Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T14:32:36.471Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hebrew verbal passives in Later Language Development: the interface of register and verb morphology*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 November 2016

DORIT RAVID*
Affiliation:
Tel Aviv University, Israel
LIZZY VERED
Affiliation:
Tel Aviv University, Israel
*
[*]Address for correspondence: Dorit Ravid, Tel Aviv University – School of Education, Ramat Aviv, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel. e-mail: doritr@post.tau.ac.il

Abstract

The current study examined the production of Hebrew verbal passives across adolescence as mediated by linguistic register and verb morphology. Participants aged eight to sixteen years and a group of adults were asked to change written active-voice sentences into corresponding passive-voice forms, divided by verb register (neutral and high), binyan pattern (Qal / Nif'al, Hif'il / Huf'al, and Pi'el / Pu'al), and verb tense (past and future tense). Results showed that Hebrew passive morphology is a very late acquisition, almost a decade later than in other languages, that passivizing neutral-register verbs was less challenging than high-register verbs, and that past tense verbs were easier to passivize than future tense verbs. An order of acquisition was determined among the three binyan pairs. The paper provides an account of these findings grounded in the event-telling role of Hebrew passives in discourse and the spurt of abstract, lexically specific vocabulary in Later Language Development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abraham, W. & Leisiö, E. (eds) (2006). Passivization and typology: form and function. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Abraham, W. & Leiss, E. (2006). The impersonal passive: voice suspended under aspectual conditions. In Abraham, W. & Leisiö, E. (eds), Passivization and typology: form and function, 502–17. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Alcock, K. J., Rimba, K. & Newton, C. R. J. C. (2011). Early production of the passive in two Eastern Bantu languages. First Language 32, 459–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, S. E. M. & Crago, M. B. (1996). Early passive acquisition in Inuktitut. Journal of Child Language 23, 129–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aschermann, E., Gulzow, I. & Wendt, D. (2004). Differences in the comprehension of passive voice in German- and English-speaking children. Swiss Journal of Psychology 63, 235–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldie, B. J. (1976). The acquisition of the passive voice. Journal of Child Language 3, 331–48.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1980). The case of an (S)VO language: subjectless constructions in Modern Hebrew. Language 56, 759–76.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1985). Acquisition of Hebrew. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1993). Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 20, 641–69.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (1994). Formal, lexical, and semantic factors in acquisition of Hebrew resultative participles. Berkeley Linguistic Society 20, 8292.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (2004). Between emergence and mastery: the long developmental route of language acquisition. In Berman, R. A. (ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 934). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (2005). Introduction: developing discourse stance in different text types and languages. Journal of Pragmatics 37, 105–24.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. (2008). The psycholinguistics of developing text construction. Journal of Child Language 35, 735–71.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2004). Linguistic indicators of inter-genre differentiation in later language development. Journal of Child Language 31, 339–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, R. A. & Ravid, D. (2009). Becoming a literate language user: oral and written text construction across adolescence. In Olson, D. R. & Torrance, N. (eds), Cambridge handbook of literacy, 92111. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: a crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Biber, D. (1995). Dimensions of register variation: a crosslinguistic comparison. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J. & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 296312.Google Scholar
Borer, H. & Wexler, K. (1987). The maturation of syntax. In Roeper, T. and Williams, E. (eds), Parameter setting (pp. 123172). Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Brooks, P. J. & Tomasello, M. (1999). Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. Developmental Psychology 35, 2944.Google Scholar
Budwig, N. (1990). The linguistic marking of nonprototypical agency: an exploration into children's use of passives. Linguistics 28, 1221–52.Google Scholar
