Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-wq2xx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T03:15:17.515Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comparison of neural substrates of temporal discounting between youth with autism spectrum disorder and with obsessive-compulsive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 April 2017

C. O. Carlisi*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
L. Norman
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
C. M. Murphy
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK Behavioural Genetics Clinic, Adult Autism Service, Behavioural and Developmental Psychiatry Clinical Academic Group, South London and Maudsley Foundation NHS Trust, London, UK
A. Christakou
Affiliation:
Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK
K. Chantiluke
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
V. Giampietro
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
A. Simmons
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) for Mental Health at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Clinical Geriatrics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
M. Brammer
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
D. G. Murphy
Affiliation:
Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK Behavioural Genetics Clinic, Adult Autism Service, Behavioural and Developmental Psychiatry Clinical Academic Group, South London and Maudsley Foundation NHS Trust, London, UK
D. Mataix-Cols
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Centre for Psychiatry Research, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
K. Rubia
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College, London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: C. Carlisi, BA, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, 16 DeCrespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. (Email: carlisi.christina@gmail.com)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) share abnormalities in hot executive functions such as reward-based decision-making, as measured in the temporal discounting task (TD). No studies, however, have directly compared these disorders to investigate common/distinct neural profiles underlying such abnormalities. We wanted to test whether reward-based decision-making is a shared transdiagnostic feature of both disorders with similar neurofunctional substrates or whether it is a shared phenotype with disorder-differential neurofunctional underpinnings.

Methods

Age and IQ-matched boys with ASD (N = 20), with OCD (N = 20) and 20 healthy controls, performed an individually-adjusted functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) TD task. Brain activation and performance were compared between groups.

Results

Boys with ASD showed greater choice-impulsivity than OCD and control boys. Whole-brain between-group comparison revealed shared reductions in ASD and OCD relative to control boys for delayed-immediate choices in right ventromedial/lateral orbitofrontal cortex extending into medial/inferior prefrontal cortex, and in cerebellum, posterior cingulate and precuneus. For immediate-delayed choices, patients relative to controls showed reduced activation in anterior cingulate/ventromedial prefrontal cortex reaching into left caudate, which, at a trend level, was more decreased in ASD than OCD patients, and in bilateral temporal and inferior parietal regions.

Conclusions

This first fMRI comparison between youth with ASD and with OCD, using a reward-based decision-making task, shows predominantly shared neurofunctional abnormalities during TD in key ventromedial, orbital- and inferior fronto-striatal, temporo-parietal and cerebellar regions of temporal foresight and reward processing, suggesting trans-diagnostic neurofunctional deficits.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is characterized by social communication difficulties and stereotyped repetitive behaviours (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) with a prevalence of 0.6–2%, predominantly in males (Blumberg et al. Reference Blumberg, Bramlett, Kogan, Schieve, Jones and Lu2013). Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) involves recurrent, intrusive and distressing thoughts (obsessions) and repetitive rituals (compulsions) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), affecting 1–3% of the population with a higher male prevalence in children (Ruscio et al. Reference Ruscio, Stein, Chiu and KessleR2010). These disorders are highly comorbid, with rates exceeding 30% (Simonoff et al. Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird2008) and can sometimes be clinically difficult to separate (Doshi-Velez et al. Reference Doshi-Velez, Ge and Kohane2014).

The allowance of co-diagnosis of OCD with ASD in DSM-5 questions whether phenotypes common to both disorders are mediated by shared or disorder-specific mechanisms. Characteristic behaviours observed in ASD are wide-ranging and heterogeneous but can include physical rocking, tapping, counting and behavioural inflexibility (e.g. insistence on performing actions in a certain order). Similarly, behaviours in OCD vary widely, but compulsions often include hand-washing, checking, and, sometimes seemingly similar to ASD, counting and behavioural inflexibility surrounding order and symmetry. It has been hypothesized that in both cases, these behaviours may relate to abnormalities in fronto-striatal circuitry that is also important in reward-based decision-making (Langen et al. Reference Langen, Durston, Kas, Van Engeland and Staal2011). In ASD, repetitive behaviours are often considered soothing and rewarding, while in OCD, compulsions are performed to reduce anxiety and are often debilitating. However, despite this distinction, converging evidence suggests repetitive behaviours in ASD and OCD may be mediated by shared mechanisms including behavioural disinhibition or motivation control (Hollander et al. Reference Hollander, Kim, Khanna and Pallanti2007). Such impairments may maintain diminished control over repetitive behaviours in ASD and compulsions in OCD and involve goal-directed reward-based decision-making. A meta-analysis of structural and functional neuroimaging studies comparing ASD and OCD found shared reduced structure and function during cognitive control in medial prefrontal regions but that OCD had disorder-specific increased function and structure in basal ganglia and insula while ASD had disorder-specific functional reduction in DLPFC and reduced PCC deactivation, presumably reflecting disorder-specific fronto-striato-insular dysregulation in OCD but fronto-striato-insular maldevelopment in ASD, both underpinned by shared reduced prefrontal control (Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Norman, Lukito, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2016b ).

Both disorders also share deficits in motivated ‘hot’ executive functions (EF) (Zelazo & Müller, Reference Zelazo, Müller and Goswami2007) including reward-based decision-making measured by choice-impulsivity tasks of gambling and temporal discounting (TD) (Hill, Reference Hill2004; Sanders et al. Reference Sanders, Johnson, Garavan, Gill and Gallagher2008; Abramovitch et al. Reference Abramovitch, Abramowitz and Mittelman2013; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Chien, Wu, Shang, Wu and Gau2016). TD requires choosing between small immediate rewards and larger later rewards, assessing the extent to which a reward is subjectively discounted when delayed in time (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009). The ability to inhibit immediate reward choices and wait for larger rewards depends on well-developed frontal lobe-mediated motivation control and temporal foresight and is a key for mature decision-making. A TD function is typically hyperbolic, with steeper rates reflecting more impulsive choice behaviour (Richards et al. Reference Richards, Zhang, Mitchell and Wit1999) (see online Supplement). TD matures with age (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Steinbeis et al. Reference Steinbeis, Haushofer, Fehr and Singer2016) and varies among individuals (Odum, Reference Odum2011), with steeper TD observed in younger people and individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and related impulsive disorders (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009; Noreika et al. Reference Noreika, Falter and Rubia2013). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of TD in healthy adults and children implicate ventromedial-fronto-limbic networks of reward-based decision-making and dorsolateral and inferior-fronto-insula-striato-parietal networks of temporal foresight (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ; Wesley & Bickel, Reference Wesley and Bickel2014).

People with ASD have been shown to have deficits in reward-motivated and forward-thinking behaviour including reward processing and reversal learning (Scott-Van Zeeland et al. Reference Scott-Van Zeeland, Dapretto, Ghahremani, Poldrack and Bookheimer2010; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons, Murphy and Rubia2015a ), incentive processing (Dichter et al. Reference Dichter, Richey, Rittenberg, Sabatino and Bodfish2012), planning (Ozonoff & Jensen, Reference Ozonoff and Jensen1999; Geurts et al. Reference Geurts, Verté, Oosterlaan, Roeyers and Sergeant2004; Hill, Reference Hill2004) and TD (Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ). However, there have also been negative findings (Antrop et al. Reference Antrop, Stock, Verté, Wiersema, Baeyens and Roeyers2006; Demurie et al. Reference Demurie, Roeyers, Baeyens and Sonuga-Barke2013). ASD is characterized by fronto-temporo-limbic abnormalities mediating socio-emotional processes (Via et al. Reference Via, Radua, Cardoner, Happé and Mataix-Cols2011; Philip et al. Reference Philip, Dauvermann, Whalley, Baynham, Lawrie and Stanfield2012; Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Norman, Lukito, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2016b ), and in ventromedial/fronto-limbic brain regions involved in TD (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Peters & Büchel, Reference Peters and Büchel2011) during reward-related and planning tasks (Just et al. Reference Just, Cherkassky, Keller, Kana and Minshew2007; Schmitz et al. Reference Schmitz, Rubia, Van Amelsvoort, Daly, Smith and Murphy2008; Dichter et al. Reference Dichter, Richey, Rittenberg, Sabatino and Bodfish2012; Kohls et al. Reference Kohls, Schulte-Rüther, Nehrkorn, Müller, Fink, Kamp-Becker, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Schultz and Konrad2013). However, only one fMRI study has been published investigating the neural correlates of TD in adolescents with ASD, which found a weaker relationship between task-performance and bilateral superior temporal and right insular activation relative to controls (Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ).

Patients with OCD show deficits during planning (van den Heuvel et al. Reference Van Den Heuvel, Mataix-Cols, Zwitser, Cath, Van Der Werf, Groenewegen, Van Balkom and Veltman2011; Shin et al. Reference Shin, Lee, Kim and Kwon2014), goal-directed learning (Gillan & Robbins, Reference Gillan and Robbins2014; Voon et al. Reference Voon, Derbyshire, Ruck, Irvine, Worbe, Enander, Schreiber, Gillan, Fineberg, Sahakian, Robbins, Harrison, Wood, Daw, Dayan, Grant and Bullmore2015), reward-based decision-making, gambling (Grassi et al. Reference Grassi, Pallanti, Righi, Figee, Mantione, Denys, Piccagliani, Rossi and Stratta2015; Figee et al. Reference Figee, Pattij, Willuhn, Luigjes, Van Den Brink, Goudriaan, Potenza, Robbins and Denys2016), and incentive processing (Figee et al. Reference Figee, Vink, De Geus, Vulink, Veltman, Westenberg and Denys2011). Despite evidence that heightened impulsivity is a phenotype associated with OCD (Benatti et al. Reference Benatti, Dell'osso, Arici, Hollander and Altamura2014), only one (Sohn et al. Reference Sohn, Kang, Namkoong and Kim2014) of three TD studies in OCD (Vloet et al. Reference Vloet, Marx, Kahraman-Lanzerath, Zepf, Herpertz-Dahlmann and Konrad2010; Pinto et al. Reference Pinto, Steinglass, Greene, Weber and Simpson2014; Sohn et al. Reference Sohn, Kang, Namkoong and Kim2014) found performance deficits.

Neuroimaging studies show that OCD is characterized by structural and functional abnormalities in medial and orbitofronto-striato-thalamo-cortical networks mediating EF (Menzies et al. Reference Menzies, Chamberlain, Laird, Thelen, Sahakian and Bullmore2008; Radua et al. Reference Radua, Van Den Heuvel, Surguladze and Mataix-Cols2010; Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Norman, Lukito, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2016b ; Norman et al. Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols, Radua and Rubia2016). No fMRI studies, however, have investigated TD in OCD. Studies using other decision-making tasks in OCD have found hyperactivity in ventral-affective regions including ventromedial prefrontal, orbitofrontal and rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) projecting to ventral striatum and mediodorsal thalamus, and hypoactivity in dorsal-cognitive cortico-striato-thalamic regions including dorsolateral prefrontal (DLPFC), temporal and parietal association cortex projecting to the dorsal striatum and caudate in patients relative to controls (Menzies et al. Reference Menzies, Chamberlain, Laird, Thelen, Sahakian and Bullmore2008; Brem et al. Reference Brem, Hauser, Iannaccone, Brandeis, Drechsler and Walitza2012). Hypoactivation in DLPFC and caudate has furthermore been shown in OCD patients during planning (van den Heuvel et al. Reference van den Heuvel, Veltman, Groenewegen, Cath, van Balkom, van Hartskamp, Barkhof and van Dyck2005, Reference Van Den Heuvel, Mataix-Cols, Zwitser, Cath, Van Der Werf, Groenewegen, Van Balkom and Veltman2011).

This suggests that ASD and OCD have abnormalities during planning and ‘hot’ EF tasks including reward-based decision-making, and that this may be underpinned by ventromedial and dorsolateral prefronto-striato-limbic abnormalities. However, it is unclear whether reward-based decision-making problems in both disorders are underpinned by shared trans-diagnostic mechanisms or by disorder-specific underlying abnormalities.

We hypothesized that adolescents with ASD would be more impaired on TD relative to adolescents with OCD and controls (Scott-Van Zeeland et al. Reference Scott-Van Zeeland, Dapretto, Ghahremani, Poldrack and Bookheimer2010; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Chien, Wu, Shang, Wu and Gau2016) and that both clinical groups compared with healthy controls would show underactivation in underlying ventromedial prefrontal, limbic and striatal regions mediating TD (Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Potenza, Chamberlain, Berlin, Menzies, Bechara, Sahakian, Robbins, Bullmore and Hollander2009), reflecting a trans-diagnostic neurofunctional phenotype (Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons, Murphy and Rubia2015a ; Grassi et al. Reference Grassi, Pallanti, Righi, Figee, Mantione, Denys, Piccagliani, Rossi and Stratta2015; Chen et al. Reference Chen, Chien, Wu, Shang, Wu and Gau2016). However, we hypothesized that people with OCD would show disorder-specific (ventro)medial and dorsolateral-prefrontal dysfunction (Menzies et al. Reference Menzies, Chamberlain, Laird, Thelen, Sahakian and Bullmore2008; Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Norman, Lukito, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2016b ; Norman et al. Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols, Radua and Rubia2016) while ASD adolescents would show disorder-specific insular and temporo-parietal dysfunction compared to controls (Di Martino et al. Reference Di Martino, Ross, Uddin, Sklar, Castellanos and Milham2009; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ; Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Norman, Lukito, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2016b ).

Methods

Participants

Sixty-nine right-handed (Oldfield, Reference Oldfield1971) boys (20 controls, 29 boys with ASD, 20 boys with OCD), 11–17 years, IQ ⩾ 70 (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999) participated. Medication–naïve boys with high-functioning ASD were recruited from local clinics and support-groups. ASD diagnosis was made by a consultant psychiatrist using ICD-10 research diagnostic criteria (WHO, 1992) and confirmed with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [ADI-R; (Lord et al. Reference Lord, Rutter and Couteur1994)]. The ADI-R and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule [ADOS; (Lord et al. Reference Lord, Risi, Lambrecht, Cook, Leventhal, Dilavore, Pickles and Rutter2000)] were completed for all ASD boys; all 29 reached autism cut-offs on all ADI-R (social/communication/restricted/stereotyped) and ADOS (communication/social) domains. ASD participants either fulfilled ICD-10 research diagnostic criteria for autism (N = 7) or fulfilled these criteria but had no history of language delay and therefore were subtyped with Asperger's syndrome (N = 22). Parents of ASD boys completed the Social Communication Questionnaire [SCQ; (Rutter et al. Reference Rutter, Bailey and Lord2003)] and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [SDQ; (Goodman & Scott, Reference Goodman and Scott1999)] (see online Supplement). ASD participants had a physical examination to exclude comorbid medical disorders and biochemical, haematological and chromosomal abnormalities associated with ASD. None of the ASD individuals had a comorbid diagnosis of OCD or any psychiatric disorder, and none of the OCD patients had comorbid ASD.

OCD boys were recruited from National and Specialist OCD clinics. Diagnosis was made by a consultant psychiatrist using ICD-10 criteria and confirmed by the Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale [CY-BOCS; (Goodman et al. Reference Goodman, Price, Rasmussen, Mazure, Delgado, Heninger and Charney1989)]. Parents of OCD patients completed the SDQ. Patients with comorbid psychiatric or neurological disorders, including ASD, were not included in the OCD sample, although OCD patients were not specifically assessed for ASD. Four boys were prescribed stable doses of antidepressants (see online Supplement).

Twenty age and handedness-matched healthy controls were recruited locally by advertisement. Controls scored below clinical threshold on the SDQ and SCQ for any disorder and did not have any psychiatric condition.

Exclusion criteria for all participants included comorbid psychiatric or medical disorders affecting brain development (e.g. epilepsy/psychosis), drug/alcohol dependency, head injury, genetic conditions associated with ASD, abnormal structural brain scan and MRI contraindications. All controls also participated in previously published studies testing fluoxetine effects on TD in ADHD (Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Chantiluke, Norman, Christakou, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons and Rubia2016a ) and neurofunctional maturation of TD in healthy adults and adolescents (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011); all but four ASD boys participated in our fMRI TD study comparing ASD and ADHD (Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ). Most ASD and control participants also participated in other fMRI tasks during their visit, published elsewhere (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Gershman, Niv, Simmons, Brammer and Rubia2013a , Reference Christakou, Murphy, Chantiluke, Cubillo, Smith, Giampietro, Daly, Ecker, Robertson and Murphy2013b ; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons and Rubia2014a , Reference Chantiluke, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons, Murphy and Rubia2015a , Reference Chantiluke, Barrett, Giampietro, Santosh, Brammer, Simmons, Murphy and Rubia b ; Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Christakou, Daly, Ecker, Giampietro, Brammer, Smith, Johnston, Robertson, Consortium, Murphy and Rubia2014).

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approval was obtained from the local Research Ethics Committee (05/Q0706/275). Study details were explained to child and guardian, and written informed consent was obtained for all participants.

TD paradigm

Prior to scanning, subjects practiced the 12-min TD task (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009; Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ) in a mock-scanner. Subjects chose by pressing a left/right button with right index/middle-finger between receiving a small amount of money immediately (£0-£100) or receiving £100 in 1 week, month or year (Fig. 1). Delays (20 trials each) were randomized, but the delayed option (£100) was consistently displayed on the right side of the screen, and variable immediate choices on the left, minimizing sensorimotor mapping effects. Choices were displayed for 4 s, followed by a blank screen of at least 8 s (inter-trial-interval:12 s). The immediate reward amount was adjusted through an algorithm based on previous choices and calculated separately for each delay. This narrows the range of values, converging on an indifference point where the immediate reward is subjectively considered equivalent to the delayed amount for the given delay (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009), ensuring comparable numbers of immediate and delayed choices for analysis.

Fig. 1. Schematic of the temporal discounting fMRI paradigm. Subjects are asked to indicate whether they would prefer a small, variable amount of money immediately (immediate reward), or whether they would rather wait for a larger delay (up to £100) later (delayed reward). An algorithm adjusts the amount of the immediate reward offered based on the choices of the participant, so as to determine the lowest immediate reward they would tolerate before instead choosing to wait for the larger delayed reward. Three hypothetical delays are presented in random order: 1 week, 1 month and 1 year. Each delay choice is presented 20 times. Trials start with the presentation of the choice display, which remains available for 4 s, within which the subject must choose between the immediate (always on left side) and delayed (always on right) rewards. Total trial duration is 12 s.

Analysis of performance data

To estimate TD steepness for each subject, indifference values between the immediate amount and delayed £100 for each delay were calculated, equal to the participant's subjective value of £100 after each delay and defined as the midpoint between the lowest chosen immediate reward and the next lowest immediate reward available (i.e. the value of the immediate reward offered at which point the subject began to choose the delayed reward) (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011).

TD was measured using area under the curve (AUC) (Myerson et al. Reference Myerson, Green and Warusawitharana2001). Smaller AUC denotes steeper discounting rates (i.e. increased choice-impulsivity) (see online Supplement).

One-way between-group analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted with AUC as dependent measure to examine group-differences.

fMRI image acquisition

Gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) data were acquired at King's College London on a 3T-General Electric SIGNA HDx MRI scanner (Milwaukee, WI) using the body coil for radio frequency transmission and a quadrature birdcage head coil for reception. See online Supplement for acquisition parameters. Total scan was 1.5 h during which subjects completed 2–3 additional fMRI tasks.

fMRI image analysis

Event-related data were acquired in randomized trial presentation and analysed using the non-parametric XBAM package (v4.1) [www.brainmap.co.uk; (Brammer et al. Reference Brammer, Bullmore, Simmons, Williams, Grasby, Howard, Woodruff and Rabe-Hesketh1997)]. The individual and group-level analysis methods are described in detail elsewhere (Brammer et al. Reference Brammer, Bullmore, Simmons, Williams, Grasby, Howard, Woodruff and Rabe-Hesketh1997; Bullmore et al. Reference Bullmore, Suckling, Overmeyer, Rabe-Hesketh, Taylor and Brammer1999b ; Cubillo et al. Reference Cubillo, Smith, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons and Rubia2014) and in the online Supplement.

Briefly, fMRI data were realigned to minimize motion-related artefacts and smoothed using a 7.2 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter (Bullmore et al. Reference Bullmore, Brammer, Rabe-Hesketh, Curtis, Morris, Williams, Sharma and Mcguire1999a ). Time-series analysis of individual activation was performed with a wavelet-based resampling method (Bullmore et al. Reference Bullmore, Long, Suckling, Fadili, Calvert, Zelaya, Carpenter and Brammer2001). The main experimental conditions were convolved with 2 Poisson model functions (peaking at 4 and 8 s). The weighted sum of these convolutions giving the best fit (least-squares) to the time series at each voxel was calculated. A goodness-of-fit statistic (SSQ ratio) was then computed at each voxel consisting of the ratio of the sum of squares of deviations from the mean intensity value due to the model (fitted time series) divided by that of the squares due to the residuals (original minus model time series). This statistic, the SSQ ratio, was used in further analyses. Individual maps were then normalised to Talairach space (Talairach & Tournoux, Reference Talairach and Tournoux1988), and a group activation map was produced for each group.

ANCOVA of between-group effects

One-way between-group analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with age as covariate was conducted using randomization-based testing to investigate case-control differences (Bullmore et al. Reference Bullmore, Suckling, Overmeyer, Rabe-Hesketh, Taylor and Brammer1999b , Reference Bullmore, Long, Suckling, Fadili, Calvert, Zelaya, Carpenter and Brammer2001). For these comparisons, statistical thresholds of 0.05 (voxel-level)/0.015 (cluster-level) were selected to obtain <1 false-positive 3D cluster per map. Standardized blood-oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) responses were extracted from significant clusters for each participant and plotted to determine effect direction. Post-hoc significance was determined among pairwise comparisons using a one-way ANOVA.

Influence of behaviour, symptoms and medication

To examine whether clusters showing significant group effects were related to TD performance or symptoms, BOLD response from these clusters was extracted for each participant and Spearman correlations (two-tailed) were performed with AUC and symptom subscales within each group. FMRI analyses were also repeated including AUC as covariate.

Lastly, analyses were repeated excluding the four OCD participants prescribed medication.

Results

Participants

There were no significant group-differences in age and IQ (Table 1). Multivariate ANOVAs showed group-differences on SDQ scores; Post-hoc tests revealed that patients had higher total-scores than controls, with ASD being more impaired than OCD patients (all p < 0.001). On the emotional-distress subscale, both patient groups were more impaired than controls (p < 0.001) but did not differ from each other. On all other SDQ subscales, ASD patients were significantly more impaired than controls and OCD patients (all p < 0.005), who did not differ on any measure, with the exception of the conduct subscale where ASD patients differed from controls only (p < 0.001).

Table 1. Participant characteristics for healthy control boys and patients with OCD or ASD

ADI, autism diagnostic interview; ADOS, autism diagnostic observation schedule; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; CY-BOCS, Children's Yale-Brown obsessive-compulsive symptoms checklist; HC, healthy controls; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SCQ, social communication questionnaire; SDQ, strengths and difficulties questionnaire.

Performance

AUC correlated inversely with k (as measured by the square-root transform of these values: r = −0.555, p < 0.001), suggesting adequate congruency between these two metrics. AUC differed between groups [controls: 0.56 ± 0.13; ASD: 0.45 ± 0.24; OCD: 0.59 ± 0.15; F(2,66) = 4.04, p = 0.02]. Post-hoc comparisons showed that ASD patients had significantly smaller AUC compared with controls (p < 0.05) and OCD patients (p < 0.01), indicating ASD patients discounted rewards more steeply than the other groups, who did not differ from each other.

fMRI data

Movement

Multivariate ANOVA showed no group-differences in mean head rotation [F(2,66) = 1.17, p = n.s.] or translation [F(2,66) = 2.59, p = n.s.] in 3-dimensional Euclidian space.

Group maps of brain activation for delayed-immediate choices

See online Supplement for maps of brain activation within each group for the contrast of delayed-immediate choices (online Supplementary Fig. S1).

Group-effects on brain activation

One-way ANOVA showed a significant group-effect for delayed-immediate choices in right ventromedial orbitofrontal cortex (vmOFC) extending into MPFC/lateral OFC/inferior frontal cortex (IFC), in cerebellum extending into occipital lobe/posterior cingulate (PCC)/precuneus, in rACC/vmPFC extending into left caudate, in left superior/middle temporal lobe (STL/MTL)/inferior parietal lobe (IPL) and in right MTL/STL extending into posterior insula/postcentral gyrus/IPL (Fig. 2a ; Table 2). ANCOVA including AUC as covariate showed that effects in rACC/vmPFC and PCC/precuneus were related to task performance.

