Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T18:06:31.283Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

35 - Do Apes and Monkeys Know What They (Don’t) Know?

The Question of Metacognition in Primates

from Part VI - Innovation and Problem-Solving

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2021

Allison B. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Josep Call
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Over the past quarter of a century, scientists have attempted to answer the question of whether humans are unique in their self-reflective abilities, or whether versions of this might exist in nonhuman animals as well. This chapter explores the research on whether nonhuman primates (hereafter, primates) have the ability to monitor and control their own knowledge states, or metacognition. The chapter describes the two main paradigms that have traditionally been used to investigate this question, as well as their associated variations and limitations. This is followed by a summary of what has been found to date, with respect to metacognitive abilities across the primate order. The chapter concludes with a brief discussion of the questions that remain and areas for future investigation.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Basile, B. M., Hampton, R. R., Suomi, S. J., & Murray, E. A. (2009) An assessment of memory awareness in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition 12, 169180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basile, B. M., Schroeder, G. R., Brown, E. K., Templer, V. L., & Hampton, R. R. (2015). Evaluation of seven hypotheses for metamemory performance in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(1), 85.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., & Washburn, D. A. (2006). Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) monitor uncertainty during numerosity judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32(2), 111.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., Coutinho, M. V. C., Couchman, J. C., & Boomer, J. (2009). The psychological organization of ‘‘uncertainty’’ responses and ‘‘middle’’ responses: A dissociation in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 371381.Google ScholarPubMed
Beran, M. J. & Smith, J. D. (2011). Information seeking by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Cognition, 120, 90105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., & Perdue, B. M. (2013). Language-trained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) name what they have seen but look first at what they have not seen. Psychological Science, 24, 660666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., & Smith, J. D. (2014). What are my chances? Closing the gap in uncertainty monitoring between rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning & Cognition. doi: 10.1037/xan0000020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., Futch, S. E., Smith, J. D., Evans, T. A., & Parrish, A. E. (2015). Go when you know: Chimpanzees’ confidence movements reflect their responses in a computerized memory task. Cognition, 142, 236246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermúdez, J. L. (2017). Can Nonlinguistic Animals Think about Thinking? In Andrews, K. & Bec, J.,. The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Animal Minds (pp. 119130). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bohn, M., Allritz, M., Call, J., & Völter, C. J. (2017). Information seeking about tool properties in great apes. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 10923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosc, M., Bioulac, B., Langbour, N., Nguyen, T. H., Goillandeau, M., Dehay, B., & Michelet, T. (2017). Checking behavior in rhesus monkeys is related to anxiety and frontal activity. Scientific Reports, 7, 45267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, E. K., Templer, V. L., & Hampton, R. R. (2017). An assessment of domain-general metacognitive responding in rhesus monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 135, 132144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. & McNeill, D. (1966). The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(4), 325337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Browne, D. (2004). Do dolphins know their own minds? Biology and Philosophy, 19, 633653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. (2004). Inferences about the location of food in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus)Journal of Comparative Psychology118(2), 232.Google Scholar
Call, J. (2010). Do apes know that they could be wrong? Animal Cognition, 13, 689700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. (2012). Seeking Information in Non-Human Animals: Weaving a Metacognitive Web. In Beran, M. J., Brandl, J., Perner, J., & Proust, J. (Eds.), Foundations of Metacognition (pp. 6275). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. & Carpenter, M. (2001). Do apes and children know what they have seen? Animal Cognition, 4, 20072220.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2008). Meta-cognition in animals: A skeptical look. Mind Language 23, 5889.Google Scholar
Couchman, J. J., Coutinho, M. V., Beran, M. J., & Smith, J. D. (2010). Beyond stimulus cues and reinforcement signals: A new approach to animal metacognition. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124(4), 356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, H. & Memmott, J. (1982). Counting behavior in animals: A critical evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 547571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. & Perusse, R. (1988). Numerical competence: From backwater to mainstream of comparative psychology. Behavioral Brain Science, 11, 602615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrigno, S., Kornell, N., & Cantlon, J. F. (2017). A metacognitive illusion in monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1862), 20171541.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fujita, K. (2009). Metamemory in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition, 12, 575585.Google Scholar
Hampton, R. R. (2001). Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 98, 53595362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, R. R. (2009). Multiple demonstrations of metacognition in nonhumans: Converging evidence or multiple mechanisms? Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews 4, 1728.Google Scholar
Hampton, R. R. & Hampstead, B. M. (2006). Spontaneous behavior of a rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) during memory tests suggests memory awareness. Behavioural Processes, 72(2), 184189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, R. R., Zivin, A., & Murray, E. A. (2004). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) discriminate between knowing and not knowing and collect information as needed before acting. Animal Cognition 7, 239246.Google Scholar
Hart, J. T. (1965). Memory and the feeling-of-knowing experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56(4), 208.Google Scholar
Kiani, R. & Shadlen, M. N. (2009). Representation of confidence associated with a decision by neurons in the parietal cortex. Science, 324, 759764.Google Scholar
Kornell, N, Son, L. K., & Terrace, H. S. (2007). Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys. Psychological Science, 18, 6471.Google Scholar
Malassis, R., Gheusi, G., & Fagot, J. (2015). Assessment of metacognitive monitoring and control in baboons (Papio papio). Animal Cognition, 18(6), 13471362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. L. (2014). Metacognitive-like information seeking in lion-tailed macaques: A generalized search response after all? Animal Cognition, 17(6), 13131328.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. L. & MacDonald, S. E. (2012a) Information seeking by orangutans: A generalized search strategy? Animal Cognition, 15, 293304.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. L. & MacDonald, S. E. (2012b). Orangutans (Pongo abelii) ‘‘play the odds’’: Information-seeking strategies in relation to cost, risk, and benefit. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 263278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. L., Vining, A. Q., Levendoski, E. K., & Judge, P. G. (2015). Inference by exclusion in lion-macaques (Macaca silenus), a hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas), capuchins (Sapajus apella), and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(3), 256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Middlebrooks, P. G. & Sommer, M. A. (2011). Metacognition in monkeys during an oculomotor task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 37, 325337.Google ScholarPubMed
Morales, J. (2016). When behavior is not enough: Reading metacognition from the brain. Proceedings of the Rotman Institute of Philosophy Annual Conference “Rethinking the taxonomy of psychology workshop,” London, Canada.Google Scholar
Morgan, G., Kornell, N., Kornblum, T., & Terrace, H. S. (2014). Retrospective and prospective metacognitive judgments in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Animal Cognition, 17(2), 249257.Google Scholar
Mulcahy, N. J. (2016). Orangutans (Pongo abelii) seek information about tool functionality in a metacognition tubes task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(4), 391.Google Scholar
Neldner, K., Collier-Baker, E., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens) know when they are ignorant about the location of food. Animal Cognition, 18(3), 683699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paukner, A., Anderson, J. R., & Fujita, K. (2006) Redundant food searches by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): A failure of metacognition? Animal Cognition, 9, 110117.Google Scholar
Perdue, B. M., Evans, T. A., & Beran, M. J. (2018). Chimpanzees show some evidence of selectively acquiring information by using tools, making inferences, and evaluating possible outcomes. PloS One, 13(4), e0193229.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (2012). MiniMeta: In Search of Minimal Criteria for Metacognition. In Beran, M. J., Brandl, J., Perner, J., & Proust, J., (Eds.), Foundations of Metacognition (pp. 94116). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515526.Google Scholar
Redford, J. S. (2010). Evidence of metacognitive control by humans and monkeys in a perceptual categorization task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 36, 248254.Google Scholar
Rosati, A. G. & Santos, L. R. (2016). Spontaneous metacognition in rhesus monkeys. Psychological Science, 27(9), 11811191.Google Scholar
Sayers, K., Evans, T. A., Menzel, E., Smith, J. D., & Beran, M. J. (2015). The misbehaviour of a metacognitive monkey. Behaviour, 152(6), 727756.Google Scholar
Shields, W. E., Smith, J. D., & Washburn, D. A. (1997). Uncertain responses by humans and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in a psychophysical same-different task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 147164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J. D. (2009). The study of animal metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13, 389396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J. D. (2010). Inaugurating the study of animal metacognition. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23(3), 401.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Schull, J., Strote, J., McGee, K., Egnor, R., & Erb, L. (1995). The uncertain response in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncates). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 391408.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Shields, W. E., Schull, J., & Washburn, D. A. (1997). The uncertain response in humans and animals. Cognition, 62, 7597.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Shields, W. E., Allendoerfer, K. R., & Washburn, D. A. (1998). Memory monitoring by animals and humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 227250.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Beran, M. J., Couchman, J. J., & Coutinho, M. V. (2008). The comparative study of metacognition: Sharper paradigms, safer inferences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 679691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., Beran, M. J., & Washburn, D. A. (2010). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) adaptively monitor uncertainty while multi-tasking. Animal cognition, 13(1), 93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, T. R., Smith, J. D., & Beran, M. J. (2018). Not knowing what one knows: A meaningful failure of metacognition in capuchin monkeys. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 5(1), 5567.Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R., Jozefowiez, J., & Cerutti, D. T. (2007). Metacognition: A problem, not a process. PsyCrit, 13(1), 15.Google Scholar
Suda, C. & Call, J. (2006). What does an intermediate success rate mean? An analysis of a Piagetian liquid conservation task in the great apesCognition99(1), 5371.Google Scholar
Suda-King, C. (2008). Do orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) know when they do not remember? Animal Cognition, 11, 2142.Google Scholar
Suda-King, C., Bania, A. E., Stromberg, E. E., & Subiaul, F. (2013). Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory tests. Animal Cognition, 16(1), 6584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takagi, S. & Fujita, K. (2018). Do capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) know the contents of memory traces? A study of metamemory for compound stimuli. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132(1), 88.Google Scholar
Templer, V. L. & Hampton, R. R. (2012). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) show robust evidence for memory awareness across multiple generalization tests. Animal Cognition, 15(3), 409419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tu, H. W., Pani, A. A., & Hampton, R. R. (2015). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) adaptively adjust information seeking in response to information accumulated. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(4), 347.Google Scholar
Vining, A. Q. & Marsh, H. L. (2015). Information seeking in capuchins (Cebus apella): A rudimentary form of metacognition? Animal cognition, 18(3), 667681.Google Scholar
Washburn, D. A., Smith, J. D., & Shields, W. E. (2006). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) immediately generalize the uncertain response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32(2), 185.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×