Christie, F. & Derewianka, B. (2008). School discourse. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Thornton, R. & Murasugi, K. (2009). Capturing the evasive passive. Language Acquisition 16, 123–33.Google Scholar
Dattner, E. (2015). Enabling and allowing in Hebrew: a Usage-Based Construction Grammar account. In Nolan, B., Rawoens, G. & Diedrichsen, E. (eds), Causation, permission, and transfer: argument realisation in GET, TAKE, PUT, GIVE and LET verbs, 271–93. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1990). Subject, topic and the Sesotho passive. Journal of Child Language 17, 6784.Google Scholar
Demuth, K., Moloi, F. & Machobane, M. (2010). 3-Year-olds' comprehension, production, and generalization of Sesotho passives. Cognition 115, 238–51.Google Scholar
Dromi, E. & Berman, R. A. (1986). Language-general and language-specific in developing syntax. Journal of Child Language 14, 371–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ensminger, M. E. & Fothergill, K. E. (2003). A decade of measuring SES: what it tells us and where to go from here. In Bornstein, M. H. & Bradley, R. H. (eds), Handbook of parenting: socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development, Vol. 5, 1327. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1994). Dialect, register, and genre: working assumptions about conventionalization. In Biber, D. & Finegan, E. (eds), Sociolinguistic perspectives on register, 1530. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F. (1994). Choice of passive voice is affected by verb type and animacy. Journal of Memory and Language 33, 715–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foley, W. A. (2007). A typology of information packaging in the clause. In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description: Vol. II, Clause structure, 362446. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fox, D. & Grodzinsky, Y. (1998). Children's passive: a view from the by-phrase. Linguistic Inquiry 29, 311–32.Google Scholar
Gámez, P. B., Shimpi, P. M., Waterfall, H. R. & Huttenlocher, J. (2009). Priming a perspective in Spanish monolingual children: the use of syntactic alternatives. Journal of Child Language 36, 269–90.Google Scholar
Gil, D. (2006). The acquisition of voice morphology in Jakarta Indonesian. In Gagarina, N. & Gülzow, I. (eds), The acquisition of verbs and their grammar: the effect of particular languages, 201–27. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (1981). Typology and functional domains. Studies in Language 5, 163–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. & Chafetz, J. (1990). Verb-based versus class-based accounts of actionality effects in children's comprehension of passives. Cognition 36, 227–54.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1989). Spoken and written language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. (1990). The grammaticalization of passive morphology. Studies in Language 14, 2572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horgan, D. (1978). The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Language 5, 6580.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. L. & Dryer, M. S. (2007). Passive in the world's languages. In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description: Vol. II, Clause structure, 325–61. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, K.-O. & Lee, Y. (2008). An event-structural account of passive acquisition in Korean. Language and Speech 51, 133–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maratsos, M., Fox, D. E., Becker, J. A. & Chalkley, M. A. (1985). Semantic restrictions on children's passives. Cognition 19, 167–91.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Bates, E., Burkardt, A. & Good, A. B. (1991). Functional constraints of the acquisition of the passive: toward a model of the competence to perform. First Language 11, 6592.Google Scholar
Messenger, K. (2010). Syntactic priming and children's production and representation of the passive. Language Acquisition 17, 121–3.Google Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P. & Mclean, J. F. (2012). Is children's acquisition of the passive a staged process? Evidence from six- and nine-year-olds’ production of passives. Journal of Child Language 39, 9911016.Google Scholar
Mitkovska, L. & Bužarovska, E. (2012). An alternative analysis of the English get-past participle constructions: Is get all that passive? Journal of English Linguistics 40, 196215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monachesi, P. (2005). The verbal complex in Romance: a case study in grammatical interfaces. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myhill, J. (1997). Towards a functional typology of agent defocusing. Linguistics 35, 799844.Google Scholar
Nippold, M. A. (2007). Later language development: school-age children, adolescents, and young adults, 3rd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Pe'er, M. (2013). Resultative passives in the Hebrew lexicon and in Hebrew-speaking children's peer talk. Unpublished MA thesis, Tel Aviv University [in Hebrew].Google Scholar
Pierce, A. (1992). The acquisition of passives in Spanish and the question of A-chain maturation. Language Acquisition 2, 5581.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., Lebeaux, D. S. & Frost, L. R. (1987). Productivity and constraints in the acquisition of the Passive. Cognition 26, 195267.Google Scholar
Prat-Sala, M., Shillcock, R. & Sorace, A. (2000). Animacy effects on the production of object dislocated descriptions by Catalan-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 27, 97117.Google Scholar
Pye, C. & Quixtan Poz, P. (1988). Precocious passives (and antipassives) in Quiche Mayan. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 27, 7180.Google Scholar
Rathert, M. (2006). Simple preterit and composite perfect tense: the role of the adjectival passive. In Abraham, W. & Leisiö, L. (eds), Passivization and typology: form and function, 518–42. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. (1995). Language change in child and adult Hebrew: a psycholinguistic perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. (2004). Later lexical development in Hebrew: derivational morphology revisited. In Berman, R. A. (ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence: psycholinguistic and crosslinguistic perspectives, 5382. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. (2005). Emergence of linguistic complexity in written expository texts: evidence from later language acquisition. In Ravid, D. & Shyldkrot, H. Bat-Zeev (eds), Perspectives on language and language development, 337–55. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, D. (2006). Semantic development in textual contexts during the school years: Noun Scale analyses. Journal of Child Language 33, 791821.Google Scholar
Ravid, D., Ashkenazi, O., Levie, R., Ben Zadok, G., Grunwald, T., Bratslavsky, R. & Gillis, S. (2016). Foundations of the root category: analyses of linguistic input to Hebrew-speaking children. In Berman, R. (ed.), Acquisition and development of Hebrew: from infancy to adolescence (pp. 95134) (TILAR (Trends in Language Acquisition Research) 19). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Avidor, A. (1998). Acquisition of derived nominals in Hebrew: developmental and linguistic principles. Journal of Child Language 25, 229–66.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Berman, R. (2009). Developing linguistic register in different text types. Pragmatics & Cognition 17, 108–45.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Chen-Djemal, Y. (2015). Spoken and written narration in Hebrew: a case study. Written Language and Literacy 18, 5681.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Epel Mashraki, Y. (2007). Prosodic reading, reading comprehension and language skills in Hebrew-speaking 4th graders. Journal of Research in Reading 30, 140–56.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Geiger, V. (2009). Promoting morphological awareness in Hebrew-speaking gradeschoolers: an intervention study using linguistic humor. First language 29, 81112.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Levie, R. (2010). Adjectives in the development of text production: lexical, morphological and syntactic analyses. First Language 30, 2755.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Saban, R. (2008). Syntactic and meta-syntactic skills in the school years: a developmental study in Hebrew. In Kupferberg, I. & Stavans, A. (eds), Language education in Israel: papers in honor of Elite Olshtain, 75110. Jerusalem: Magnes Press.Google Scholar
Ravid, D. & Zilberbuch, S. (2003). Morpho-syntactic constructs in the development of spoken and written Hebrew text production. Journal of Child Language 30, 395418.Google Scholar
Schwarzwald, O. (2008). The special status of Nif'al in Hebrew. In Armon-Lotem, S., Danon, G., and Rothstein, S. (eds), Current issues in generative Hebrew linguistics, 6175. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Shibatani, M. (2006). On the conceptual framework for voice phenomena. Linguistics 44, 217–69.Google Scholar
Strauss, S. (2007). Ministry of Education Chief Scientist report on the Cultivation Measure. Jerusalem: Ministry of Education [in Hebrew].Google Scholar
Svenonius, P. (2004). Slavic prefixes inside and outside VP. In Svenonius, P. (ed.), Nordlyd, Tromsø Working Papers on Language and Linguistics 32(2): Special issue on Slavic prefixes, 205253. Tromsø: University of Tromsø.Google Scholar
Timberlake, A. (2007). Aspect, tense, mood. In In Shopen, T. (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description: Vol. II, Grammatical categories and the lexicon, 280333. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Brooks, P. J. & Stern, E. (1998). Learning to produce passive utterances through discourse. First Language 18, 223–37.Google Scholar