J, Pujol-Ribera, E, Berenguera, A, Rodriguez-Blanco, T, Epstein, RM. Effectiveness of a mindfulness education program in primary health care professionals: a pragmatic controlled trial. J Contin Educ Health Prof 2014; 34; 412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16 Cattan, M, White, M, Bond, J, Learmouth, A. Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: a systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing Soc 2005; 25: 4167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.