Fig. 2. Between-group activation differences for delayed minus immediate choices. (a) Axial slices showing split-plot analysis of variance (ANOVA) effects of group on brain activation to delayed – immediate choices. Talairach Z coordinates are indicated for slice distance (in mm) from the intercommissural line. The right side of the image corresponds to the right side of the brain. (b) Extracted statistical measures of BOLD response are shown for each of the three groups for each of the brain regions that showed a significant group effect. Black asterisks indicate a significant difference between controls and patient group. Red asterisk indicates a difference between the two patient groups. (*) = significant at a trend level; * = significant at the p < 0.05 level; ** = significant at the p ⩽ 0.005 level; *** = significant at the p ⩽ 0.001 level.

Table 2. Between-group activation differences for delayed minus immediate choices

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; HC, healthy controls; IFC, inferior frontal cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; L, left; MTL, middle temporal lobe; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; R, right; STL, superior temporal lobe; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; vmOFC, ventromedial orbitofrontal cortex; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

Post-hoc analyses based on extracted SSQs showed that abnormalities in vmOFC/MPFC/IFC were shared between OCD and ASD patients, who had increased activation to immediate-delayed choices relative to controls (both p < 0.001), who had more activation to delayed choices. In cerebellum/occipital lobe/PCC/precuneus, ASD and OCD patients had reduced activation to delayed-immediate choices compared with controls (both p < 0.001). In rACC/vmPFC/caudate, both patient groups had decreased activation to immediate-delayed choices relative to controls (ASD: p < 0.001; OCD: p < 0.05), who had enhanced activation to immediate-delayed choices, but this effect was more pronounced in ASD v. OCD patients at trend-level (p < 0.1). Findings in right MTL/STL/insula/postcentral gyrus/IPL (all p < 0.005) and left STL/MTL/IPL were due to shared abnormalities in ASD (p < 0.001) and OCD (p < 0.005) patients, who had less activation to immediate-delayed choices relative to controls who activated this region for immediate v. delayed choices (Fig. 2b ). When the four OCD patients prescribed medication were excluded from analyses, main findings remained, suggesting medication did not influence task-related activation.

Correlations between differentially activated brain regions and performance

Correlations between areas that differed between groups and AUC showed that greater activation to delayed-immediate choices in cerebellum/occipital lobe/PCC/precuneus was correlated with less-steep TD in the ASD (r = 0.66, p < 0.001) and OCD groups (r = 0.45, p < 0.05). Greater activation to immediate-delayed choices in left STL/IPL correlated with less-steep TD performance in the ASD group (r = −0.41, p < 0.05). In right MTL/STL/insula/postcentral gyrus/IPL, it correlated with better TD performance in both ASD (r = −0.39, p < 0.05) and OCD (r = −0.59, p < 0.005).

Correlations between differentially activated brain regions and symptoms

In ASD boys, greater activation to delayed v. immediate choices in right vmOFC/MPFC/lateral OFC/IFC correlated at trend-level with lower symptom severity on the repetitive behaviour subscale of the ADI-R (r = −0.34, p = 0.07). In bilateral STL/insula, lower repetitive behaviour symptom severity was related to increased activation to immediate-delayed choices in the ASD group (left:r = 0.47, p < 0.01; right:r = 0.42, p < 0.05). In the OCD group, increased activation to delayed v. immediate choices in cerebellum/occipital lobe/PCC/precuneus correlated with lower symptom severity on the CY-BOCS compulsions subscale (r = −0.58, p < 0.01). There were no correlations between activation and other subscales from the CY-BOCS in OCD or ADOS/ADI-R in ASD.

Discussion

This comparison between ASD and OCD adolescents on a ‘hot’ EF measure of decision-making showed disorder-specific impaired TD in ASD relative to OCD boys and controls. Despite this, patients had predominantly shared neurofunctional deficits in key TD areas including vmOFC/MPFC/IFC, bilateral temporo-parietal and cerebellar regions, suggesting that the neural basis of TD is a trans-diagnostic feature of both disorders. In ACC/vmPFC extending into caudate, ASD boys had trend-level more severe underactivation relative to OCD and controls for immediate v. delayed choices.

Disorder-specific performance impairment in ASD relative to OCD boys extends previous findings of impairments in ASD during TD (Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ), although there have been negative findings (Demurie et al. Reference Demurie, Roeyers, Baeyens and Sonuga-Barke2012). The absence of performance differences between OCD boys and controls is in line with previous studies (Vloet et al. Reference Vloet, Marx, Kahraman-Lanzerath, Zepf, Herpertz-Dahlmann and Konrad2010; Pinto et al. Reference Pinto, Steinglass, Greene, Weber and Simpson2014) [but see (Sohn et al. Reference Sohn, Kang, Namkoong and Kim2014)]. Moreover, ASD boys had elevated scores on the hyperactive-impulsive/inattention subscale of the SDQ compared with OCD boys and controls. The disorder-specific performance impairment in the ASD group may relate to these elevated impulsivity symptoms observed in ASD but not OCD, given that ADHD patients are consistently impaired in TD (Jackson & MacKillop, Reference Jackson and Mackillop2016). This finding exclusive to ASD lends support to the distinction between impulsive and compulsive behaviours (Robbins et al. Reference Robbins, Gillan, Smith, De Wit and Ersche2012), suggesting that while both disorders exhibit deficits in top-down cognitive control and related circuitry (Dalley et al. Reference Dalley, Everitt and Robbins2011), ASD individuals exhibit more impulsive decision-making during TD, as evidenced by disorder-specific impairments and possibly supported by trend-level disorder-specific abnormalities in ACC/vmPFC/caudate, while OCD patients are more habitually compulsive, supported by intact choice behaviour and no disorder-specific abnormalities.

Both patient groups had reduced activation relative to controls to delayed-immediate choices in ventromedial and ventrolateral OFC/IFC. Ventromedial and ventrolateral fronto-limbic regions are key temporal foresight areas (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Peters & Büchel, Reference Peters and Büchel2011) thought to support calculation of discounted reward value. Moreover, right IFC is a key region for working memory, attention to time and integration of external information with internal value representations, supporting goal-directed EF and mediation of temporal foresight (Wittmann et al. Reference Wittmann, Leland and Paulus2007; Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009; Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Chantiluke, Norman, Christakou, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons and Rubia2016a ) and has previously been shown to be abnormal during reward-related decision-making in both OCD (Bari & Robbins, Reference Bari and Robbins2013; Stern & Taylor, Reference Stern and Taylor2014) and ASD (Dichter et al. Reference Dichter, Richey, Rittenberg, Sabatino and Bodfish2012; Kohls et al. Reference Kohls, Schulte-Rüther, Nehrkorn, Müller, Fink, Kamp-Becker, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Schultz and Konrad2013).

Both patient groups showed reduced activation in PCC/precuneus/occipital lobe/cerebellum to delayed-immediate choices compared with controls. These areas are important parts of fronto-limbic-parieto-cerebellar networks involved in motivation, reward evaluation and reward response (Vogt et al. Reference Vogt, Finch and Olson1992; McCoy et al. Reference McCoy, Crowley, Haghighian, Dean and Platt2003). The cerebellum is typically activated during delayed choices in healthy populations and has been associated with future outcome expectancy and temporal bridging (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Taylor, Lidzba and Rubia2003; Wittmann et al. Reference Wittmann, Leland and Paulus2007, Reference Wittmann, Simmons, Aron and Paulus2010; Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009; Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Peters & Büchel, Reference Peters and Büchel2011; Noreika et al. Reference Noreika, Falter and Rubia2013). We previously found similar effects in ADHD patients relative to controls during the same task, suggesting that cerebellar underactivation maybe a trans-diagnostic feature of disorders that are challenged in TD (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009). Moreover, given the aforementioned role of fronto-limbic-parieto-temporo-cerebellar networks in motivation and reward evaluation, shared abnormalities in this network could possibly relate to neurofunctional similarities in the motivational and reward salience of e.g. performing repetitive behaviours in each disorder, in line with theories of shared impairments in motivation control underpinning these behaviours in each disorder (Hollander et al. Reference Hollander, Kim, Khanna and Pallanti2007). This collectively provides first evidence for shared functional abnormalities in ventromedial and ventrolateral fronto-parieto-striato-cerebellar regions between ASD and OCD.

