Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T15:17:34.934Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Alternating-color words facilitate reading and eye movements among second-language learners of Chinese

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Wei Zhou
Affiliation:
Capital Normal University, Beijing
Wanwen Ye
Affiliation:
Capital Normal University, Beijing
Ming Yan*
Affiliation:
University of Macau, Taipa
*
*Corresponding author. Room 3055, E21B Humanities and Social Sciences Building Avenida da Universidade, Taipa, Macau. Email: mingyan@um.edu.mo

Abstract

The present study investigated whether word-boundary information, provided by alternating colors (consistent or inconsistent with word-boundary information) in a Chinese sentence would facilitate the reading of second-language (L2) learners. Thirty-three Korean students were recruited in the eye-movement experiment. Relative to a baseline (i.e., mono-colors) condition, incorrect word segmentation produced closer fixation location toward the beginning of words, longer fixation duration, higher refixation rate, and slower reading speed. In contrast, word segmentation with alternating colors produced further fixation location toward the center of words, shorter fixation duration, lower refixation rate, and faster reading speed. These results indicate that L2 readers are capable of making use of word-boundary knowledge for saccade generation, which can result in a facilitation of reading efficiency.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bai, X., Yan, G., Liversedge, S. P., Zang, C., & Rayner, K. (2008). Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 12771287. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1277Google ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S., (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blythe, H. I., Liang, F., Zang, C., Wang, J., Yan, G., Bai, X., … & Liversedge, S. P. (2012). Inserting spaces into Chinese text helps readers to learn new words: an eye movement study. Journal of Memory & Language, 67, 241254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43, 10351045. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00084-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E. M., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112, 777813. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epelboim, J., Booth, J. R., & Steinman, R. M. (1994). Reading unspaced text: Implications for theories of reading eye movements. Vision Research, 34, 17351766. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90130-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, D. F. (1975). Reading and visual search. Memory & Cognition, 3, 188196. doi: 10.3758/BF03212897CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Häikiö, T., Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2016). The hyphen as a syllabification cue in reading bisyllabic and multisyllabic words among Finnish 1st and 2nd graders. Reading and Writing, 29, 159182. doi: 10.1007/s11145-015-9584-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2015). Remef: Remove partial effects [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://github.com/hohenstein/remefGoogle Scholar
Hoosain, R. (1992). Psychological reality of the word in Chinese. In Chen, H. C. & Tzeng, O. J. L. (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 111130). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, S. H., & Huang, K. C. (2000a). Effects of word spacing on reading Chinese text from a video display terminal. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 8192. doi: 10.2466/PMS.90.1.81-92CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, S. H., & Huang, K. C. (2000b). Interword spacing in Chinese text layout. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 355365. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.2.355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inhoff, A. W., & Liu, W. (1998). The perceptual span and oculomotor activity during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 2034. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.1.20Google ScholarPubMed
Inhoff, A. W., & Wu, C. (2005). Eye movements and the identification of spatially ambiguous words during Chinese sentence reading. Memory & Cognition, 33, 13451356. doi: 10.3758/BF03193367CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kajii, N., Nazir, T. A., & Osaka, N. (2001). Eye movement control in reading unspaced text: The case of the Japanese script. Vision Research, 41, 25032510. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00132-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasisopa, B., Reilly, R. G., Luksaneeyanawin, S., & Burnham, D. (2013). Eye movements while reading an unspaced writing system: The case of Thai. Vision Research, 86, 7180. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliegl, R., Masson, M. E., & Richter, E. M. (2010). A linear mixed model analysis of masked repetition priming. Visual Cognition, 18, 655–681. doi: 10.1080/13506280902986058CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 1235. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.12CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohsom, C., & Gobet, F. (1997). Adding spaces to Thai and English: Effects on reading. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 19, 388393.Google Scholar
Liu, I. M., Yeh, J. S., Wang, L. H., & Chang, Y. K. (1974). Effects of arranging Chinese words as units on reading efficiency. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 16, 2532.Google Scholar
