Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T07:16:54.227Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

23 - Developmental Phonetics of Speech Production

from Section V - Applications of Phonetics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2021

Rachael-Anne Knight
Affiliation:
City, University of London
Jane Setter
Affiliation:
University of Reading
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we provide an overview of past and current research on developmental phonetics. We situate our work within the body of literature on child language phonetics and phonology, which describes speech production patterns, often in the context of other aspects (e.g. phonological, lexical) of the child’s larger linguistic system. Research on phonetic development poses unique challenges, in particular concerning the modelling of the child’s constantly evolving linguistic abilities, which take place in the context of other aspects of the child’s development. We discuss areas of interaction between phonology and other aspects of children’s linguistic behaviours, including how issues affecting speech articulation may influence the emergence of phonological processes, covert contrasts in production, and individual patterns of lexical selection and avoidance. Throughout this discussion, we review methodological approaches to developmental phonetics, from traditional, corpus-based investigations to more recent developments in instrumental studies of child speech, and discuss their contributions to our understanding of phonetic and phonological development in child language.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

23.7 References

Adriaans, F. & Kager, R. (2010). Adding generalization to statistical learning: the induction of phonotactics from continuous speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(3), 311–31.Google Scholar
Barlow, J. A. (1997). A Constraint-Based Account of Syllable Onsets: Evidence from Developing Systems. PhD dissertation, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Baum, S. R. & McNutt, J. C. (1990). An acoustic analysis of frontal misarticulation of /s/ in children. Journal of Phonetics, 18(1), 5163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, M. & Tessier, A.-M. (2011). Trajectories of faithfulness in child-specific phonology. Phonology, 28(2), 163–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhardt, B. H., Bacsfalvi, P., Gick, B., Radanov, B. & Williams, R. (2005). Exploring the use of electropalatography and ultrasound in speech habilitation. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology/Revue d’orthophonie et d’audiologie, 29(4), 169–82.Google Scholar
Bernhardt, B. H. & Stoel-Gammon, C. (1996). Underspecification and markedness in normal and disordered phonological development. In Johnson, C. E. & Gilbert, J. H. V., eds., Children’s Language: Volume 9. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 3354.Google Scholar
Bosma, J. F. (1975). Anatomic and physiologic development of the speech apparatus. In Tower, D. B., ed., The Nervous System, vol. 3. New York: Raven Press, pp. 469–81.Google Scholar
Crelin, E. S. (1987). The Human Vocal Tract: Anatomy, Function, Development, and Evolution. New York: Vantage Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, 285–94.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (1996). The prosodic structure of early words. In Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K., eds., From Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 171–84.Google Scholar
Deville, G. (1891). Notes sur le développement du langage II. Revue de Linguistique et de Philologie Comparée, 24, 1042, 128–43, 242–57, 300–20.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A. (1996). Context effects in the acquisition of fricatives. In Bernhardt, B. H., Gilbert, J. & Ingram, D., eds., Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 136–48.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A. & Gierut, J. A., eds. (2008). Optimality Theory, Phonological Acquisition and Disorders, London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Edwards, J., Fourakis, M., Beckman, M. E. & Fox, R. A. (1999). Characterizing knowledge deficits in phonological disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(1),169–86.Google Scholar
Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K. & Benito-Garcia, C. R. (1984). The acquisition of voicing contrasts in Spanish and English learning infants and children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Language, 11, 313–36.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist, 66(6.2), 103–14.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1978). Fricatives in child language acquisition. In Honsa, V. & Hardman-de-Bautista, M. J., eds., Papers on Linguistics and Child Language, vol. 6. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 93115.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419–39.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P. (1994). On the Acquisition of Prosodic Structure. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.Google Scholar
Fikkert, P. & Levelt, C. (2008). How does place fall into place? The lexicon and emergent constraints in children’s developing grammars. In Avery, P., Dresher, B. E. & Rice, K., eds., Contrast in Phonology: Theory, Perception, Acquisition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 231–68.Google Scholar
Fletcher, P. (1992). Language development from two to three. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 6(4), 329–30.Google Scholar
Fletcher, S. G. (1973). Maturation of the speech mechanism. Folia Phoniatrica, 25, 161–72.Google Scholar
Fletcher, S. G. (1989). Palatometric specification of stop, affricate, and sibilant sounds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 32, 736–48.Google Scholar
Forrest, K., Weismer, G., Hodge, M., Dinnsen, D. A. & Elbert, M. (1990). Statistical analysis of word-initial /k/ and /t/ produced by normal and phonologically disordered children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 4(4), 327–40.Google Scholar
Freitas, M. J. (2003). The acquisition of onset clusters in European Portuguese. Probus, 15, 2746.Google Scholar
Gibbon, F. E. (1999). Undifferentiated lingual gestures in children with articulation/ phonological disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(2), 382.Google Scholar
Gibbon, F. E., Hardcastle, B. & Dent, H. (1995). A study of obstruent sounds in school-age children with speech disorders using electropalatography. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 30(2), 213–25.Google Scholar
Gick, B., Allen, B., Roewer-Després, F. & Stavness, I. (2017). Speaking tongues are actively braced. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(3), 494506.Google Scholar
Gnanadesikan, A. E. (2004). Markedness and faithfulness constraints in child phonology. In Kager, R., Pater, J. & Zonneveld, W., eds., Constraints in Phonological Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 73108.Google Scholar
Goad, H. (1997). Consonant harmony in child language: An optimality-theoretic account. In Hannahs, S. J. & Young-Sholten, M., eds., Focus on Phonological Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 113–42.Google Scholar
Goad, H. (2001). Assimilation phenomena and initial constraint ranking in early grammars. In Do, A. H.-J., Domínguez, L. & Johansen, A., eds., Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 307–18.Google Scholar
Goad, H. & Rose, Y. (2004). Input elaboration, head faithfulness and evidence for representation in the acquisition of left-edge clusters in West Germanic. In Kager, R., Pater, J. & Zonneveld, W., eds., Constraints in Phonological Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 109–57.Google Scholar
Green, J. R., Moore, C. A., Higashikawa, M. & Steeve, R. W. (2000). The physiologic development of speech motor control: Lip and jaw coordination. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 239–55.Google Scholar
Gulian, M. & Levelt, C. (2009). An acoustic analysis of child language productions with reduced clusters. In Chandlee, J., Franchini, M., Lord, S. & Rheiner, M., eds., BUCLD Online Proceedings Supplement. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 112.Google Scholar
Gulian, M. & Levelt, C. (2011). Temporal measures of reduced/sC/-clusters in toddler speech: Evidence for a detailed lexical specification. In Online Proceedings ICPhS XVII Hong Kong. https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2011/OnlineProceedings/RegularSession/Gulian/Gulian.pdf.Google Scholar
Hale, M. & Reiss, C. (1998). Formal and empirical arguments concerning phonological acquisition. Linguistic Inquiry, 29(4), 656–83.Google Scholar
Hall, P. K., Jordan, L. S. & Robin, D. A. (1993). Developmental Apraxia of Speech: Theory and Clinical Practice. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Hardcastle, W. J., Morgan Barry, R. A. & Clark, C. J. (1987). An instrumental phonetic study of lingual activity in articulation-disordered children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 30(2), 171–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, B. & Wilson, C. (2008). A maximum entropy model of phonotactics and phonotactic learning. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(3), 379440.Google Scholar
Hewlett, N. (1988). Acoustic properties of /k/ and /t/ in normal and phonologically disordered speech. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 2(1), 2945.Google Scholar
Howard, S. J. (1998). Phonetic Constraints on Phonological Systems: Combining Perceptual and Instrumental Analysis in the Investigation of Speech Disorders. PhD dissertation, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1974). Phonological rules in young children. Journal of Child Language, 1, 4964.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1989). First Language Acquisition: Method, Description, and Explanation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (2004). The representation of voice and contrasts in children’s early phonological systems. Folia Linguistica, 38(1–2), 145–55.Google Scholar
Inkelas, S. & Rose, Y. (2003). Velar fronting revisited. In Beachley, B., Brown, A. & Conlin, F., eds., Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 334–45.Google Scholar
Inkelas, S. & Rose, Y. (2007). Positional neutralization: A case study from child language. Language, 83(4), 707–36.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Uppsala: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. (1992). The biology of phonological development. In Ferguson, C. A., Menn, L. & Stoel-Gammon, C., eds., Phonological Development: Models, Research, Implications. Timonium, MD: York Press, pp. 6590.Google Scholar
Kewley-Port, D. & Preston, M. S. (1974). Early apical stop production: A voice onset time analysis. Journal of Phonetics, 2(3), 195210.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Ramírez, R. R., Bosseler, A., Lin, J.-F. L. & Imada, T. (2014). Infants’ brain responses to speech suggest analysis by synthesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(31), 11238–45.Google Scholar
Levelt, C. (1994). On the Acquisition of Place. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.Google Scholar
Levelt, C. & van Oostendorp, M. (2007). Feature co-occurrence constraints in L1 acquisition. In Los, B. & van Koppen, M., eds., Linguistics in the Netherlands 2007, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 162–72.Google Scholar
Lewis, B. A., Freebairn, L. A., Hansen, A. J., Iyengar, S. K. & Taylor, H. G. (2004). School-age follow-up of children with childhood apraxia of speech. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35(2), 122–40.Google Scholar
Li, F. (2012). Language-specific developmental differences in speech production: A cross-language acoustic study. Child Development, 83(4), 1303–15.Google Scholar
Li, F., Edwards, J. & Beckman, M. E. (2009). Contrast and covert contrast: The phonetic development of voiceless sibilant fricatives in English and Japanese toddlers. Journal of Phonetics, 37(1), 111–24.Google Scholar
Li, F., Munson, B., Edwards, J., Yoneyama, K. & Hall, K. (2011). Language specificity in the perception of voiceless sibilant fricatives in Japanese and English: Implications for cross-language differences in speech-sound development. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(2), 9991011.Google Scholar
Lin, S. & Demuth, K. (2015). Children’s acquisition of English onset and coda /l/: Articulatory evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(1), 13.Google Scholar
Lin, Y. & Mielke, J. (2008). Discovering place and manner features: What can be learned from acoustic and articulatory data. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 14(1), 19.Google Scholar
Macken, M. A. & Barton, D. (1980). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonants. Journal of Child Language, 7, 4174.Google Scholar
MacKenzie, C. L., Marteniuk, R. G., Dugas, C., Liske, D. & Eickmeier, B. (1987). Three-dimensional movement trajectories in Fitts’ Task: implications for control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 39(4), 629–47.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. & Davis, B. L. (1990). Acquisition of speech production: Frames, then content. In Jeannerod, M., ed., Attention and Performance XIII: Motor Representation and Control. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 453–76.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2020). The TalkBank Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk – Electronic Edition. Part 1: The CHAT Transcription Format, Retrieved from https://talkbank.org/manuals/CHAT.pdf.Google Scholar
Majorano, M., Vihman, M. M. & DePaolis, R. A. (2013). The relationship between infants’ production experience and their processing of speech. Language Learning and Development, 10(2), 179204.Google Scholar
Marshall, C. & Chiat, S. (2003). A foot domain account of prosodically-conditioned substitutions. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 17(8), 645–57.Google Scholar
McAllister Byun, T. (2009). The Articulatory Basis of Positional Asymmetries in Phonological Acquisition (Ph.D. Dissertation), Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
McAllister Byun, T. (2012). Positional velar fronting: An updated articulatory account. Journal of Child Language, 39(5), 1043–76.Google Scholar
McAllister Byun, T. & Tessier, A.-M. (2016). Motor influences on grammar in an emergentist model of phonology. Language and Linguistics Compass, 10(9), 431–52.Google Scholar
McAllister Byun, T., Inkelas, S. & Rose, Y. (2016). The A-map Model: Articulatory reliability in child-specific phonology. Language, 92(1), 141–78.Google Scholar
McCune, L. & Vihman, M. M. (2001). Early phonetic and lexical development: A productivity approach. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(3), 670–84.Google Scholar
McLeod, S. & Crowe, K. (2018). Children’s consonant acquisition in 27 languages: A cross-linguistic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(4), 1546–71.Google Scholar
McLeod, S., van Doorn, J. & Reed, V. A. (1998). Homonyms in children’s productions of consonant clusters. In Ziegler, W. & Deger, K., eds., Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics. London: Whurr Publishers, pp. 99110.Google Scholar
McLeod, S., Harrison, L. J., McAllister, L. & McCormack, J. (2013). Speech sound disorders in a community study of preschool children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22(3), 503.Google Scholar
Morrisette, M. L., Dinnsen, D. A. & Gierut, J. A. (2003). Markedness and context effects in the acquisition of place features. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics / La Revue Canadienne de Linguistique, 48(3/4), 329–55.Google Scholar
Munson, B., Edwards, J., Schellinger, S., Beckman, M. E. & Meyer, M. K. (2010). Deconstructing phonetic transcription: Language-specificity, covert contrast, perceptual bias, and an extraterrestrial view of vox humana. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 24, 245–60.Google Scholar
Munson, B., Edwards, J. & Beckman, M. E. (2011). Phonological representations in language acquisition: climbing the ladder of abstraction. In Cohn, A. C., Fougeron, C. & Huffman, M. K., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Laboratory Phonology. Oxford University Press, pp. 288309.Google Scholar
Nam, H., Goldstein, L. M., Giulivi, S., Levitt, A. G. & Whalen, D. H. (2013). Computational simulation of CV combination preferences in babbling. Journal of Phonetics, 41(2), 6377.Google Scholar
Pater, J. (1997). Minimal violation and phonological development. Language Acquisition, 6(3), 201–53.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2003). Phonetic diversity, statistical learning, and acquisition of phonology. Language and Speech, 46(2–3), 115–54.Google Scholar
Priestly, T. M. S. (1977). One idiosyncratic strategy in the acquisition of phonology. Journal of Child Language, 4, 4565.Google Scholar
Pye, C., Ingram, D. & List, H. (1987). A comparison of initial consonant acquisition in English and Quiché. In Nelson, K. E. & van Kleeckl, A., eds., Children’s Language, vol. 6. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 175–90.Google Scholar
Richtsmeier, P. T. (2010). Child phoneme errors are not substitutions. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics, 33(1).Google Scholar
Rose, Y. (2000). Headedness and Prosodic Licensing in the L1 Acquisition of Phonology. PhD dissertation, McGill University, Quebec, Canada.Google Scholar
Rose, Y. (2003). Place specification and segmental distribution in the acquisition of word-final consonant syllabification. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics / La Revue Canadienne de Linguistique, 48(3/4), 409–35.Google Scholar
Rose, Y. (2014). The emergence of first language phonology: Perception, articulation and representation. In Costa, J., Fiéis, A., Freitas, M. J., Lobo, M. & Santos, A. L., eds., New Directions in the Acquisition of Romance Languages: Selected Proceedings of the Romance Turn V. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing, pp. 3561.Google Scholar
Rose, Y. (2017). Child phonology. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. http://oxfordre.com/linguistics/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.001.0001/acrefore-9780199384655-e-150.Google Scholar
Rose, Y. & Inkelas, S. (2011). The interpretation of phonological patterns in first language acquisition. In Ewen, C. J., Hume, E., van Oostendorp, M. & Rice, K., eds., The Blackwell Companion to Phonology. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 2414–38.Google Scholar
Rose, Y. & MacWhinney, B. (2014). The PhonBank Project: Data and software-assisted methods for the study of phonology and phonological development. In Durand, J., Gut, U. & Kristoffersen, G., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Corpus Phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 380401.Google Scholar
Scobbie, J. M., Gibbon, F. E., Hardcastle, W. J. & Fletcher, P. (1996). Covert contrast as a stage in the acquisition of phonetics and phonology. In Broe, M. B. & Pierrehumbert, J. B., eds., Papers in Laboratory Phonology V: Acquisition and the Lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 4362.Google Scholar
Smith, B. L. (1979). A phonetic analysis of consonantal devoicing in children’s speech. Journal of Child Language, 6(1), 1928.Google Scholar
Smith, N. V. (1973). The Acquisition of Phonology: A Case Study, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Spencer, A. (1986). Towards a theory of phonological development. Lingua, 68, 338.Google Scholar
Stoehr, A., Benders, T., Hell, J. G. V. & Fikkert, P. (2018). Heritage language exposure impacts voice onset time of Dutch–German simultaneous bilingual preschoolers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(3), 598617.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (1996). On the acquisition of velars in English. In Bernhardt, B. H., Gilbert, J. & Ingram, D., eds., Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 201–14.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 134.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. & Cooper, J. A. (1984). Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language, 11, 247–71.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. & Stemberger, J. P. (1994). Consonant harmony and underspecification in child phonology. In Yavas, M., ed., First and Second Language Phonology. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, pp. 6380.Google Scholar
Stone, M. (2005). A guide to analysing tongue motion from ultrasound images. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 19(6–7), 455501.Google Scholar
Stone, M., Faber, A., Raphael, L. J. & Shawker, T. H. (1992). Cross-sectional tongue shape and linguopalatal contact patterns in [s], [ʃ], and [l]. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 253–70.Google Scholar
Velleman, S. L. (1996). Metathesis highlights feature-by-position constraints. In Bernhardt, B. H., Gilbert, J. & Ingram, D., eds., Proceedings of the UBC International Conference on Phonological Acquisition. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press, pp. 173–86.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. (2014). Phonological Development: The First Two Years, 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M. & Croft, W. (2007). Phonological development: toward a ‘radical’ templatic phonology. Linguistics, 45(4), 683725.Google Scholar
Vorperian, H. K., Kent, R. D., Gentry, L. R. & Yandell, B. S. (1999). Magnetic resonance imaging procedures to study the concurrent anatomic development of vocal tract structures: Preliminary results. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 49(3), 197206.Google Scholar
Vorperian, H. K., Kent, R. D., Lindstrom, M. J., Kalina, C. M., Gentry, L. R. & Yandell, B. S. (2005). Development of vocal tract during early childhood: A magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 117(1), 338–50.Google Scholar
Warner, N., Jongman, A., Sereno, J., Kemps, R. (2004). Incomplete neutralization and other sub-phonemic durational differences in production and perception: Evidence from Dutch. Journal of Phonetics, 32(2), 251–76.Google Scholar
Weismer, G., Dinnsen, D. & Elbert, M. (1981). A study of the voicing distinction associated with omitted, word-final stops. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46(3), 320–8.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1(2), 197234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, A. C. L. (2007). Understanding near mergers: The case of morphological tone in Cantonese. Phonology, 24(01), 187–14.Google Scholar
Zharkova, N., Hewlett, N. & Hardcastle, W. J. (2011). Coarticulation as an indicator of speech motor control development in children: an ultrasound study. Motor Control, 15(1), 118–40.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×