Conversely, relative to controls, both patient groups had reduced activation to immediate choices in the rACC/vmPFC reaching into caudate. However, these abnormalities were at trend-level more pronounced in ASD relative to OCD, possibly linking to ASD-specific performance impairments. rACC mediates decision conflict (Pochon et al. Reference Pochon, Riis, Sanfey, Nystrom and Cohen2008) and typically is increased in activation with decision difficulty during intertemporal choice (Pine et al. Reference Pine, Seymour, Roiser, Bossaerts, Friston, Curran and Dolan2009). Our recent meta-analysis of structural and functional MRI studies also found shared reductions in this region in ASD and OCD relative to controls both in volume and in activation during cognitive control (Carlisi et al., Reference Carlisi, Norman, Lukito, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2016b ). In this study, however, we find that this dysfunction was trend-wise more impaired in ASD, implying a gradual rather than dichotomic effect of more severe impairment in ASD.

Findings of shared reduced vmPFC, left caudate, posterior insula and STL/IPL activation during immediate v. delayed choices in patients relative to controls are in line with a wealth of evidence implicating these regions in temporal foresight and reward-based decision-making as well as possible abnormal maturation of networks mediating these processes in ASD and OCD. We showed previously that vmPFC activation to immediate choices during TD increases with age and AUC, indicating an increase in delay-tolerant behaviour linked to increased limbic-corticostriatal activation with age (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Gershman, Niv, Simmons, Brammer and Rubia2013a ). In children and adults, steeper TD has been associated with an imbalance between reduced activation in ventromedial prefrontal and lateral frontal systems mediating evaluation of future reward and temporal foresight, and reduced top-down control over ventral-striatal and limbic systems, which respond to immediate reward (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011; Peters & Büchel, Reference Peters and Büchel2011; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ). Moreover, tasks indexing vmPFC functioning have shown age-dependent increases in sensitivity to future consequences (Crone & van der Molen, Reference Crone and Van Der Molen2004) and behavioural control during TD (Steinbeis et al. Reference Steinbeis, Haushofer, Fehr and Singer2016).

The caudate is involved in time discrimination (Smith et al. Reference Smith, Taylor, Lidzba and Rubia2003), has been linked to reward expectation and evaluation (Hinvest et al. Reference Hinvest, Elliott, Mckie and Anderson2011) and is activated during immediate choices in healthy individuals (Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011). In OCD, OFC-caudate loops are proposed to drive impulsivity as well as compulsive behaviour (Fineberg et al. Reference Fineberg, Potenza, Chamberlain, Berlin, Menzies, Bechara, Sahakian, Robbins, Bullmore and Hollander2009; Dalley et al. Reference Dalley, Everitt and Robbins2011). Thus, results could suggest that adolescents with ASD and OCD both have problems with context-dependent decision-making but that this is more problematic for people with ASD, potentially relating to the findings of disorder-specific behavioural deficits in the ASD group. Moreover, the posterior insula is associated with decision-making in the context of prior risk (Xue et al. Reference Xue, Lu, Levin and Bechara2010) and is important for the integration of temporal-affective information (Elliott et al. Reference Elliott, Friston and Dolan2000) and temporal encoding (Wittmann et al. Reference Wittmann, Simmons, Aron and Paulus2010). While previous studies have found specifically anterior insula activation during TD in children (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Halari, Christakou and Taylor2009) and adults (Tanaka et al. Reference Tanaka, Doya, Okada, Ueda, Okamoto and Yamawaki2004; Bickel et al. Reference Bickel, Pitcock, Yi and Angtuaco2009; Hinvest et al. Reference Hinvest, Elliott, Mckie and Anderson2011), the present results highlight a differential abnormality in the posterior insula during reward presentation and internal state evaluation (Elliott et al. Reference Elliott, Friston and Dolan2000) shared between ASD and OCD.

Findings of reduced activation to immediate-delayed choices in STL/IPL in ASD relative to controls are in line with evidence of weaker brain-behaviour correlations in this region in ASD relative to controls during TD (Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Christakou, Murphy, Giampietro, Daly, Brammer, Murphy and Rubia2014b ) and extend these findings to OCD. These regions are important for temporal coding and reward selection (Cardinal, Reference Cardinal2006; Christakou et al. Reference Christakou, Brammer and Rubia2011), suggesting deficits with planning, consistent with behavioural deficits in this domain in ASD (Hill, Reference Hill2004) and OCD (Shin et al. Reference Shin, Lee, Kim and Kwon2014). IPL is specifically sensitive to delay (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Overmeyer, Taylor, Brammer, Williams, Simmons, Andrew and Bullmore1998) and attention-allocation to time (Ortuno et al. Reference Ortuno, Ojeda, Arbizu, Lopez, Marti-Climent, Penuelas and Cervera2002; Coull, Reference Coull2004; Rubia, Reference Rubia and Glicksohn2006), as well as duration encoding (Wittmann, Reference Wittmann2009) and quantity representation, which may contribute to inter-temporal choices regarding the IPL's role in comparing time and value (Sandrini et al. Reference Sandrini, Rossini and Miniussi2004). Correlations between enhanced activation to immediate choices in the patient groups and better TD performance suggest that in both groups, this upregulation is related to a shift in performance towards that of controls, providing possible mechanistic implications of this region in the context of TD behaviour. Moreover, increased activation bilaterally in this region in the ASD group correlated with lower levels of repetitive behaviours, linking performance improvement and symptom reduction to brain activation in these individuals, further highlighting the mechanistic implications of this region in the context of repetitive behaviours and decision-making.

Clinically, the fact that these disorders exhibit shared neural abnormalities during TD has implications for identification of common mechanisms, which may drive overlapping behaviours in each disorder. While symptoms such as compulsions in OCD can sometimes appear similar to repetitive behaviours in ASD at an observational level, less is known about the mechanistic underpinnings of these behaviours and related cognitive functions and whether they are shared or disorder-specific. Thus, this evidence sheds light on trans-diagnostic phenotypes that could aid in future treatment targets and work toward providing a biological explanation of commonalities and differences in clinical behaviour. This has similarly been shown in the case of inhibitory control and brain structure/function differences/similarities in a recent meta-analysis comparing ASD and OCD (Carlisi et al. Reference Carlisi, Norman, Lukito, Radua, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2016b ), and this study extends this understanding to temporal foresight and decision-making.

This study's strengths include the thoroughness with which ASD individuals were assessed for the presence of ASD-related symptomatology and the exclusion of patients with psychiatric comorbidities. However, sub-threshold symptoms may have been present in the patient samples. The group of ASD patients tested in this study had a relatively high IQ, comparable with that of controls. While matching groups for IQ is important for fMRI studies to disentangle the effects of ASD from the effects of low IQ, this also means that the findings are not generalizable to other more typical ASD patients with low IQ (Charman et al. Reference Charman, Pickles, Simonoff, Chandler, Loucas and Baird2011; Crespi, Reference Crespi2016). The fact that most patients had high-functioning Asperger's syndrome further limits generalizability. Thus, it is possible that OCD-related symptoms were present in the ASD sample and could account for some of the neurobiological overlap in results. In addition, sub-clinical levels of ASD-related symptoms may have been present in the OCD sample, as reflected by shared impairments compared with controls on the emotional-distress SDQ subscale. It would also be interesting to examine the possible effects of puberty on any observed abnormalities. However, it has been shown that impulsive behaviour is independent of puberty in males (Steinberg et al. Reference Steinberg, Albert, Cauffman, Banich, Graham and Woolard2008). Additionally, four OCD patients were prescribed antidepressant medication. While there is evidence for effects of serotonin on brain function (Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Longhitano, Ayres, Cowen, Harmer and Rogers2008; Murphy, Reference Murphy2010), results remained when analyses were repeated excluding these patients. Lastly, It is a common finding that brain activation is more sensitive than performance to detect differences between groups in these patient groups (Fitzgerald et al. Reference Fitzgerald, Stern, Angstadt, Nicholson-Muth, Maynor, Welsh, Hanna and Taylor2010; Duerden et al. Reference Duerden, Taylor, Soorya, Wang, Fan and Anagnostou2013; Ambrosino et al. Reference Ambrosino, Bos, Van Raalten, Kobussen, Van Belle, Oranje and Durston2014; Marsh et al. Reference Marsh, Horga, Parashar, Wang, Peterson and Simpson2014; Chantiluke et al. Reference Chantiluke, Barrett, Giampietro, Santosh, Brammer, Simmons, Murphy and Rubia2015b ; Morein-Zamir et al. Reference Morein-Zamir, Voon, Dodds, Sule, Van Niekerk, Sahakian and Robbins2015). While the subject numbers have been shown to be sufficient for fMRI analyses (Thirion et al. Reference Thirion, Pinel, Mériaux, Roche, Dehaene and Poline2007), the performance and correlation analyses, however, were underpowered.

Conclusions

This is the first study to compare brain function between these disorders and provides novel evidence to suggest that ASD and OCD share trans-diagnostic abnormalities during TD in ventromedial and ventrolateral fronto-striatal and fronto-temporo-parieto-cerebellar regions important for temporal foresight and reward-related decision-making. This may drive shared problems with reward-related behaviours and delaying repetitive actions.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717001088.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by grants from the Medical Research Council (MRC G0300155) to K. R. and the MRC UK Autism Imaging Multicentre Study (G0400061) to D. M. This paper represents independent research part-funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. A.C. was supported by a post-doctoral fellowship from MRC G0300155. K.C. and L.N. were supported by Ph.D. studentships from the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London. C.C. was supported by a NIHR-BRC Ph.D. studentship.

Declaration Interest

K. R. has received funding from Lilly for another project and speaker's honoraria from Lilly, Shire, Novartis and Medice. D. M. has received funding for another project from Lilly. M. B. has served as a consultant for P1Vital. The other authors have no conflict of interests to declare.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

CC and LN contributed equally to this work.

The MRC AIMS Consortium is a collaboration of Autism research centres in the UK including the Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience. London, the Autism Research Centre, University of Cambridge, and the Autism Research Group, University of Oxford. It is funded by the MRC UK and headed by the Department of Forensic and Developmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience. The Consortium members are in alphabetical order: Bailey A.J., Baron-Cohen S., Bolton P.F., Bullmore E.T., Carrington S., Chakrabarti B., Daly E.M., Deoni S.C., Ecker C,. Happe F., Henty J., Jezzard P., Johnston P., Jones D.K., Lombardo M., Madden A., Mullins D., Murphy C.M., Murphy D.G., Pasco G., Sadek S., Spain D., Steward R., Suckling J., Wheelwright S., Williams S.C.