Manguel, A. (1996). A history of reading. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Malt, B. C., & Seamon, J. G. (1978). Peripheral and cognitive components of eye guidance in filled-space reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 399402. doi: 10.3758/BF03204142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConkie, G. W., & Rayner, K. (1975). The span of the effective stimulus during a fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 578586. doi: 10.3758/BF03203972CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nuthmann, A., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Mislocated fixations during reading and the inverted optimal viewing position effect. Vision Research, 45, 22012217. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.02.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Regan, J. K., Lévy-Schoen, A., Pynte, J., & Brugaillère, B. É. (1984). Convenient fixation location within isolated words of different length and structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 250257. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.250.Google ScholarPubMed
Pan, J., Laubrock, J., & Yan, M. (2016). Parafoveal processing in silent and oral reading: Reading mode influences the relative weighting of phonological and semantic information in Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 1257. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000242Google ScholarPubMed
Perea, M., & Wang, X. (2017). Do alternating-color words facilitate reading aloud text in Chinese? Evidence with developing and adult readers. Memory & Cognition, 45, 11601170. doi: 10.3758/s13421-017-0717-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perea, M., Tejero, P., & Winskel, H. (2015). Can colours be used to segment words when reading? Acta Psychologica, 159, 813. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.05.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinna, B., Uccula, A., & Tanca, M. (2010). How does the color influence figure and shape formation, grouping, numerousness and reading? The role of chromatic wholeness and fragmentation. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 30, 583593. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2010.00743.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1982). Eye movement control in reading: The role of word boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 817833. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.8.6.817Google Scholar
R Core Team (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL: http://www.R-project.orgGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Fischer, M. H., & Pollatsek, A. (1998). Unspaced text interferes with both word identification and eye movement control. Vision Research, 38, 11291144. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00274-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, R. G., Radach, R., Corbic, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2005). Comparing reading in English and Thai: The role of spatial word unit segmentation in distributed processing and eye movement control. In Proceedings of the 13th European conference on eye movements (pp. 14–18).Google Scholar
Richter, E. M., Yan, M., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2010). Modeling Chinese reading with SWIFT: How does word segmentation affect targeting? In 4th China International Conference on Eye Movements, Tianjin, China.Google Scholar
Saenger, P. (1997). Space between words: The origins of silent reading. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Sainio, M., Hyönä, J., Bingushi, K., & Bertram, R. (2007). The role of interword spacing in reading Japanese: An eye movement study. Vision Research, 47, 25752584. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.05.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shen, D., Liversedge, S. P., Tian, J., Zang, C., Cui, L., Bai, X., Yan, G., & Rayner, K. (2012). Eye movements of second language learners when reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 192. doi: 10.1037/a0027485Google ScholarPubMed
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitu, F., McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P., & O’Regan, J. K. (2001). Fixation location effects on fixation durations during reading: An inverted optimal viewing position effect. Vision Research, 41, 35133533. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00166-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, A., Zhou, W., Shu, H., & Yan, M. (2014). Reading proficiency modulates parafoveal processing efficiency: Evidence from reading Chinese as a second language. Acta Psychologica, 152, 2933. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.07.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitford, V., & Titone, D. (2015). Second-language experience modulates eye movements during first-and second-language sentence reading: Evidence from a gaze-contingent moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 11181129. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000093Google ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H. (2009) Ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yan, M., & Kliegl, R. (2016). CarPrice versus CarpRice: Word boundary ambiguity influences saccade target selection during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 18321838. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000276Google ScholarPubMed
Yan, M., Kliegl, R., Richter, E. M., Nuthmann, A., & Shu, H. (2010). Flexible saccade-target selection in Chinese reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 705725. doi: 10.1080/17470210903114858CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, M., Zhou, W., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2015). Perceptual span depends on font size during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 209219. doi: 10.1037/a0038097Google ScholarPubMed
Yen, M. H., Radach, R., Tzeng, O. J. L., Hung, D. L., & Tsai, J. L. (2009). Early parafoveal processing in reading Chinese sentences. Acta Psychologica, 131, 2433. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.02.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, W., Wang, A., Shu, H., Kliegl, R., & Yan, M. (2018). Word segmentation by alternating colors facilitates eye guidance in Chinese reading. Memory & Cognition, 46, 729740. doi: 10.3758/s13421-018-0797-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed