Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T05:52:19.849Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The color of night: Surface color perception under dim illuminations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2006

JOEL POKORNY
Affiliation:
Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
MARGARET LUTZE
Affiliation:
Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
DINGCAI CAO
Affiliation:
Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois Department of Health Studies, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
ANDREW J. ZELE
Affiliation:
Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois

Abstract

Several studies document rudimentary color vision under dim illumination. Here, hue perceptions of paper color samples were determined for a wide range of light levels, including very low light levels where rods alone mediate vision. The appearances of 24 paper color samples from the OSA Uniform Color Scales were gauged under successively dimmer illuminations from 10–0.0003 Lux. Triads of samples were chosen representing each of eight basic color categories; red, pink, orange, yellow, green, blue, purple, and gray. Samples within each triad varied in lightness. Observers sorted samples into groups that they could categorize with specific color names. Above 0.32 Lux, observers sorted the samples into the originally chosen color groups with few exceptions. For 0.1–0.01 Lux, the red and orange samples were usually correctly identified as either red or orange. The remaining samples tended to be grouped into two categories, associated with the scotopic sample reflectance. The lowest reflectance samples were below threshold and were named black. The higher reflectance group was named predominately as green or blue-green (three observers; the fourth observer used blue or achromatic). At the three dimmest levels (≤ 0.0032 Lux) there continued to be conspicuous color percepts. Color categories were reliably assigned based on relative sample scotopic lightness. Of the samples above threshold, those with lower reflectance were classified as red or orange (all observers) and the higher reflectance samples as green or blue-green (three observers) or achromatic or blue (the fourth observer). Rods and L-cones presumably mediated color percepts at the intermediate light levels used in the study. At the three lowest light levels there were distinct color appearances mediated exclusively by rods. We speculate that at these light levels the visual system estimates probable colors based on prior natural experience.

Type
ROD-CONE INTERACTION
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

ASTM. (1994). Standards on Color and Appearance Measurement. Philadelphia: American Society for Testing and Materials.
Boynton, R.M. & Olson, C.X. (1987). Locating basic colors in the OSA space. Color Research and Application 12, 94105.Google Scholar
Brown, W.R.J. (1951). The influence of luminance level on visual sensitivity to color differences. Journal of the Optical Society of America 41, 684688.Google Scholar
Buck, S.L. (2004). Rod-cone interactions in human vision. In The Visual Neurosciences, eds. Chalupa, L.M. & Werner, J.S., pp. 863878. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cao, D., Pokorny, J., & Smith, V.C. (2005a). Associating color appearance with the cone chromaticity space. Vision Research 45, 19291934.Google Scholar
Cao, D., Pokorny, J., & Smith, V.C. (2005b). Matching rod percepts with cone stimuli. Vision Research 45, 21192128.Google Scholar
CIE. (1951). Proceedings 1951, Vol. 3, p. 37, Paris: Bureau Central de la CIE.
Hecht, S. & Hsia, Y. (1945). Dark adaptation following light adaptation to red and white lights. Journal Optical Society of America 35, 261267.Google Scholar
Ishida, T. (2002). Color identification data obtained from photopic to mesopic illuminance levels. Color Research & Application 27, 252259.Google Scholar
Kohlrausch, A. (1931). Tagesehen, Dämmersehen, Adaptation. In Handbuch der Normalen und Pthologischen Physiologie, eds. Bethe, A., Bergmann, G.V., Embden, G. & Ellinger, A., pp. 14991594. Berlin: Springer.
Kuehni, R.G. (2003). Color Space and Its Divisions, pp. 1408. New York: Wiley-Interscience.
MacAdam, D.L. (1978). Colorimetric Data for samples of OSA uniform color scales. Journal of the Optical Society of America 68, 121130.Google Scholar
McCann, J.J. & Benton, J.L. (1969). Interaction of the long-wave cones and the rods to produce color sensations. Journal of the Optical Society of America 59, 103107.Google Scholar
Middleton, W.E.K. & Mayo, E.G. (1952). The appearance of colors in twilight. Journal of the Optical Society of America 42, 116121.Google Scholar
Nagel, W. (1924). Appendix: Adaptation, Twilight Vision and the Duplicity Theory. In Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics Translated from the Third German Edition by JPC Southall, Third German Edition, Vol. 2, pp. 313343. Rochester, NY: Optical Society of America.
Purkinje, J. (1825). Beobachtungen und Versuche zur Physiologie der Sinne. Neue Beiträge zur Kenntniss des Sehens in subjectiver Hinsicht, pp. 1192. Berlin: Reimer.
Schneider, N. & Von Campenhausen, C. (1998). Color and lightness constancy in different perceptual tasks. Biological Cybernetics 79, 445455.Google Scholar
Shin, J.C., Yaguchi, H., & Shioiri, S. (2004). Change of color appearance in photopic, mesopic and scotopic vision. Optical Review 11, 265271.Google Scholar
Simon, R. (1904). Über Fixation im Dämmerungssehn. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnersorgane 36, 186193.Google Scholar
Smith, V.C. & Pokorny, J. (1975). Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm. Vision Research 15, 161171.Google Scholar
Verriest, G., Buyssens, A., & Vanderdonck, R. (1963). Etude quantitative de l'effet qu'exerce sur les resultats de quelques tests de la discrimination chromatique une diminution non selective du niveau d'un eclairage c. Revue d'Optique 3, 105119.Google Scholar
Wald, G. (1945). Human vision and spectrum. Science 101, 653658.Google Scholar
Walkey, H.C., Barbur, J.L., Harlow, J.A., & Makous, W. (2001). Measurements of chromatic sensitivity in the mesopic range. Color Research & Application 26, S36S42.Google Scholar