References

Abramovitch, A, Abramowitz, JS, Mittelman, A (2013). The neuropsychology of adult obsessive–compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 33, 11631171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambrosino, S, Bos, D, Van Raalten, T, Kobussen, N, Van Belle, J, Oranje, B, Durston, S (2014). Functional connectivity during cognitive control in children with autism spectrum disorder: an independent component analysis. Journal of Neural Transmission 121, 11451155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
Antrop, I, Stock, P, Verté, S, Wiersema, JR, Baeyens, D, Roeyers, H (2006). ADHD and delay aversion: the influence of non-temporal stimulation on choice for delayed rewards. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 11521158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bari, A, Robbins, TW (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology 108, 4479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benatti, B, Dell'osso, B, Arici, C, Hollander, E, Altamura, AC (2014). Characterizing impulsivity profile in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice 18, 156160.Google Scholar
Bickel, WK, Pitcock, JA, Yi, R, Angtuaco, EJC (2009). Congruence of BOLD response across intertemporal choice conditions: fictive and real money gains and losses. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 88398846.Google Scholar
Blumberg, SJ, Bramlett, MD, Kogan, MD, Schieve, LA, Jones, JR, Lu, MC (2013). Changes in prevalence of parent-reported autism spectrum disorder in school-aged US children: 2007 to 2011–2012. National Health Statistics Reports 65, 17.Google Scholar
Brammer, MJ, Bullmore, ET, Simmons, A, Williams, SCR, Grasby, PM, Howard, RJ, Woodruff, PWR, Rabe-Hesketh, S (1997). Generic brain activation mapping in functional magnetic resonance imaging: a nonparametric approach. Magnetic Resonance Imaging 15, 763770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brem, S, Hauser, TU, Iannaccone, R, Brandeis, D, Drechsler, R, Walitza, S (2012). Neuroimaging of cognitive brain function in paediatric obsessive compulsive disorder: a review of literature and preliminary meta-analysis. Journal of Neural Transmission 119, 14251448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullmore, E, Brammer, M, Rabe-Hesketh, S, Curtis, V, Morris, R, Williams, S, Sharma, T, Mcguire, P (1999 a). Methods for diagnosis and treatment of stimulus-correlated motion in generic brain activation studies using fMRI. Human Brain Mapping 7, 3848.Google Scholar
Bullmore, E, Long, C, Suckling, J, Fadili, J, Calvert, G, Zelaya, F, Carpenter, TA, Brammer, M (2001). Colored noise and computational inference in neurophysiological (fMRI) time series analysis: resampling methods in time and wavelet domains. Human Brain Mapping 12, 6178.Google Scholar
Bullmore, ET, Suckling, J, Overmeyer, S, Rabe-Hesketh, S, Taylor, E, Brammer, MJ (1999 b). Global, voxel, and cluster tests, by theory and permutation, for a difference between two groups of structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging 18, 3242.Google Scholar
Cardinal, RN (2006). Neural systems implicated in delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Neural Networks 19, 12771301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlisi, CO, Chantiluke, K, Norman, L, Christakou, A, Barrett, N, Giampietro, V, Brammer, M, Simmons, A, Rubia, K (2016 a). The effects of acute fluoxetine administration on temporal discounting in youth with ADHD. Psychological Medicine 46, 11971209.Google Scholar
Carlisi, CO, Norman, LJ, Lukito, SS, Radua, J, Mataix-Cols, D, Rubia, K (2016 b). Comparative multimodal meta-analysis of structural and functional brain abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.10.006.Google ScholarPubMed
Chantiluke, K, Barrett, N, Giampietro, V, Brammer, M, Simmons, A, Murphy, DG, Rubia, K (2015 a). Inverse effect of fluoxetine on medial prefrontal cortex activation during reward reversal in ADHD and autism. Cerebral Cortex 25, 17571770.Google Scholar
Chantiluke, K, Barrett, N, Giampietro, V, Brammer, M, Simmons, A, Rubia, K (2014 a). Disorder-dissociated effects of fluoxetine on brain function of working memory in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Medicine 45, 11951205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chantiluke, K, Barrett, N, Giampietro, V, Santosh, P, Brammer, M, Simmons, A, Murphy, D, Rubia, K (2015 b). Inverse fluoxetine effects on inhibitory brain activation in non-comorbid boys with ADHD and with ASD. Psychopharmacology 232, 20712082.Google Scholar
Chantiluke, K, Christakou, A, Murphy, CM, Giampietro, V, Daly, EM, Brammer, M, Murphy, DG, Rubia, K (2014 b). Disorder-specific functional abnormalities during temporal discounting in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Autism and comorbid ADHD and Autism. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 223, 113120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T, Pickles, A, Simonoff, E, Chandler, S, Loucas, T, Baird, G (2011). IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders: data from the special needs and autism project (SNAP). Psychological Medicine 41, 619627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, S, Chien, Y, Wu, C, Shang, C, Wu, Y, Gau, S (2016). Deficits in executive functions among youths with autism spectrum disorders: an age-stratified analysis. Psychological Medicine 46, 16251638.Google Scholar
Christakou, A, Brammer, M, Rubia, K (2011). Maturation of limbic corticostriatal activation and connectivity associated with developmental changes in temporal discounting. NeuroImage 54, 13441354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christakou, A, Gershman, SJ, Niv, Y, Simmons, A, Brammer, M, Rubia, K (2013 a). Neural and psychological maturation of decision-making in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25, 18071823.Google Scholar
Christakou, A, Murphy, C, Chantiluke, K, Cubillo, A, Smith, A, Giampietro, V, Daly, E, Ecker, C, Robertson, D, Murphy, D (2013 b). Disorder-specific functional abnormalities during sustained attention in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and with Autism. Molecular Psychiatry 18, 236244.Google Scholar
Coull, JT (2004). fMRI studies of temporal attention: allocating attention within, or towards, time. Cognitive Brain Research 21, 216226.Google Scholar
Crespi, BJ (2016). Autism as a disorder of high intelligence. Frontiers in Neuroscience 10, 300. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00300 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, EA, Van Der Molen, MW (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision making: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Neuropsychology 25, 251279.Google Scholar
Cubillo, A, Smith, AB, Barrett, N, Giampietro, V, Brammer, MJ, Simmons, A, Rubia, K (2014). Shared and drug-specific effects of atomoxetine and methylphenidate on inhibitory brain dysfunction in medication-naive ADHD boys. Cerebral Cortex 24, 174185.Google Scholar
Dalley, JW, Everitt, BJ, Robbins, TW (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69, 680694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demurie, E, Roeyers, H, Baeyens, D, Sonuga-Barke, E (2012). Temporal discounting of monetary rewards in children and adolescents with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science 15, 791800.Google Scholar
Demurie, E, Roeyers, H, Baeyens, D, Sonuga-Barke, E (2013). Domain-general and domain-specific aspects of temporal discounting in children with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders (ASD): a proof of concept study. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 18701880.Google Scholar
Dichter, GS, Richey, JA, Rittenberg, AM, Sabatino, A, Bodfish, JW (2012). Reward circuitry function in autism during face anticipation and outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42, 147160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Martino, A, Ross, K, Uddin, LQ, Sklar, AB, Castellanos, FX, Milham, MP (2009). Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry 65, 6374.Google Scholar
Doshi-Velez, F, Ge, Y, Kohane, I (2014). Comorbidity clusters in autism spectrum disorders: an electronic health record time-series analysis. Pediatrics 133, e54e63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duerden, EG, Taylor, MJ, Soorya, LV, Wang, T, Fan, J, Anagnostou, E (2013). Neural correlates of inhibition of socially relevant stimuli in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research 1533, 8090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, R, Friston, KJ, Dolan, RJ (2000). Dissociable neural responses in human reward systems. Journal of Neuroscience 20, 61596165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figee, M, Pattij, T, Willuhn, I, Luigjes, J, Van Den Brink, W, Goudriaan, A, Potenza, MN, Robbins, TW, Denys, D (2016). Compulsivity in obsessive–compulsive disorder and addictions. European Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 856868.Google Scholar
Figee, M, Vink, M, De Geus, F, Vulink, N, Veltman, DJ, Westenberg, H, Denys, D (2011). Dysfunctional reward circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 69, 867874.Google Scholar
Fineberg, NA, Potenza, MN, Chamberlain, SR, Berlin, HA, Menzies, L, Bechara, A, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW, Bullmore, ET, Hollander, E (2009). Probing compulsive and impulsive behaviors, from animal models to endophenotypes: a narrative review. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 591604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitzgerald, KD, Stern, ER, Angstadt, M, Nicholson-Muth, KC, Maynor, MR, Welsh, RC, Hanna, GL, Taylor, SF (2010). Altered function and connectivity of the medial frontal cortex in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 68, 10391047.Google Scholar
Geurts, HM, Verté, S, Oosterlaan, J, Roeyers, H, Sergeant, JA (2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 836854.Google Scholar
Gillan, CM, Robbins, TW (2014). Goal-directed learning and obsessive–compulsive disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 369, 20130475. http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R, Scott, S (1999). Comparing the strengths and difficulties questionnaire and the child behavior checklist: is small beautiful? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 27, 1724.Google Scholar
Goodman, WK, Price, LH, Rasmussen, SA, Mazure, C, Delgado, P, Heninger, GR, Charney, DS (1989). The yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale: II. validity. Archives of General Psychiatry 46, 10121016.Google Scholar
Grassi, G, Pallanti, S, Righi, L, Figee, M, Mantione, M, Denys, D, Piccagliani, D, Rossi, A, Stratta, P (2015). Think twice: impulsivity and decision making in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions 4, 263272.Google Scholar
Hill, EL (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 2632.Google Scholar
Hinvest, NS, Elliott, R, Mckie, S, Anderson, IM (2011). Neural correlates of choice behavior related to impulsivity and venturesomeness. Neuropsychologia 49, 23112320.Google Scholar
Hollander, E, Kim, S, Khanna, S, Pallanti, S (2007). Obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders: diagnostic and dimensional issues. CNS Spectrums 12(S3), 513.Google Scholar
Jackson, JNS, Mackillop, J (2016). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and monetary delay discounting: a meta-analysis of case-control studies. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 1, 316325.Google Scholar
Just, MA, Cherkassky, VL, Keller, TA, Kana, RK, Minshew, NJ (2007). Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an fmri study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex 17, 951961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohls, G, Schulte-Rüther, M, Nehrkorn, B, Müller, K, Fink, GR, Kamp-Becker, I, Herpertz-Dahlmann, B, Schultz, RT, Konrad, K (2013). Reward system dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8, 565572.Google Scholar
Kriete, T, Noelle, DC (2015). Dopamine and the development of executive dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE 10, e0121605.Google Scholar
Langen, M, Durston, S, Kas, MJ, Van Engeland, H, Staal, WG (2011). The neurobiology of repetitive behavior:… and men. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35, 356365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C, Risi, S, Lambrecht, L, Cook, E Jr., Leventhal, B, Dilavore, P, Pickles, A, Rutter, M (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule – generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30, 205223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C, Rutter, M, Couteur, A (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24, 659685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, R, Horga, G, Parashar, N, Wang, Z, Peterson, BS, Simpson, HB (2014). Altered activation in fronto-striatal circuits during sequential processing of conflict in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 75, 615622.Google Scholar
McCoy, AN, Crowley, JC, Haghighian, G, Dean, HL, Platt, ML (2003). Saccade reward signals in posterior cingulate cortex. Neuron 40, 10311040.Google Scholar
Menon, V, Uddin, LQ (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function 214, 655667.Google Scholar
Menzies, L, Chamberlain, SR, Laird, AR, Thelen, SM, Sahakian, BJ, Bullmore, ET (2008). Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 525549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesulam, M-M (1999). Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 354, 13251346.Google Scholar
