Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T19:25:41.671Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cannabis, COMT and psychotic experiences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Stanley Zammit*
Affiliation:
MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University, Cardiff and School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol
Michael J. Owen
Affiliation:
MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University
Jonathan Evans
Affiliation:
School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Jon Heron
Affiliation:
School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
Glyn Lewis
Affiliation:
School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
*
Stanley Zammit, Department of Psychological Medicine & Neurology, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Heath Park, Cardiff CF14 4XN, UK. Email: zammits@cardiff.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

A putative interaction between cannabis and variation at rs4680 within the catechol-methyl-transferase (COMT) gene on psychosis has been reported, but not adequately replicated.

Aims

To examine whether the relative risk of developing psychosis following use of cannabis is dependent upon variation within COMT.

Method

A longitudinal study of 2630 individuals from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort who completed questionnaire-based assessments for cannabis use at age 14 and incident psychotic experiences at age 16. Six SNPs within COMT were genotyped.

Results

There was no evidence of an interaction under multiplicative models between cannabis use and COMT on the risk of developing psychotic experiences in our primary analyses. In sensitivity analyses we observed highly variable evidence of interaction, whereby psychotomimetic effects of cannabis were greater in methionine homozygotes under some scenarios, but in valine homozygotes under others.

Conclusions

Cannabis increases risk of psychosis irrespective of underlying COMT genotypes. These findings argue against the widely held belief that the relative risk of developing psychosis following use of cannabis is dependent upon variation within COMT. The public health message about the potential increase in risk of psychotic disorders following cannabis use should not be tempered by reports that this harm is subgroup specific in the absence of robust evidence of replication.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2011 

Cannabis use has been consistently associated with an increased risk of developing psychosis, Reference Moore, Zammit, Lingford-Hughes, Barnes, Jones and Burke1 but risk factors for multifactorial complex diseases are of course rarely, if ever, necessary or sufficient to cause disease. Although it is very reasonable therefore to expect that the effects of using cannabis on risk of psychosis will vary across individuals in relation to the distribution of other risk factors, both genetic and non-genetic, the implications of findings from studies aimed specifically at examining gene-environment interactions have been challenged. Reference Clayton and McKeigue2-Reference Zammit, Wiles and Lewis4

The catechol-methyl-transferase (COMT) gene on chromosome region 22q11 encodes an enzyme that breaks down catecholamine neurotransmitters. A valine to methionine substitution at the single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs4680 (Val158Met) within COMT results in reduced enzymatic activity and thus slower breakdown of dopamine, with evidence of a dose-response effect according to the number of methionine alleles present. Reference Chen, Lipska, Halim, Ma, Matsumoto and Melhem5,Reference Lachman, Papolos, Saito, Yu, Szumlanski and Weinshilboum6 Although this polymorphism has been extensively studied as a putative risk factor for psychosis, results have been inconsistent. Reference Munafo, Bowes, Clark and Flint7,Reference Okochi, Ikeda, Kishi, Kawashima, Kinoshita and Kitajima8

One study using the Dunedin cohort reported a putative interaction between cannabis and variation at rs4680 such that use of cannabis was associated with a 10-fold increase in risk of developing schizophreniform disorder at age 26 in individuals homozygous for the valine allele, whereas individuals homozygous for methionine appeared to be able to use cannabis without any increase in risk of developing psychosis. Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, McClay, Murray and Harrington9 Most studies have failed to support this finding, Reference Costas, Sanjuan, Ramos-Rios, Paz, Agra and Tolosa10-Reference Zammit, Spurlock, Williams, Norton, Williams and O'Donovan12 or reported interactions but only in subgroups of individuals Reference Henquet, Rosa, Delespaul, Papiol, Fananas and van Os13,Reference Henquet, Rosa, Krabbendam, Papiol, Fananas and Drukker14 or with patterns of interaction that do not appear consistent with those in the Dunedin cohort. Reference Estrada, Fatjo-Vilas, Munoz, Pulido, Minano and Toledo15 There are therefore no studies to date that have replicated the relationship between cannabis and COMT on psychosis as originally described, yet this relationship remains a widely cited example of a gene-environment interaction in the psychiatric literature.

We aimed to examine further the relationship between cannabis, COMT and psychosis. Our measure of psychosis, age at outcome and potential duration of exposure to cannabis differ from to those in the Dunedin cohort as our cohort consists of younger individuals. We examine whether the relationship between cannabis use prior to age 15 and incidence of psychotic experiences at age 16 in a birth cohort study varies according to variation at rs4680 as well as at other loci within COMT.

Method

Sample

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort initially consisted of 14 062 children born to residents of the former Avon Health Authority area who had an expected date of delivery between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 (www.alspac.bris.ac.uk). The children, since 7.5 years of age, have attended annual assessment clinics where they participate in a range of face-to-face interviews, and have also completed postal questionnaires annually since they were age 6. There were 4263 children who participated in the psychosis-like symptoms questionnaires (PLIKS-Q) Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis16 at both 14 and 16 years of age (data restricted to 1 child per nuclear family). There were 530 individuals who responded ‘Yes’ to any psychosis questions at baseline (age 14 questionnaire), and these were omitted from the study. Genotype data (restricted to those of White ethnicity) was available on 2630 of these individuals (defined as the main sample). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and the local research ethics committees.

Measures

Outcomes

The PLIKS-Q at age 16 consists of 11 questions covering hallucinations (visual and auditory); delusions (delusions of being spied on, persecution, thoughts being read, reference, control, grandiose ability); and experiences of thought interference (thought broadcasting, insertion and withdrawal). Individuals were asked whether they had ever experienced these symptoms, and their frequency since their 15th birthday. The level of distress associated with each symptom was also measured. The questionnaire did not allow symptoms of cannabis intoxication to be identified separately from non-intoxication experiences. The 11 psychosis questions were based on the PLIKS semi-structured interview (PLIKSi) that has been used in this cohort at age 12. Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis16 Our primary outcome was a binary measure of a ‘Yes definitely’ response for any of the 11 psychotic experiences assessed.

Exposures

Cannabis

Data on cannabis use were obtained from self-report postal questionnaires completed at age 14. Information was obtained on whether an individual had ever used cannabis, the frequency of recent use and the total cumulative use over time. Our primary measure of exposure was cumulative use of cannabis reported at age 14 (none, <5, 5-20, 21-60, >60 occasions). Collapsing this measure into fewer categories by combining all those who used cannabis on >20 times into one group, or expanding this to separate use on 61-100 times from >100 times, made minimal difference to the results.

COMT

Our primary genetic exposure was variation at rs4680 (Val158Met), as examined in the original study. Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, McClay, Murray and Harrington9 We also examined interactions between cannabis use and a three-marker haplotype within COMT (SNPs rs6269-rs4818-rs4680) that has been shown to reflect COMT functional activity more accurately than variation at rs4680 alone (rs4633 that was part of this original haplotype was omitted as it was in perfect linkage disequilibrium with rs4680, with D′ and r 2 of 1). Reference Nackley, Shabalina, Tchivileva, Satterfield, Korchynskyi and Makarov17

As secondary analyses we also examined interactions between cannabis and three other SNPs typed within COMT (rs737865, rs2097603 and rs165599), selected on the basis that: they had been reported as being associated with psychosis in some studies; and they were not in high linkage disequilibrium (maximum r 2 = 0.32) with any of the other COMT SNPs typed.

Genotyping was performed by KBioscience (http://www.kbioscience.co.uk); SNPs were genotyped using the KASP SNP genotyping system. KASP is a competitive allele-specific polymerase chain reaction incorporating a fluorescent resonance energy transfer quencher cassette (for more information see http://www.kbioscience.co.uk/reagents/KASP.html). Genotyping was successful for over 97% of the sample across the six SNPs, with error rates of approximately 0.5%. None of the SNPs deviated significantly from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium.

Confounders

A number of sociodemographic variables were considered as potential confounders in the interaction between cannabis and COMT: gender, parental social class (highest of both parents, based on occupation using the 1991 Office of Population, Censuses and Surveys classification), 18 parental welfare benefit, parental marital status, housing (own home, privately rented, council housing), urban/rural index at birth (urban/town, village/hamlet), childhood measures of victimisation (Bullying Questionnaire; age 8), Reference Wolke, Woods, Bloomfield and Kerstadt19 depression (Moods & Feeling Questionnaire; age 10), Reference Angold, Costello, Messer, Pickles, Winder and Silver20 emotional and behavioural problems (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; age 10), Reference Goodman21 tobacco, alcohol and other drug use (age 14), and family history of depression, schizophrenia or any mental health illness in biological parents or grandparents.

Sensitivity analyses

We undertook a number of sensitivity analyses to test out the robustness of our interaction results. These analyses examine to what extent variation in our conclusions occur when input parameters for the analyses are altered.

Sample

Data on psychotic experiences were also collected at ages 11 and 13 using questionnaires and at age 12 using a semi-structured interview. For our primary analyses we only used data from participants who responded to questionnaires at both 14 and 16 to maximise the size of our sample while maintaining a longitudinal study design. However, as sensitivity analyses we also examined: (a) the effect of omitting from the sample any individuals who endorsed any psychotic items as being definitely present from questionnaires completed at ages 11 or 13, or those rated as having suspected or definite psychotic experiences during a semi-structured interview at age 12 (restricted sample), and (b) the effect of relaxing our criteria to use the age 16 questionnaire data for our measure of cannabis exposure as well as our outcome, and irrespective of whether or not they had reported the presence of psychotic experiences at previous assessments. This was done to increase the prevalence of exposure to cannabis use, although at the cost of substituting the design from longitudinal to cross-sectional (cross-sectional sample).

Outcome

As sensitivity analyses we also examined (a) a narrower outcome of experiences rated as ‘Yes definitely’ that had either occurred at least once per month, or were rated as ‘quite distressing’ or ‘very distressing’, and (b) an outcome of only auditory or visual hallucinations that were endorsed as ‘Yes definitely’, as questionnaire assessments of these phenomena show higher validity than delusions when compared with interviews. Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis16,Reference Kelleher, Harley, Murtagh and Cannon22

Exposures

As sensitivity analyses we also examined associations using: (a) a binary measure of ever use of cannabis (no, yes) at age 14 (main sample and restricted sample) or age 16 (cross-sectional sample) and (b) average frequency of cannabis use (none, less than weekly, weekly or more frequent) at these ages.

Statistical analysis

Logistic regression was used to calculate odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for associations between both (a) cannabis use and (b) COMT on psychotic experiences at age 16, both before and after adjustment for potential confounders. For all SNPs, we assumed an ‘additive’ genetic model, i.e. that the odds of our outcome increases proportionately with each additional minor allele copy. Haplotypes of rs6269-rs4818-rs4680 were derived using the software PLINK (version 1.06) for Windows. Reference Purcell, Neale, Todd-Brown, Thomas, Ferreira and Bender23 Haplotype G-G-G has been reported as being associated with the greatest protein expression and highest COMT enzyme activity, whereas A-C-G was associated with lowest activity. Reference Nackley, Shabalina, Tchivileva, Satterfield, Korchynskyi and Makarov17 Individuals were coded according to the number of copies of the G-G-G haplotype that they had (high expression haplotype, coded as 0,1,2), and of the A-C-G haplotype (low expression haplotype, coded as 0,1,2). The haplotype probabilities were greater than 0.85 for over 95% of the sample (mean posterior probability 0.98 (s.d. = 0.05).

Statistical interactions on a multiplicative scale between cannabis use and COMT genotypes or haplotypes on risk of psychotic experiences were investigated using likelihood ratio tests within logistic regression models. All analyses were conducted using Stata for Windows (version 11).

This study had >99% power to detect an interaction odds ratio of 3.5 as previously reported for cannabis and rs4680 on risk of schizophreniform disorder, Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, McClay, Murray and Harrington9 and >80% power to detect an interaction odds ratio of 2.0 as reported for self-reported hallucinatory experiences, at α = 0.05. Power calculations were conducted using QUANTO.

Missing data

The non-responders to PLIKS-Q were more likely to come from lower social class families, to have parents with lower education, to be male, and to be of minority ethnic status (all P<0.001). Children not participating in the PLIKS questionnaire at age 16 were more likely to have used cannabis at age 14 (11% compared with 8%, P<0.001) but did not differ by COMT genotypes (P-values 0.119 to 0.733).

Results

Of the 2630 individuals in the main sample, 225 (8.6%) responded ‘Yes, definitely’ to any of the 11 psychosis questions at age 16, with 90 of these rating the frequency as occurring once or more per month or rating their experience as quite or very distressing.

Cannabis and self-reported psychotic experiences

In the main sample, there were 168 individuals (6.5%) who reported having ever used cannabis at age 14. The odds of endorsing definite psychotic experiences at age 16 were 2.5-times greater in those who had used cannabis by age 14 compared with those who had never used cannabis by this age (95% CI 1.62-3.77, P<0.001). There was evidence supportive of a dose-response relationship, with an odds ratio for linear trend across cumulative use categories of 1.60 (95% CI 1.28-1.99, P<0.001).

COMT and self-reported psychotic experiences

The distribution of self-reported psychotic experiences in relation to COMT genotypes is shown in Table 1 (rs4680) and online Table DS1. There was no evidence of association between

Table 1 Number of individuals (%) endorsing definite psychotic experiences in relation to cumulative use of cannabis and COMT rs4680 genotype

No symptoms n Psychotic symptoms, n (%)
Cannabis use
    None 2209 194 (8)
    <5 times 88 16 (15)
    5–20 times 33 9 (21)
    21–60 times 8 1 (11)
    >60 times 6 3 (33)
COMT rs4680
    AA (met/met) 657 58 (8.1)
    GA (val/met) 1158 119 (9.3)
    GG (val/val) 532 47 (8.1)

psychotic experiences and any of the COMT SNPs or haplotypes examined.

Cannabis×COMT

There was no evidence of an interaction under a multiplicative model between COMT rs4680 and cumulative use of cannabis on development of psychotic experiences (Table 2 and online Table DS2). Cumulative use of cannabis at age 14 was associated with an increased odds of psychotic experiences compared with non-use in individuals homozygous for the methionine allele (OR per one-category increase in cumulative use 1.56, 95% CI 1.05-2.31), and this association was very similar in heterozygotes (OR = 1.68, 95% CI 1.23-2.28) as well as valine homozygotes (OR = 1.47, 95% CI 0.85-2.26). Furthermore, there was no evidence that the strength of association between cannabis and psychosis differed according to the number of copies of the G-G-G (high activity) or A-C-G (low activity) rs6269-rs4818-rs4680 haplotypes (interaction P = 0.373 and P = 0.499 respectively), or across the other COMT SNPs examined (interaction P-values 0.304 to 0.574). None of the potential confounders adjusted for made any difference to the interaction results.

COMT and cannabis use

The COMT genotype and cumulative use of cannabis at age 14 were not associated in the full sample with data available on both these measures (P = 0.426), although there was weak evidence that use was greater in methionine allele carriers compared with valine ones in the main sample used for analyses (OR per valine allele 0.86, 95% CI 0.73-1.01, P = 0.063).

Sensitivity analyses

We also examined the main effects of cannabis and COMT rs4680, as well as their interaction in a number of sensitivity analyses (online Table DS3). We utilised different measures of exposure (ever use of cannabis, and average frequency of cannabis use), different measures of our outcome (psychotic experiences that occurred frequently or were distressing, and hallucinatory experience only), and different samples (a sample that omitted any individuals who endorsed definite experiences at any of the assessments prior to that at age 14 (omitted n = 727; restricted sample n = 1844), and a sample that used both exposure and outcome data from the age 16 assessment (cross-sectional sample n = 3425). For all of these 26 sensitivity analyses, main effects remained relatively stable for cumulative cannabis use, ever use of cannabis, frequency of cannabis use and COMT genotypes.

Table 2 Interaction odds ratios (95% CI) between cumulative cannabis use and COMT genotypes on risk of self-reported psychotic experience

Cannabis × COMT interaction
n OR (95% CI) P
rs4680 2567 1.00 (0.73–1.36) 0.981
rs737865 2525 0.87 (0.58–1.29) 0.488
rs2097603 2405 1.10 (0.79–1.51) 0.574
rs6269 2391 0.89 (0.64–1.24) 0.499
rs4818 2417 0.83 (0.60–1.18) 0.304
rs165599 2415 0.85 (0.60–1.21) 0.362
Haplotype (G–G–G) a 2357 0.86 (0.59–1.23) 0.373
Haplotype (A–C–G) a 2357 1.33 (0.58–3.05) 0.499

a rs6269–rs4818–rs4680.

However, interaction effects showed greater variation. For example, the interaction odds ratio between cumulative use of cannabis and rs4680 in the cross-sectional sample indicated that risk of hallucinations from cannabis might be greater in valine homozygotes compared with methionine ones (interaction OR = 1.16, P = 0.039); however, the interaction between cumulative use of cannabis and rs4680 in the restricted sample showed evidence of an interaction in the opposite direction, i.e. that risk of psychotic experiences from cannabis might be greater in methionine homozygotes compared with valine ones (interaction OR = 0.50, P = 0.034).

Discussion

In this study we found evidence of association between cannabis use and subsequent onset of self-reported psychotic experiences, consistent with other studies to date, Reference Moore, Zammit, Lingford-Hughes, Barnes, Jones and Burke1 but no association between any of the COMT SNPs or haplotypes examined with this outcome. There was no evidence of statistical interaction under a multiplicative model between cannabis use and COMT rs4680, or between cannabis use and any of the other COMT SNPs or haplotypes examined on risk of developing psychotic experiences.

We are unable to make direct comparisons between our findings and those from the Dunedin cohort that examined schizophreniform disorder at age 26 as an outcome. Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, McClay, Murray and Harrington9 Nevertheless, our findings do not support a hypothesis that the relationship between cannabis and psychosis is moderated by variation at COMT rs4680, as originally reported. In the Dunedin study, the odds of developing schizophreniform disorder was approximately ten times greater in cannabis users compared with non-users in valine homozygotes, but was no different in methionine homozygotes. Similarly, the odds ratios for reporting hallucinations in relation to cannabis use were 5.3 and 1.2 in valine and methionine homozygotes respectively. In our study however, the association between cumulative cannabis use and onset of psychotic experiences was comparable across valine and methionine homozygotes (OR = 1.47 and OR = 1.56 respectively).

In our sensitivity analyses, main effects for cannabis use and for rs4680 remained relatively consistent, whereas the interaction effect showed much greater variability. Under some scenarios we observed evidence that the effect of cannabis on increasing psychotic experiences was greater in valine homozygotes than methionine ones, whereas under other scenarios we observed evidence to support a relationship in the opposite direction, i.e. that psychotomimetic effects of cannabis were greater in methionine compared with valine homozygotes.

Possible explanations

It is possible that the reason we fail to find an interaction as previously reported is because of the different age group of our sample or the different measures of psychotic outcomes or cannabis use. Outcomes in our study were assessed at age 16 and cannabis use assessed at age 14, whereas in the Dunedin study these were assessed at ages 26 and 15-18 respectively. It is possible that the differential effect of cannabis use on psychosis according to genotype only becomes manifest during early adulthood, or is only present for more chronic use of cannabis.

Questionnaires almost certainly overrate psychotic experiences compared with semi-structured interviews, although they are not so dissimilar to structured interviews. The positive predictive value (PPV) of highly structured stem questions compared with semi-structured interview ratings in the ALSPAC cohort at age 12 was relatively high for auditory hallucinations (PPV = 70%) but much lower for most delusion items. Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis16 A similar finding of reasonably good sensitivity and PPV for hallucinations was also reported in another study, Reference Kelleher, Harley, Murtagh and Cannon22 suggesting that although questionnaire-based assessments overestimate the true prevalence of psychotic experiences, self-reported hallucinations are likely to be a more valid measure of psychotic phenomena. Nevertheless, we found no consistent evidence to support an interaction between cannabis and COMT as originally reported when we examined incident self-reported hallucinations as our outcome in our sensitivity analyses.

Furthermore the Diagnostic Interview Schedule as used to assess psychotic experiences in the Dunedin cohort is a highly structured interview, and the frequency of psychotic experiences reported over a 1-year period was approximately double those endorsed from the questionnaire at age 16 in our study (26% compared with 13% for any psychotic experience; 13% compared with 6% for hallucinations). Variation according to age and location, as well as sample attrition in ALSPAC, might account for this difference, but nevertheless they suggest that overreporting of psychotic experiences within our study may be no more problematic than for studies that have utilised structured interviews.

The interaction reported in the Dunedin cohort was also observed when using informant reports of psychotic experiences, a measure that is also likely to be a less valid marker of psychotic experiences than an interview measure. Given all the above, and the fact that we observed a main effect of cannabis use on psychotic experiences in our study of a magnitude comparable with other studies that have used interview measures of psychosis, it seems rather unlikely that the lack of evidence of interaction between cannabis and COMT is due to misclassification of psychosis data.

We believe that a more likely explanation for our findings is that there is no substantive difference in the effect of cannabis use on psychotic outcomes between valine and methionine homozygotes at rs4680 under a multiplicative model. This is in keeping with other studies that have failed to support an interaction between COMT and cannabis as originally reported. Reference van Winkel11,Reference Zammit, Spurlock, Williams, Norton, Williams and O'Donovan12,Reference Henquet, Rosa, Krabbendam, Papiol, Fananas and Drukker14,Reference Costas, Sanjuan, Ramos-Rios, Paz, Agra and Tolosa24 Furthermore, if an interaction effect size as originally reported were correct, we would also expect to observe a main effect for rs4680 without the need to study interactions given the frequency of cannabis use in the population. However, even in adequately powered meta-analyses the association between rs4680 and schizophrenia remains uncertain, Reference Munafo, Bowes, Clark and Flint7,Reference Okochi, Ikeda, Kishi, Kawashima, Kinoshita and Kitajima8 and a recent large-scale study also failed to provide support in favour of COMT as a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia. Reference Sanders, Duan, Levinson, Shi, He and Hou25

It is also possible that sample attrition may have biased our findings. In common with other large-scale longitudinal studies, missing data due to attrition and wave non-response was not insubstantial. Reference Callaway, McIntyre, O'Callaghan, Williams, Najman and Lawlor26,Reference Plewis, Calderwoof, Hawkes and Nathan27 Differential attrition, whereby those who use drugs and have mental health problems are more likely to drop out, would be expected to lead to underestimation of the association between cannabis and psychosis. Genotype was not associated with missingness and it is unclear what effect attrition may have had on our results for interaction. It seems unlikely though that attrition bias would be an adequate explanation for the large variability in interaction results in the sensitivity analyses.

Potential problems of studying gene-environment interaction

Spurious findings as a result of multiple testing are an important concern in studies of gene-environment interaction, and tests for interaction may also be more susceptible to the effects of subgroup testing and sample selection than those for main effects. For example, although a cannabis×COMT interaction was not observed in the primary analysis within one study, Reference Henquet, Rosa, Krabbendam, Papiol, Fananas and Drukker14 there was evidence of an interaction when including another (three-way) interaction with a previous measure of psychotic phenomena (psychosis vulnerability). Furthermore, the interaction between cannabis and COMT reported in the original Dunedin cohort was observed only in participants who first used cannabis prior to age 18 years, with no evidence of interaction in those who first used after this age. Although it is possible that there are valid reasons why interaction effects are only observed in specific subgroups, it is also possible that such findings are spurious, and should probably be considered as such until robust evidence of replication exists.

It may also be that tests of interaction are more sensitive than main effects to changes in the way exposure or outcome measures are defined, and indeed in our sensitivity analyses the variation in results for main effects of cannabis and COMT did not appear to occur to the same extent as for the interaction between them. This increases concerns regarding the robustness of many putative interactions published to date, and emphasises the importance of strong and consistent replication before such reports are accepted as a true reflection of how exposures combine to alter disease risk.

Although power to reject the null hypothesis of a statistical test is in part determined by sample size, our findings indicate that compared with the study of main effects evidence of interaction may be much more susceptible to the effects of small numbers of events within subgroups, whereby the misclassification of only a small number of individuals may substantially alter findings or even reverse the direction of an interaction. The numbers of events in our study, when stratified by genotype, were very small. In our primary analyses for example, although there were 29 outcomes in those who had used cannabis, for the highest category of use (>60 occasions) there were only three psychosis outcomes in the methionine homozygotes and none in the valine ones. Yet by conducting a number of different analyses we observed evidence of two interactions that would lead us to diametrically opposite conclusions from the same set of data. Interaction results based on relatively few events are not uncommon in the gene-environment literature to date.

Implications

Even if the relative risk of developing psychosis following use of cannabis does actually differ slightly across COMT genotypes, it is very questionable as to what extent such evidence would advance our understanding of aetiological mechanisms or inform potential strategies for prevention or intervention. Reference Zammit, Wiles and Lewis4,Reference Zammit, Owen and Lewis28 An interaction between cannabis and COMT as originally reported, where the effect of one exposure only occurs in the presence of another, could have potentially important implications in this regard. However, an interaction, if present, whereby cannabis increases the relative risk of psychosis in both valine and methionine homozygotes, but slightly more so in one homozygote group than another, would have a much more limited scope for potential benefits. This argument applies equally to potential interactions between cannabis and other polymorphisms that influence dopaminergic or other neurotransmitter pathways.

The public health message from our findings is somewhat different to that from the Dunedin cohort. Results from the Dunedin study found no evidence that risk of psychosis was increased by cannabis use in individuals who were methionine homozygotes. Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, McClay, Murray and Harrington9 It would be incorrect however to interpret this as meaning that methionine homozygotes can use cannabis with impunity in relation to risk of psychosis. Our results indicate that cannabis use is likely to increase the risk of individuals developing psychotic experiences irrespective of underlying COMT genotypes, and argue against the widely held and commonly expressed belief that the relative risk of developing psychosis following use of cannabis is dependent upon variation within COMT.

The public health message about the potential increase in risk of psychotic disorders following cannabis use should not be tempered by reports that this harm is subgroup specific with respect to COMT in the absence of robust evidence of replication.

Funding

This study was funded through a Clinician Scientist Award awarded to S.Z. from the National Assembly for Wales.

Acknowledgements

The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

S.Z. and M.J.O. have received contributions from pharmaceutical companies as honoraria for talks.

References

1 Moore, TH, Zammit, S, Lingford-Hughes, A, Barnes, TR, Jones, PB, Burke, M, et al. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet 2007; 370: 319–28.Google Scholar
2 Clayton, D, McKeigue, PM. Epidemiological methods for studying genes and environmental factors in complex diseases. Lancet 2001; 358: 1356–60.Google Scholar
3 Munafo, MR, Durrant, C, Lewis, G, Flint, J. Gene×environment interactions at the serotonin transporter locus. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 65: 211–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 Zammit, S, Wiles, N, Lewis, G. The study of gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: limited gains at a substantial cost? Psychol Med 2010; 40: 711–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
5 Chen, J, Lipska, BK, Halim, N, Ma, QD, Matsumoto, M, Melhem, S, et al. Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 2004; 75: 807–21.Google Scholar
6 Lachman, HM, Papolos, DF, Saito, T, Yu, YM, Szumlanski, CL, Weinshilboum, RM. Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics 1996; 6: 243–50.Google Scholar
7 Munafo, MR, Bowes, L, Clark, TG, Flint, J. Lack of association of the COMT (Val158/108 Met) gene and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of case-control studies. Mol Psychiatry 2005; 10: 765–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Okochi, T, Ikeda, M, Kishi, T, Kawashima, K, Kinoshita, Y, Kitajima, T, et al. Meta-analysis of association between genetic variants in COMT and schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr Res 2009; 110: 140–8.Google Scholar
9 Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Cannon, M, McClay, J, Murray, R, Harrington, H, et al. Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: longitudinal evidence of a gene×environment interaction. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57: 1117–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10 Costas, J, Sanjuan, J, Ramos-Rios, R, Paz, E, Agra, S, Tolosa, A, et al. Interaction between COMT haplotypes and cannabis in schizophrenia: a case-only study in two samples from Spain. Schizophr Res 2011; 127: 22–7.Google Scholar
11 van Winkel, R. Family-based analysis of genetic variation underlying psychosis-inducing effects of cannabis: sibling analysis and proband follow-up. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 148–57.Google Scholar
12 Zammit, S, Spurlock, G, Williams, H, Norton, N, Williams, N, O'Donovan, MC, et al. Genotype effects of CHRNA7, CNR1 and COMT in schizophrenia: interactions with tobacco and cannabis use. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 191: 402–7.Google Scholar
13 Henquet, C, Rosa, A, Delespaul, P, Papiol, S, Fananas, L, van Os, J, et al. COMT ValMet moderation of cannabis-induced psychosis: a momentary assessment study of ‘switching on’ hallucinations in the flow of daily life. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009; 119: 156–60.Google Scholar
14 Henquet, C, Rosa, A, Krabbendam, L, Papiol, S, Fananas, L, Drukker, M, et al. An experimental study of catechol-o-methyltransferase Val158Met moderation of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol-induced effects on psychosis and cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006; 31: 2748–57.Google Scholar
15 Estrada, G, Fatjo-Vilas, M, Munoz, MJ, Pulido, G, Minano, MJ, Toledo, E, et al. Cannabis use and age at onset of psychosis: further evidence of interaction with COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011; 123: 485–92.Google Scholar
16 Horwood, J, Salvi, G, Thomas, K, Duffy, L, Gunnell, D, Hollis, C, et al. IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 193: 185–91.Google Scholar
17 Nackley, AG, Shabalina, SA, Tchivileva, IE, Satterfield, K, Korchynskyi, O, Makarov, SS, et al. Human catechol-O-methyltransferase haplotypes modulate protein expression by altering mRNA secondary structure. Science 2006; 314: 1930–3.Google Scholar
18 Office of PopulationCensus and Survey. Standard Occupational Classification. HMSO, 1991.Google Scholar
19 Wolke, D, Woods, S, Bloomfield, L, Kerstadt, L. The association between direct and rational bullying and behaviour problems among primary school children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000; 41: 9891002.Google Scholar
20 Angold, A, Costello, EJ, Messer, SC, Pickles, A, Winder, F, Silver, D. The development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. J Method Psychiatr Res 1995; 5: 237–49.Google Scholar
21 Goodman, R. The extended version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire as a guide to child psychiatric caseness and consequent burden. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40: 791–9.Google Scholar
22 Kelleher, I, Harley, M, Murtagh, A, Cannon, M. Are screening instruments valid for psychotic-like experiences? A validation study of screening questions for psychotic-like experiences using in-depth clinical interview. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37: 362–9.Google Scholar
23 Purcell, S, Neale, B, Todd-Brown, K, Thomas, L, Ferreira, MA, Bender, D, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet 2007; 81: 559–75.Google Scholar
24 Costas, J, Sanjuan, J, Ramos-Rios, R, Paz, E, Agra, S, Tolosa, A, et al. Interaction between COMT haplotypes and cannabis in schizophrenia: a case-only study in two samples from Spain. Schizophr Res; 127: 22–7.Google Scholar
25 Sanders, AR, Duan, J, Levinson, DF, Shi, J, He, D, Hou, C, et al. No significant association of 14 candidate genes with schizophrenia in a large European ancestry sample: implications for psychiatric genetics. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 497506.Google Scholar
26 Callaway, LK, McIntyre, HD, O'Callaghan, M, Williams, GM, Najman, JM, Lawlor, DA. The association of hypertensive disorders of pregnancy with weight gain over the subsequent 21 years: findings from a prospective cohort study. Am J Epidemiol 2007; 166: 421–8.Google Scholar
27 Plewis, I, Calderwoof, L, Hawkes, D, Nathan, G. National Child Development Study and 1970 British Cohort Study Technical Report: Changes in the NCDS and BCS70 Populations and Samples over Time. Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Institute of Education, 2004.Google Scholar
28 Zammit, S, Owen, MJ, Lewis, G. Misconceptions about gene-environment interactions in psychiatry. Evid Based Ment Health 2010; 13: 65–8.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Number of individuals (%) endorsing definite psychotic experiences in relation to cumulative use of cannabis and COMT rs4680 genotype

Figure 1

Table 2 Interaction odds ratios (95% CI) between cumulative cannabis use and COMT genotypes on risk of self-reported psychotic experience

Supplementary material: PDF

Zammit et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1-S3

Download Zammit et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 41.9 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.