Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T11:52:14.903Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Origins of verbal logic: spontaneous denials by two- and three-year olds*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Roy D. Pea
Affiliation:
Clark University, Heinz Werner Institute of Developmental Psychology

Abstract

Children two to three years of age can spontaneously correct false statements and affirm true ones in a modified sentence verification paradigm. Such performances imply that very young children display knowledge of the rules of correspondence between language and reality (truth conditions) which are central to propositional logic, at an age when logical development in cognition has not been previously acknowledged.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work derives from part of an unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oxford, England. I especially thank Jerome S. Bruner, John Churcher, Michael Scaife, Susan Sugarman, and Cathi Urwin for advice and encouragement during the project, Kristine MacKain and Susan Sugarman for criticisms of an early version of this paper, and the children and mothers who patiently participated in the study. The research was supported by The Rhodes Trust, Oxford, and the writing by NIMH Traineeship 15125. Address for correspondence: Clark University, Heinz Werner Institute of Developmental Psychology, Worcester, MA 01610, U.S.A.

References

REFERENCES

Akiyama, M. M. (1979). Yes–no answering systems in young children. CogPsychol 11. 485504.Google Scholar
Anglin, J. M. (1977). Word, object and conceptual development. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Austin, J. L. (1975). How to do things with words. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U. (1967). The acquisition of the system of negation in children's speech. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1970). Language development:form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time: the use of single word utterances before syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A. & Just, M. A. (1975). Sentence-comprehension: a psycholinguistic processing model of verification. PsycholRev 82. 4573.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G. & Clark, H. H. (1972). Mental operations in the comparison of sentences and pictures. In Gregg, L. (ed.), Cognition in learning & memory. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Clark, E. V. (1977). Psychology and language. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Collins, A. W. (1968). How one could tell were a bee to guide his behaviour by a rule. Mind 77. 556–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, A. (1974). Linguistic play and language acquisition. PRCLD 8. 179–87.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G. & Flusberg, H. B. T. (1975). Some facts one simply cannot deny. JChLang 2. 279–86.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1972). Cognitive development in preschool children: the comprehension of quantifiers. Brit. SSRC Report.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. (1970). Developmental aspects of performance with negatives. In D'Arcais, G. B. Flores & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds), Advances in psycholinguistics. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Donaldson, M. & Lloyd, P. (1974). Sentences and situations: children's judgements of match and mismatch. In Bresson, F. (ed.), Problèmes actuels en psycholinguistique. Paris: C.N.R.S.Google Scholar
Dummett, M. (1973). Frege: philosophy of language. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Falmagne, R. J. (1980). The development of logical competence: a psycholinguistic perspective. In Kluwe, R. H. & Spada, H. (eds), Developmental models of thinking. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Falmagne, R. J. (ed.) (1975). Reasoning: representation and process in children and adults. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Givon, T. (1975). Negation in language: pragmatics, function and ontology. Pragmatics Microfiche, 1.2, A2.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Seligman, M. E. P. & Gelman, R. (1976). Language in the two-year-old. Cognition 4. 189202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. M. (1970 a). The semantic function of negatives and passives. BJPsychol 61. 1722.Google Scholar
Greene, J. M. (1970 b). Syntactic form and semantic function. QJExpPsychol 22. 1427.Google ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1917). Negation in English and other languages. Copenhagen.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Wason, P. C. (eds). (1977). Thinking: readings in cognitive science. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. F. (1939, 1949). Speech development of a bilingual child: a linguistic record. Vol. 1: Vocabulary growth in the first two years. Vol. 3: Grammar and general problems in the first two years. Evanston, Ill.: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Lloyd, P. & Donaldson, M. (1976). On a method of eliciting true/false judgements from young children. JChLang 3. 411–16.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1956). A system of logic. New York: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monog. Soc. Res. Ch. Dev. 38 (12).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osgood, C. E. (1971). Where do sentences come from ? In Steinberg, D. D. & Jakobovits, L. A. (eds), Semantics: an interdisciplinary reader in philosophy, linguistics and psychology. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Pea, R. D. (1980). The development of negation in early child language. In Olson, D. R. (ed.), The social foundations of language and thought: essays in honor of Jerome S. Bruner. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation (trans. Gattegno, C. & Hodgson, F. M.). New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child (trans. Cook, M. C.). New York: Ballantine Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child (trans. Weaver, H.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pope, E. (1973). Question-answering systems. Papers from the Ninth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 482–92.Google Scholar
Premack, D. (1976). Language and intelligence in ape and man. American Scientist 64. 674–83.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. A. (1976). Concept formation in word learning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1974). Subject and predicate in logic and grammar. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T., Rollins, H. & Shaughnessy, E. (1971). Storage and verification stages in processing concepts. CogPsychol 2. 239–89.Google Scholar
Wason, P. C. (1965). The contexts of plausible denial. JVLVB 4. 711.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wason, P. C. (1972). In real life negatives are false. Logique et Analyses 57–8. 1738.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: an organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Winner, E. (1979). New names for old things: the emergence of metaphoric language. JChLang 6. 469–91.Google ScholarPubMed