Morein-Zamir, S, Voon, V, Dodds, CM, Sule, A, Van Niekerk, J, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2015). Divergent subcortical activity for distinct executive functions: stopping and shifting in obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychological Medicine FirstView 112.Google Scholar
Murphy, CM, Christakou, A, Daly, EM, Ecker, C, Giampietro, V, Brammer, M, Smith, AB, Johnston, P, Robertson, DM, Consortium, MA, Murphy, DG, Rubia, K (2014). Abnormal functional activation and maturation of fronto-striato-temporal and cerebellar regions during sustained attention in autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 11071116.Google Scholar
Murphy, SE (2010). Using functional neuroimaging to investigate the mechanisms of action of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). Current Pharmaceutical Design 16, 19901997.Google Scholar
Murphy, SE, Longhitano, C, Ayres, RE, Cowen, PJ, Harmer, CJ, Rogers, RD (2008). The role of serotonin in nonnormative risky choice: the effects of tryptophan supplements on the ‘reflection effect’ in healthy adult volunteers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21, 17091719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myerson, J, Green, L, Warusawitharana, M (2001). Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 76, 235243.Google Scholar
Noreika, V, Falter, CM, Rubia, K (2013). Timing deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): evidence from neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 51, 235266.Google Scholar
Norman, LJ, Carlisi, C, Lukito, S, Hart, H, Mataix-Cols, D, Radua, J, Rubia, K (2016). Structural and functional brain abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparative meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 815825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odum, AL (2011). Delay discounting: trait variable? Behavioural Processes 87, 19.Google Scholar
Oldfield, RC (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97113.Google Scholar
Ortuno, F, Ojeda, N, Arbizu, J, Lopez, P, Marti-Climent, J, Penuelas, I, Cervera, S (2002). Sustained attention in a counting task: normal performance and functional neuroanatomy. NeuroImage 17, 411420.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S, Jensen, J (1999). Brief report: specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29, 171177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, MP, Rogalsky, C, Simmons, A, Feinstein, JS, Stein, MB (2003). Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. NeuroImage 19, 14391448.Google Scholar
Peters, J, Büchel, C (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 66, 138148.Google Scholar
Peters, J, Büchel, C (2011). The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 227239.Google Scholar
Philip, RCM, Dauvermann, MR, Whalley, HC, Baynham, K, Lawrie, SM, Stanfield, AC (2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 901942.Google Scholar
Pine, A, Seymour, B, Roiser, JP, Bossaerts, P, Friston, KJ, Curran, HV, Dolan, RJ (2009). Encoding of Marginal Utility across Time in the Human Brain. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 95759581.Google Scholar
Pinto, A, Steinglass, JE, Greene, AL, Weber, EU, Simpson, HB (2014). Capacity to delay reward differentiates obsessive-compulsive disorder and obsessive compulsive personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry 75, 653659.Google Scholar
Pochon, J-B, Riis, J, Sanfey, AG, Nystrom, LE, Cohen, JD (2008). Functional imaging of decision conflict. Journal of Neuroscience 28, 34683473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radua, J, Pozo, NOD, Gómez, J, Guillen-Grima, F, Ortuño, F (2014). Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies indicates that an increase of cognitive difficulty during executive tasks engages brain regions associated with time perception. Neuropsychologia 58, 1422.Google Scholar
Radua, J, Van Den Heuvel, OA, Surguladze, S, Mataix-Cols, D (2010). Meta-analytical comparison of voxel-based morphometry studies in obsessive-compulsive disorder vs other anxiety disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 701711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, JB, Zhang, L, Mitchell, SH, Wit, H (1999). Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: effect of alcohol. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 71, 121143.Google Scholar
Robbins, TW, Gillan, CM, Smith, DG, De Wit, S, Ersche, KD (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 8191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K (2006). The neural correlates of timing functions. In Timing the Future: the Case for a Time-Based Prospective Memory (ed. Glicksohn, J.), pp. 213238. World Scientific Publishing: Hackensack, NJ.Google Scholar
Rubia, K, Halari, R, Christakou, A, Taylor, E (2009). Impulsiveness as a timing disturbance: neurocognitive abnormalities in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder during temporal processes and normalization with methylphenidate. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364, 19191931.Google Scholar
Rubia, K, Overmeyer, S, Taylor, E, Brammer, M, Williams, S, Simmons, A, Andrew, C, Bullmore, E (1998). Prefrontal involvement in ‘temporal bridging’ and timing movement. Neuropsychologia 36, 12831293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruscio, A, Stein, D, Chiu, W, KessleR, R (2010). The epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Molecular Psychiatry 15, 5363.Google Scholar
Rutter, M, Bailey, A, Lord, C (2003). The Social Communication Questionnaire: Manual. Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Sanders, J, Johnson, KA, Garavan, H, Gill, M, Gallagher, L (2008). A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research in autistic spectrum disorders: attention, inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 2, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandrini, M, Rossini, PM, Miniussi, C (2004). The differential involvement of inferior parietal lobule in number comparison: a rTMS study. Neuropsychologia 42, 19021909.Google Scholar
Schmitz, N, Rubia, K, Van Amelsvoort, T, Daly, E, Smith, A, Murphy, DGM (2008). Neural correlates of reward in autism. British Journal of Psychiatry 192, 1924.Google Scholar
Scott-Van Zeeland, AA, Dapretto, M, Ghahremani, DG, Poldrack, RA, Bookheimer, SY (2010). Reward processing in autism. Autism Research 3, 5367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, NY, Lee, TY, Kim, E, Kwon, JS (2014). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 44, 11211130.Google Scholar
Simonoff, E, Pickles, A, Charman, T, Chandler, S, Loucas, T, Baird, G (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 47, 921929.Google Scholar
Singer, T, Critchley, HD, Preuschoff, K (2009). A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, 334340.Google Scholar
Smith, A, Taylor, E, Lidzba, K, Rubia, K (2003). A right hemispheric frontocerebellar network for time discrimination of several hundreds of milliseconds. NeuroImage 20, 344350.Google Scholar
Sohn, SY, Kang, JI, Namkoong, K, Kim, SJ (2014). Multidimensional measures of impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: cannot wait and stop. PLoS ONE 9, e111739.Google Scholar
Steinbeis, N, Haushofer, J, Fehr, E, Singer, T (2016). Development of behavioral control and associated vmPFC–DLPFC connectivity explains children's increased resistance to temptation in intertemporal choice. Cerebral Cortex 26, 3242.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L, Albert, D, Cauffman, E, Banich, M, Graham, S, Woolard, J (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology 44, 17641778.Google Scholar
Stern, ER, Taylor, SF (2014). Cognitive neuroscience of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 37, 337352.Google Scholar
Talairach, J, Tournoux, P (1988). Coplanar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain, a 3-dimensional Proportional System: an Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Thieme: New York.Google Scholar
Tanaka, SC, Doya, K, Okada, G, Ueda, K, Okamoto, Y, Yamawaki, S (2004). Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nature Neuroscience 7, 887893.Google Scholar
Thirion, B, Pinel, P, Mériaux, S, Roche, A, Dehaene, S, Poline, J-B (2007). Analysis of a large fMRI cohort: statistical and methodological issues for group analyses. NeuroImage 35, 105120.Google Scholar
Van Den Heuvel, OA, Mataix-Cols, D, Zwitser, G, Cath, DC, Van Der Werf, YD, Groenewegen, HJ, Van Balkom, AJLM, Veltman, DJ (2011). Common limbic and frontal-striatal disturbances in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder, panic disorder and hypochondriasis. Psychological Medicine 41, 23992410.Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, OA, Veltman, DJ, Groenewegen, HJ, Cath, DC, van Balkom, AJ, van Hartskamp, J, Barkhof, F, van Dyck, R (2005). Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 301309.Google Scholar
Via, E, Radua, J, Cardoner, N, Happé, F, Mataix-Cols, D (2011). Meta-analysis of gray matter abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder: should asperger disorder be subsumed under a broader umbrella of autistic spectrum disorder? Archives of General Psychiatry 68, 409418.Google Scholar
Vloet, T, Marx, I, Kahraman-Lanzerath, B, Zepf, F, Herpertz-Dahlmann, B, Konrad, K (2010). Neurocognitive performance in children with ADHD and OCD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 38, 961969.Google Scholar
Vogt, BA, Finch, DM, Olson, CR (1992). Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cerebral Cortex 2, 435443.Google Scholar
Voon, V, Derbyshire, K, Ruck, C, Irvine, MA, Worbe, Y, Enander, J, Schreiber, LRN, Gillan, C, Fineberg, NA, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW, Harrison, NA, Wood, J, Daw, ND, Dayan, P, Grant, JE, Bullmore, ET (2015). Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Molecular Psychiatry 20, 345352.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Wesley, MJ, Bickel, WK (2014). Remember the future II: meta-analyses and functional overlap of working memory and delay discounting. Biological Psychiatry 75, 435448.Google Scholar
WHO (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
Wiener, M, Turkeltaub, P, Coslett, HB (2010). The image of time: a voxel-wise meta-analysis. NeuroImage 49, 17281740.Google Scholar
Wittmann, M (2009). The inner experience of time. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 364, 19551967.Google Scholar
Wittmann, M, Leland, DS, Paulus, MP (2007). Time and decision making: differential contribution of the posterior insular cortex and the striatum during a delay discounting task. Experimental Brain Research 179, 643653.Google Scholar
Wittmann, M, Simmons, AN, Aron, JL, Paulus, MP (2010). Accumulation of neural activity in the posterior insula encodes the passage of time. Neuropsychologia 48, 31103120.Google Scholar
Xue, G, Lu, Z, Levin, IP, Bechara, A (2010). The impact of prior risk experiences on subsequent risky decision-making: the role of the insula. NeuroImage 50, 709716.Google Scholar
Zelazo, PD, Müller, U (2007). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development (ed. Goswami, U.), pp. 445469. Blackwell Publishers Ltd: Malden, MA.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Schematic of the temporal discounting fMRI paradigm. Subjects are asked to indicate whether they would prefer a small, variable amount of money immediately (immediate reward), or whether they would rather wait for a larger delay (up to £100) later (delayed reward). An algorithm adjusts the amount of the immediate reward offered based on the choices of the participant, so as to determine the lowest immediate reward they would tolerate before instead choosing to wait for the larger delayed reward. Three hypothetical delays are presented in random order: 1 week, 1 month and 1 year. Each delay choice is presented 20 times. Trials start with the presentation of the choice display, which remains available for 4 s, within which the subject must choose between the immediate (always on left side) and delayed (always on right) rewards. Total trial duration is 12 s.

Figure 1

Table 1. Participant characteristics for healthy control boys and patients with OCD or ASD

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Between-group activation differences for delayed minus immediate choices. (a) Axial slices showing split-plot analysis of variance (ANOVA) effects of group on brain activation to delayed – immediate choices. Talairach Z coordinates are indicated for slice distance (in mm) from the intercommissural line. The right side of the image corresponds to the right side of the brain. (b) Extracted statistical measures of BOLD response are shown for each of the three groups for each of the brain regions that showed a significant group effect. Black asterisks indicate a significant difference between controls and patient group. Red asterisk indicates a difference between the two patient groups. (*) = significant at a trend level; * = significant at the p < 0.05 level; ** = significant at the p ⩽ 0.005 level; *** = significant at the p ⩽ 0.001 level.

Figure 3

Table 2. Between-group activation differences for delayed minus immediate choices

Supplementary material: File

Carlisi supplementary material

Carlisi supplementary material 1

Download Carlisi supplementary material(File)
File 575.8 KB